Aggression: Sibling and Peer Relationships Pro-social and anti-social influences through adolescence Evolution of responses to (un)fairness Sarah Brosnan & Frans de Waal https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=meiU6Txys Cg Will M. Problem of cooperation Cooperation yields higher benefits than could be attained alone, but reaping the benefits without paying the cost would be even better Examples - Tragedy of the commons - Bystander effect - Stinginess Will M. Ultimatum Game Why not accept any positive offer, no matter how low? Puzzling when the game is only played once. Will M. Problem of cooperation Yet we know that humans cooperate with nonkin, as do primates (a bit) How can cooperative behavior among nonkin evolve if it is more beneficial to cheat? Inequity aversion may stabilize cooperation (it should be called inequality aversion in the context of the Ultimatum Game) Will M. Why aversion to inequity? First-order IA: take stance against personal exploitation Second-order IA: attract cooperative partners These are present in humans and some apes. More developed in humans because of language, enhanced self-control, etc. Amount of partner choice influences adaptive value of IA. (Also true in humans: collectivist societies will tolerate more inequality) Punishment is more effective when there is less partner choice. Will M. Genuine second-order IA? The evidence is strong for kin and allies, but not for strangers Chimps don’t reduce advantageous inequality even at no cost to self In humans, there is aversion to inequality with strangers when reputation is at stake, but not otherwise. Will M. Relational model. aggressive behavior is one of several ways in which conflicts of interest can be settled. Other possible ways are tolerance (e.g., sharing of resources), or avoidance of confrontation (e.g., by subordinates to dominants). Kiss to make-up Fig. 2. Chimpanzees typically seal a postconflict reunion, or reconciliation, with a mouth-to-mouth kiss, as here by a female (right) to the dominant male. Aggression De Waal, F. B. M. (2000). Primates--a natural heritage of conflict resolution. Science, 289(5479), 586-590. Most primates show a dramatic increase in body contact between former opponents during post conflict (PC) as compared with matched-control (MC) observations The cumulative percentage of opponentpairs seeking friendly contact during a 10-min time window after 670 spontaneous aggressive incidents in a zoo group of stumptail macaques Reconciliations allow rhesus monkeys to maintain tight kinship bonds despite frequent intrafamilial squabbles. Shortly after two adult sisters bit each other, they reunite sitting on the left and right of their mother, the alpha female of the troop, each female holding her own infant. The sisters smack their lips while the matriarch loudly grunts. Reconciliation The nature of the social relationship determines whether repair attempts will be made, or not. If there is a strong mutual interest in maintenance of the relationship, reconciliation is most likely. Parties negotiate the terms of their relationship by going through cycles of conflict and reconciliation. Weaning A weaning compromise has been arrived at between a mother chimpanzee and her 4year-old son. After repeated nursing conflicts, the son is permitted to suck on a part of the mother's body other than the nipple. Human aggression Types Reactive and proactive aggression Overt and covert anti-social behavior Processes Under-socialized aggressive conduct disorder associated with weak inhibition system (BIS) – Impulsivity a key (Quay) Information Processing – Real-time processes Somebody bumps into you at a party Stability of aggression The earlier a person start, the more intense the form of aggression and the longer it lasts Stability of aggression can be as high as .76 – – Remarkably stable over up to 10 years The aggressive remain so One of the more stable psychological characteristics Behavior genetics One inherits a propensity toward antisociality which interacts with an environment in its (non)emergence – – Genetic effects greater for self-reported than adjudicated measures of aggression Environmental, genetic, and interactive effects evident in petty crime (806) Real-time coercion A parent–child dyad with two main interaction patterns: A cooperative, mutually positive pattern and a hostile–withdrawn pattern in which the parent berates the child and the child ignores the parent. As mutual positivity declines in early adolescence, existing habits of withdrawal will constrain the interactions that emerge next. (Granic & Patterson, 2006). Granic & Patterson, 2006 Relational aggression Relational aggression “Attempts to harm the victim through the manipulation of relationships, threat of damage to them, or both” (Crick et al, ’02 p.98) Associated with internalizing/externalizing problems and later peer rejection Preschool • Direct, face-to face behaviors • Exclusion from party Middle Childhood • More sophisticated, direct and indirect behaviors • Direct: refuse to choose as a team member • Indirect: spread a rumor Adolescence • Sophisticated and focused on opposite-sex relationships • Stealing a boyfriend Provocation aggression Physically aggressive children exhibited hostile attributional biases and reported relatively greater distress for instrumental provocation situations – Getting pushed into the mud Relationally aggressive children exhibited hostile attributional biases and reported relatively greater distress for relational provocation contexts – Not getting invited to a birthday party. – 662 third- to sixth-grade children Crick et al., 2002. CD. Aggression type and gender Boys more physically victimized by their friends. Girls more relationally victimized. Friend physical victimization was particularly related to boys adjustment difficulties Friend relational victimization particularly related to girls’ adjustment difficulties. Is relational aggression a cause for concern or part of everyday life? Are some people more impacted by relational aggression? • Crick & Nelson, 2002. Disentangle forms and functions of aggression in early childhood Forms – physical, relational Functions – A Longitudinal Study of Forms and Functions of Aggressive Behavior in Early Childhood Murray-Close & Ostrov, 2009 proactive, reactive Potential predictors of aggression subtypes – Gender, age, social dominance, exclusion Gangi Measures Observer Ratings Observations of Aggression Ratings of Aggression – Preschool Social Behavior Scale- Observer Form Ratings of Form and Function of Aggression – Preschool Proactive and Reactive AggressionObserver Report Teacher report Report of Exclusion – Child Behavior Scale Report of Social Dominance and Resource Control Latent aggression factors Forms stable, functions unstable, over time Proactive increase in physical Prediction Findings Support for distinction between aggression forms and functions by early childhood Forms were stable over time, functions were not – May use distinct forms to meet variety of needs Predictors of aggression: – – – – Females more relationally aggressive Older children less likely to use physical aggression Dominance associated with concurrent relational aggression, decreases in physical aggression over time Exclusion related to increases in relational aggression over time Gangi Conclusions Results from both studies support the pathway from PV in elementary school to DPA in middle school – Early externalizing behavior likely sets this process in motion and eventually leads to DPA PV has negative long-term social consequences Early behavior problems are a risk factor for experiencing PV and eventually associating with deviant peer groups – Role of internalizing symptoms is less clear Peer victimization (PV)—a stressor for youth However, little is known about PV’s longterm impact on social relationships – Specifically, authors were interested in predictors of deviant peer affiliation (DPA; e.g., antisocial behaviors, getting into fights, stealing, cheating) Rudolph, K. D., Lansford, J. E., Agoston, A. M., Sugimura, N., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (2014). Peer Victimization and Social Alienation: Predicting Deviant Peer Affiliation in Middle School. Child Development, 85(1), 124-139. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12112 Peer victimization in elementary school deviant peer affiliation (DPA) in middle school Early externalizing behavior likely sets this process in motion and eventually leads to DPA PV has negative long-term social consequences Early behavior problems are a risk factor for experiencing PV and eventually associating with deviant peer groups – Role of internalizing symptoms is less clear