Contrast Sensitivity Handout PSYC 230-Burnham Spatial Acuity and Contrast Recall, spatial frequency refers to the number of times that a pattern (dark/light cycles in a grating) repeats in a given unit of space. For example, to the right, if the width of each box represents 1° of visual angle, then the upper pattern has a spatial frequency of 4 cycles/° and the lower patter has a spatial frequency of 8 cycles/°. The size of a retinal ganglion cell’s receptive field is ‘tuned’ for certain spatial frequencies. That is, a ganglion cell will fire a maximum number of action potentials when light is reflected into its ON-area and nothing is reflected into its OFF-area (the OFF-area is in darkness). For example, in the figure to the right, the hypothetical firing rate of a retinal ganglion cell is displayed for three spatial frequencies. (The firing rate is to the right, where each vertical line marks an action potential over a given unit of time.) The spatial frequencies in (a) and (c) do not elicit a strong response from the ganglion cell. This is, because, in (a) the spatial frequency is to low and the receptive field fits within the lighted area, and in (c) several cycles fit in the ON-area and also Firing Rate the OFF-area. But, the frequency of (b) elicits a near-maximum response rate from the ganglion cell, because the ON-area is contained within the lighted area, and the OFF-area is contained within the darkened areas. Thus, this ganglion cell’s receptive filed best ‘fits’ the spatial frequency in (b). Interestingly, spatial acuity is influenced by both spatial frequency and contrast. The contrast threshold is the minimum difference in intensity, between the darkened bars and the lighted bars of a grating pattern, which is needed to detect the pattern. Thus, it is the absolute threshold for detecting a grating pattern against a background. Typically, to measure the contrast thresholds, a person is presented with a grating pattern against a uniform gray background (see examples below), and is asked to adjust the contrast of the pattern until it is just noticeable. For example, if the dark stripes of a grating patch must be 2% darker than the light stripes, then if the light stripes reflect 1000 photons, the dark stripes need to reflect 980 photons (or fewer) for the pattern to be detected. This 2% difference is the contrast threshold. Contrast Sensitivity Handout PSYC 230-Burnham The contrast sensitivity function is a plot of the reciprocal of the contrast threshold of an individual against the spatial frequency of various grating patches. The contrast sensitivity function resembles an upside-down U, and is presented in the figure to the below. The x-axis of the figure is the spatial frequency of grating patches (in cycles per degree). The y-axis if the contrast sensitivity to a particular frequency, which is the reciprocal of the contrast threshold. Using the example from above, say that the dark stripes of a grating patch must be 2% darker than the light stripes for the pattern to be detected. This 2% (0.2) difference is the contrast threshold, the reciprocal of which is 1/.02 = 200, which is the contrast sensitivity. Higher values on the y-axis are associated with contrasts that we are more sensitive to. From the graph above, you should see that we are most sensitive to spatial frequencies between 6 and 10 cycles/°. Any pattern whose spatial frequency and contrast falls within the area under the upside-down U is visible, and those patterns outside of this area would be invisible (i.e., not visible). The figure below represents the contrast sensitivity function with sine-wave gratings. In the figure below, you should see a sine-wave grating whose contrast increases from the top of the figure to the bottom, and whose spatial frequency increases from left to right along the x-axis. If you hold this paper at about 2-meters, you should, be able to see the upside-down U in the figure. This demonstrates the relationship between spatial frequency and contrast sensitivity.