I I : ! MOTOR SKILLS LEARNING AND THE SPECIFICITY OF TRAINING PRINCIPLE Mury L. Barnett, Diane Ross, Richard A. Schmidt, and Bette Todd The specificity of training principle from exercise physiology was tested in a motor skills learning context. Two criterion conditions (either fatigued or nonfatigued) for performing a movement-time task were defined on day 2 and subjects practiced 't'or this criterion under either latigued or nonfatigued conditions on day 1. Regardless ol whether the criterion day 2 performance was under fatigued or nonfatigued conditions, practice under nonfatigued conditions produced more effective learning than did practice under fatigued conditions, although the efiect was considerablv larger for the nonlatigued criterion condition. This evidence was contrary to the specificity of training principle lor motor skills situations. Tnr spscIFIcITy of training principle, borrowed from the field of exercise physiology, has at least potential relevance for the field of motor learning. For physical exercise and training, it states that if an individual wants to maximize criterion endurance performance in running or on the bicycle ergometer, then he should train for this event at the same workload as the criterion performance. The implication is that the systems that support the exercise are developed in different ways for different intensities of training and that training is maximally eftective for a criterion performance of the same intensity. Presumably, training at a different intensity than that for the criterion task results in changes that are appropriate for that particular workload, but that are not appropriate for the specified criterion performance. It is reasonable to ask whether the specificity of training principle could be relevant for motor skills learning situations. Since many skills in sports MARY L. BARNETT teaches physical education at Marion High School, Birmingham, Michigan. DIANE ROSS is assistant professor of physical education at California State College, Fullerton, California. RICHARD A. SCHI,IIDT is a professor of physical education at The University of Michigan. Ann Arbor, Michigan. BETTE TODD teaches physical education at Whitmer High School, Toledo, Ohio. 440 Barnett, Ross, Schmidt and Todd 441 and industrial settings are performed under rather distinct environmental conditions (e.g., noise, heat, spectators) and internal conditions (e.g., fatigue, loss of sleep), would it not be most beneficial for subjects to learn these motor skills under the same environmcntal and internal conditions as are present in the criterion performance? A number of lines of evidence suggest that such might be the case. For example, Henry's (3) specificity hypothesis sees motor skills as being quite specific, bearing only superficial resemblance to each other. There is the implication in his work that changing the motor task only slightly produces a "new" motor task for which a new motor program must be iearned. The evidence for this conclusion is based upon the low correlations typically found among very similar-appearing motor skills, and the implications about the changing motor program are not strong. Additionally, however, it is known ( I ) that fatigue produces changes in the recruitment pattern and intensity of motor units within a muscle. It is not unreasonable to suggest that a motor skill performed under rested conditions would require a different pattern and intensity of innervation (and hence, a different motor program) than would the same task performed under fatigued conditions. If such is the case, then practicing the motor task under fatigue conditions different from those in the criterion situation would result in acquisition of a somewhat inappropriate motor program for the criterion task, and learning would be more efficient if the practice conditions simulated exactly the criterion conditions. A rival point of view to the principle just stated would be that, regardless of the conditions under which the criterion task is to be performed, practice should be under those conditions that produce the highest level of performance during practice trials; this idea might be termed the "optimal conditions" view. There has been little formal activity to test these two opposing points of view within the realm of motor skills. An exception is a study by Singer (4) providing some support for the specificity of training principle using two spectator conditions with error scores on a mirror-tracing task, but speed scores did not support this conclusion. Other than this mixed support, however, there was no evidence found favoring either point of view. The present experiment, therefore, was concerned with difterentiating among them. The "criterion" task was performed either under rested conditions or under fatigued conditions, and subjects learned this task under either rested or fatigued conditions. Support for the specificity of training view would be provided if practicing under the same conditions as the criterion task provided more efficient learning than practicing under the opposite condition, for both criterion conditions. Support for the rival "optimal conditions" view would be provided if practice under the rested condition always provided more efficient learning of the criterion task than did practice under the fatigued condition. 442 The Research Quarterly, Vol.44, No.4 Method Subiects. The subjects were 104 right-handed, female undergraduate and graduate students from The University of Michigan. They volunteered to take part and were not paid for their services. Motor learning task. The learnin_q task consisted of a large wooden box (20 X 12)X 4 in.) mounted on a standard 28-in. table. In one corner of the box. a vertical axle was mounted in bushings and supported a horizontal 11-in. aluminum arm with a 5-in. hi_eh vertical wooden handle at the end. The arm rotated freely in the horizontal plane through ncarlv 360o, but a sturdv metal stop prevented continuous circular rotation. At the upper right-hand corner of the box, a hinged masonite (4 Y.4 in.) barrier was attached, and a slight tap to the right was sufficient to make the barrier fall. When the handle and arm were aeainst thc stop so that only clockwise movement was possible. a microswitch inside the box was "open," and moving l/4 in. clockwise "closed" it causing a 0.01-scc. timer (Standard. Type S-1) to run. When the hinged barrier was knocked over, contact points were "opened" and the timer stopped. The task was begun with the seateC subject graspine the handle with the right hand, and the aluminum arm against the stop. The location of the stop was such that the starting position was at "6 o'clock" with respect to the subject, and the barrier approximately in the direction of "2 o'clock." The subject moved the handle clockwise nearly one full revolution until it hit the stop from the right, then reversed direction and rotated the handle counterclockwise until the stop was struck from the left, then released the handle and moved the hand rightward to knock over the barrier. The score was the time (in .01 sec.) between the initial movement of the handle from the stop until the barrier was struck, the movement being performed as rapidly as possible. The task has been termed the Sigma task because of the o-like movement required (2). The fatigue task. The apparatus to induce muscular fatigue was an arm ergometer with an 1l-in. arm and vertical handle mounted on a vertical axle. so that the arm and handle rotated horizontally. The axle drove a 12-in. v-pulley with a nylon cord in the groove (under adjustable spring tension) to provide frictional resistance to the hand movement. The ergometer was mounted to the right of the Sigma task, with their respective handles being the same length and height from the floor. Design. The basic design involved 2 days of practice. On day 1, all subjects practiced 20 trials of the Sigma task under one of two conditions. The fatigued condition (F) involved 2 min. of ann ergometer cranking before the first trial, with 14 sec. cranking during each of the subsequent intertrial intervals. The rate was 60 rpm with a workload of 383 kgm/min. A nonfatigued condition (NF) used the same intertrial interval as with condition F, but subjects tapped a large "x" on the table top for 2 min. (1 tap/sec. to a metronome) before trial 1 and for 14 sec. during each Barnett, Ross, Schmidt and Todd 443 intertrial interval. The tapping was designed to minimize the difference between conditions in terms of the attention demand during the intertrial intervals. Subjects were assigned at random to one of two conditions on day l, with the restriction that each condition have 52 subjects. On day 2 (1 week +3 hr. after day 1) the subject returned for 10 additional trials on the Sigma task. Each of the two day 1 groups was subdivided randomly into two groups of 26 subjects each, and these groups performed either under the same or opposite conditions as on day 1. This formed a 2 X 2 experimental design, with dimensions being day 1 conditions (either F or NF) and day 2 conditions (either F or NF), with 26 independent subjects per cell. Procedures. On day 1, the Sigma task was explained and the subject was allowed to perform one very slow response to determine if instructions had been understood. Then those subjects under condition F were given arm ergometer instructions (including one slow revolution) while those under condition NF were told how to tap on the "x." Actual testing began with 2 min. of either cranking or tapping, and this was ended by the command "Stop." This was the signal to move to the Sigma task, to prepare to grasp the handle in the starting position and, upon the command "Start," to begin the Sigma task trial. Subjects did not use "Start" as a stimulus as in reaction time situations, as they were free to wait a moment or so before they desired. After completing the movement, the subject responding moved quickly to the secondary apparatus and immediately began either tapping or cranking. The next "Stop" was presented 15 sec. after the command "Start," with cranking or tapping time being approximately 14 sec. Knowledge of results in.01 sec. (e.g., 1.23 sec.) was provided while the subject was tapping or cranking. This procedure resulted in 20-sec. intertrial intervals, including 14 sec. cranking or tapping, and 6 sec. for moving to the appropriate apparatus. Day 2 procedures were identical to those on day 1 for a given condition, except that no slow Sigma task trial was provided. Instructions were given for the new interpolated task for those subjects who changed conditions from day 1 to day 2, and a slow tapping or cranking trial was presented. if Results Day 1 perfornidnce. The mean movement times for the two day 1 practice conditions are presented as a function of trials in Figure 1. Subjects in both conditions improved considerably with practice, and the trials efiect in the day 1 conditions X trials ANOVA w:$ significant with F(19,1938) - 183.0, P <.01. Condition F appeared to lengthen the movement time approximately .15 sec. relative to condition NF over all day 1 trials, and thiseffect was significantwith F(1,102) - 10.2, P <.01. The groups were remarkably "parallel" except for the slightly greater improvement for condition NF over trials 1-6, but this difference was The Research Quarterly, Vol.44, No. 4 444 2.2 :o 2.t zo o IJ F = F zu.r 1.9 t.8 l'7 1.6 =, Io r.5 2 e t.4 r.3 lrJ = 1.2 to TRIALS I practice apparently sufficiently large to produce a significant day 1 condition X trials Figure l. Mean movement times over trials as a lunction ol the day conditions. interaction, wirh F(1,9,1938) - 3.3, P <.01. Thus, the day 1 analysis indicated that subjects in both conditions improved considerably during day 1, but that the ergometer cranking depressed performance considerably on all practice trials. Day 2 performance. The mean movement times for all four groups created on day 2 are shown in Figure 2. All groups appeared to demonstrate continued improvement throughout day 2 trials, and the trials eftect was significant in a day 1 y day 2 X trials ANOVA, with F(9,900) 52.37, P < .01. Again, as on day 1, the presence or absence of fatigue on day 2 appeared to have a large effect on performance, with condition F producing approximately.12 sec. greater movement times than condition NF, and this day 2 conditions efiect was significant with F(1,000) - 9.21, P <.01. Therefore, the presence of fatigUe had nearly the same depressing effect on day 2 as it did on day 1. However, the variable of chief interest was the day I conditions effect, that is, the eftect of the type of practice on day 1 on the level of day 2 performance. Figure 2 shows that. when the criterion task was performed under condition NF, day 1 practice under the opposite conditions (i.e., condition F) produced longer movement times than when day 1 practice was NF. This effect has been found before (2), and it indicates that it is Barnett, Ross, Schmidt and Todd 44s F-F F- NF NF-F d (l) 3 NF- NF t.4 trJ F F A l-3 t.z 'e--+--o-- *-{ -+--{--'--G-__G__O._ -! lrJ A r.r = e r-o IJ z 3 5 7 I I 9 I o TRIA LS Figure 2. Mean day 2 movement times over trials as a ioint function and day 2 practice conditions. ol the day I more beneficial (for a nonfatigued criterion task) to practice the task under condition NF. The findings appeared to be somewhat difterent in the case where the criterion task was to be performed under condition F. In this case, there appeared to be little effect of the type of day 1 practice on the performance under condition F on day 2, and even a slight tendency for day 1 condition NF to result in superior day 2 performance relative to day 1 condition F, indicating that it might be preferable to practice the fatigued criterion task under nonfatigued conditions. If the specificity of training view is correct, there should be an interaction between day I and day 2 dimensions, but this interaction appeared not to be present and was not significant, with F(1,100) - 0.81, P >.05. However, if the "optimal conditions" view is correct, we should see an eftect of day I practice conditions on day 2 performance. There was a tendency for the condition NF on day 1 to produce superior performance on day 2 for both day 2 conditions F and NF, but this effect just failed significance, with F(1,100) 2.73, P .10. These data provided rather strong evidence against the - of training view, and some support for the "optimal conditions" specificity view for skills learning. The Research Quarterly, VoI. 44, No. 4 Discussion The specificity of training principle, borrowed from the literature dealing with cardiovascular and strength training. seemed a likely candidate for application in motor skills contexts because of the rather strong support for it in the exercise areas. However. the present data offered no support for it. If the view were correct for skills. the day 2 data should have shown that those treatment groups that had the same conditions on day 1 and day 2, regardless of the day 2 conditions, would out perform those groups that had a shift in conditions from day 1 to day 2. Such a trend was found for condition NF on day 2, in which practicing on day I under condition F produced less learning of the criterion NF task than did practicing under condition NF. This finding supported others (e.g., 2) in the literature dealing with physical fatipue and learning. showing that it is more efficient to learn a task to be performed under NF conditions by practicing under NF rather than F conditions. However, this trend was not present for the situation in which the criterion task was to be performed under condition F. In this situation, it seemed to make little difterence under which conditions the task was learned on day 1, with the subjects who practiced under condition NF on day t having a slight day 2 advantage over those subjects with F practice on day 1, which was contrary to the predictions of the specificity of training principle. The more attractive possibility in the light of the present data is the "optimal conditions" view which states that regardless of the conditions under which the criterion task is to be performed, it is more efficient to practice the task under those conditions that produce the optimal performance in practice. The predictions from this hypothesis are that practicing under NF conditions on day 1 should lead to better performance than practicing under day 1 condition F, regardless of the day 2 conditions. This pattern of day 2 findings was found in the present data, but with a much larger effect when the criterion task was performed under condition NF than when under condition F. Statistically. the prediction of this optimal conditions hypothesis is a significant main effect of day 1 practice condition on the day 2 performance, and this effect was nearly statistically significant (P .10) in the present data. Therefore, with some desree of caution because of the somewhat elevated alpha level for the statistical test, there is support for the "optimal conditions" view. While there was little support for the specificity of training principle in the present data. there may be other siruations in which this view has merit. Consider a motor task where the specific stimuli to which specific responses are learned change markedly as the environmental conditions change. For example, if a task involves responses to rather weak auditory stimuli, addine a noisy environment could have the effect of masking the auditory stimuli so that other cues (e.g., visual) become dominant. In such a case, it would seem more efficient to practice the task under those conditions that most resemble the conditions for criterion performance. The chief Barnett, Ross, Schmidt ancl I oclct ++t difference between this hypothetical situation and the one in the present data seems to be that the environmental conditions cause a change in the cues used for proper performance, a condition apparently not present with the manipulation of fatigue. While it is still possible that the specificity of training principle might be supported in subsequent work with other experimental performance variables, this does not deny the fact that for fatigue as a perforrnance variable there was no support for the principle and some support for an "optimal conditions" alternative. References t. BrsuuteN, J. V. Muscles alive-their functions revealed by electromyography. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1962. ,, GoDwrN, M. A., and ScHptror, R. A. "Muscular fatigue and discrete motor learning." Research Quarterly 42:37 4-82, 197 l. J. HENRv, F. M. "Specificity vs. generality in learning motor skills." Proceedings of the College Physical Education Association, Washington, D.C., 1958. 4. SrNcrR, R. N. "Effect of an audience on performance of a motor task." Iournal ol Motor Behavior,2:88-95, 1970.