Improving Students' Spatial Thinking Skills

advertisement
Improving Students’
Spatial Thinking Skills
Carol Ormand, SERC, Carleton College
Based on research with
Cathy Manduca, SERC, Carleton College
Thomas Shipley, Temple University
Basil Tikoff, UW-Madison
What Research Tells Us
• Spatial skills cannot be measured and characterized with a
single test; a suite of tests is required
• Spatial thinking skills improve with practice
• There is a wide range of spatial thinking abilities, even
among geoscience majors in upper-level courses
• Although students can succeed in undergraduate geoscience
courses with weak spatial thinking skills, incorporating
spatial skills training is likely to have two benefits:
• Increasing the pool of potential geoscientists
• Improving gender equity in the geoscience workforce
What is Spatial Thinking?
• What are some of the tasks that you have your students do
that have a spatial component?
• What are some of the tasks that you do as a geoscientist
that have a spatial component?
• How can we help our
students progress from
where they are to where
you are?
Examples of Spatial Thinking
Student tasks:
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
•
Interpret maps and aerial photos
Create physical geographic profiles
Interpret and correlate well logs
Draw a diagram of isotopic
composition of water in different
locations within a system
Making maps in GIS; 3D mapping;
drawing subsurface hydrology
Calculate the variation of properties in
space, statistically
Make a beach profile, interpret
sediment samples, compare to prior
years’ data
Develop computer models to solve
characteristic equations
Model 3D flow
Describe coastal upwelling (narrative)
Expert tasks:
• Reconstruct spatial cross-section from
time series from buoy and Gulf Stream
• Observe, interpret, and model what we see
in outcrops (sedimentary rocks)
• Analyze nutrients in water to interpret
inputs
• Contour maps – recognize errors in
computer-generated contouring
• Groundwater – surface water interactions
through time
• River scale flow modeling
• Using isotopes to reconstruct paleoclimate
• Reconstruct the subsurface from seismic
data
• Develop a GIS curriculum, including
teaching basic geographic concepts
• Organizing classroom to maximize
student interactions
Spatial thinking includes
(but is not limited to)…
• Mental rotation: imagining rotating an object
• In cognitive science research, mental rotation skills have served
as a proxy for all spatial thinking until quite recently. Data from
many different sources (including our studies) suggest this is
insufficient.
Spatial thinking includes
(but is not limited to)…
• Visualizing: imagining
a 3D object from a 2D
representation
• Perspective taking: imagining
looking at an object or scene
from a different direction
Spatial thinking includes
(but is not limited to)…
• Penetrative thinking:
imagining what a slice
thru an object would
look like
• Disembedding: attending to
one aspect of a complex scene
Spatial thinking includes
(but is not limited to)…
• Navigation
• Scaling
Spatial Thinking in Geoscience
• Spatial thinking appears to be essential in a number of
subdisciplines in the geosciences
• Faculty describe poor spatial skills as a barrier to success
• Spatial skills training in engineering has led to improved
class performance and retention in the major
Measuring Spatial Skills
• Mental rotation
Measuring Spatial Skills
• Penetrative thinking: imagining the shape of a slice through
an object
Measuring Spatial Skills
• Penetrative thinking: imagining a slice through an object,
including its interior
Measuring Spatial Skills
• Paper folding
• Disembedding *
* Disembedding: isolating and attending to one aspect of a complex display or scene
Measuring Spatial Skills
• Water level
Examples of the range of test
scores, geoscience students
Correlations between
spatial skills tests
Measured improvement, mental
rotation, one academic term
Our Conclusions
• There is a wide range of spatial thinking abilities, even
among geoscience majors in upper-level courses
• Spatial skills cannot be measured and characterized with a
single test; a suite of tests is required
• Spatial thinking skills improve with practice
So… what kind of training/practice would be most
effective?http://serc.carleton.edu/spatialworkbook/index.
html
What kinds of brief, weekly exercises
could you give your students?
Take a few minutes to brainstorm
a list of 10-minute exercises you
could have your students do to
practice the kinds of spatial
thinking that show up in your
courses.
Suggestions:
• Focus on what you want them to be able to do.
• Start simple, and build up to more difficult tasks over time.
• Demonstrate how to approach the exercises. Work an
example before having students tackle the first one.
Spatial Workbook Project:
focuses on penetrative thinking
• Builds on prior work:
• From geoscience education:
• Titus & Horsman’s exercises (JGE, 2009)
• Steve Reynolds’ computer visualizations
of landscapes and structures
• Visible geology tool
(http://app.visiblegeology.com/)
• Our studies thus far
Spatial Workbook Project
• Builds on prior work:
• From cognitive science:
• Progressive alignment
• Gesture
• From STEM education:
• Sorby workbook for engineers
Detailed look at instructional
“intervention” at UW-Madison
• Structural Geology class at the UW-Madison
• Test of 2011-2014 spatial thinking workbook concept
• Spring 2010: collect baseline data
• Pre- and post-tests of students’ penetrative thinking, mental rotation,
and disembedding skills
• Spring 2011:
• Same pre- and post-tests of penetrative thinking, mental rotation, and
disembedding skills
• Weekly penetrative thinking exercises at the beginning of lab
• Designed to take 10-15 minutes
• Variety of forms (pencil & paper, computer visualizations, play-doh)
• Geological and non-geological content
• All required students to sketch one or more cross-sections
Weekly Exercises: analog modeling
• Use play-doh to construct a model of
• An anticline
• Boudinage
• Lineation (for example, a stretched
pebble conglomerate)
• Slice through your model in several
different directions
• Sketch cross-sections parallel,
perpendicular, and oblique to the structural
grain
• How would your cross-sections be different
if the structure (or long axis of the strain
ellipsoid) was plunging?
Weekly Exercises: analog modeling
• Construct a model of a faulted fold, like the one illustrated
below, using play-doh. Slice the model
• Parallel to the fold hinge
• Perpendicular to the fold hinge, through the fault surface
• Sketch the resulting
cross-sections
• Investigate similar
structures:
• Faulted syncline
• Faulted plunging fold
Image by Titus and Horsman
Weekly Exercises:
computer visualizations
• Sketch a cross-section for the slicing
plane indicated, as viewed from the
right side of the block
• Sketch the 3 cross-sections of a human
foot indicated in the diagram below
A
C
B
Weekly Exercises: thought exercises
• Construct a structure contour
map for the top of the gray
unit, using 100m contour
intervals; describe the
orientation of this structure
and the structure contour
lines in words
• Sketch a cross-section from A-A’
Images & exercises by Titus and Horsman
Results: Penetrative Thinking
(ETS planes of reference)
Pre-test
Post-test
Gain
T-test
2010 (N=17)
8.6 (3.2)
10.1 (2.2)
1.5 (2.6)
0.03
2011 (N=16)
7.3 (2.5)
9.0 (3.1)
1.7 (2.2)
0.01
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2010
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2011
5
5
4
4
3
3
2
2
1
1
0
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Pre-test
8
9
10
11
Post-test
12
13
14
15
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Pre-test
8
9
10
11
Post-test
12
13
14
15
Results: Mental Rotation
(PVRT)
Pre-test
Post-test
Gain
T-test
2010
6.0 (2.1)
6.4 (1.9)
0.4 (2.0)
0.48
2011
4.9 (1.8)
6.2 (2.0)
1.3 (1.5)
0.00
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2010
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2011
6
6
5
5
4
4
3
3
2
2
1
1
0
0
1
2
3
4
5
Pre-test
6
7
Post-test
8
9
10
1
2
3
4
5
Pre-test
6
7
Post-test
8
9
10
Results: Disembedding
(ETS hidden figures)
Pre-test
Post-test
Gain
T-test
2010
8.0 (3.2)
9.3 (2.8)
1.3 (3.3)
0.12
2011
7.4 (3.0)
9.3 (3.7)
1.9 (2.8)
0.02
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2010
UW-Madison, Structure, Spring 2011
5
5
4
4
3
3
2
2
1
1
0
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Pre-test
9
10
11
12
Post-test
13
14
15
16
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
Pre-test
9
10
11
12
Post-test
13
14
15
16
UW Structure Class Study Results
• Spring 2010:
• Students demonstrated a wide range of spatial thinking skills
• Students showed modest improvement (on average) on all 3
measures, with statistically significant improvement in
penetrative thinking
• Spring 2011:
• Students demonstrated a wide range of spatial thinking skills
• Students showed greater average gains on all 3 measures, and
all gains were statistically significant
Conclusions
• Spatial thinking skills improve with practice (even 10-15
minutes each week can make a difference)
• Interventions focused on strengthening penetrative thinking
may also help develop other aspects of spatial thinking
• Next year: testing progressive alignment and gesture
exercises. Stay tuned.
What kinds of brief, weekly exercises
could you give your students?
Download