Redefining Pitch Proximity: Tessitura and Mobility as Constraints on Melodic Intervals Author(s): Paul Von Hippel Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 17, No. 3 (Spring, 2000), pp. 315327 Published by: University of California Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285820 Accessed: 11-06-2015 01:29 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions © 2000 by the regents of the university of California Music Perception Spring 2000, Vol. 17, No. 3, 315-327 RedefiningPitch Proximity:Tessituraand Mobility as Constraintson Melodic Intervals PAUL VON HIPPEL Ohio State University In descriptionsof melodicstructure,pitchproximityis usuallydefinedas the tendencyfor smallpitchintervalsto outnumberlargeones. Thisdefinitionis validas far as it goes;however,an alternativedefinitionis preferable.The alternativedefinespitchproximityin termsof two moreba- a constrainton tessitura(or pitch distribution)and a sic constraints constrainton mobility(or freedomof motion). This new definitionoffersseveraladvantages.Whereasthe usualdefinitionpredictsonly interval size, the new definitionpredictsintervaldirectionas well. The usual definitionpredictssmallintervalsgenerally,whereasthe new definition predictscontext-sensitivevariationsin intervalsize. Finally,if the new definitionis given the first few notes in a melody,it can assigna probability to each of the pitches that could occur next. In sum, the new definitionoffersa morepreciseanddetaileddescriptionof melodicstructure. ReceivedFebruary 29, 1999. 22, 1999;acceptedforpublicationSeptember the simplestpropertyof melodic structureis the rule known as Perhaps pitch proximity.This rule has traditionallybeen definedas a tendency for smallpitchintervalsto outnumberlargeones. A preponderanceof small intervalshas beenmeasuredin a wide varietyof musicalcultures,including indigenousmelodiesfrom Europe,Africa,North America,and the Caribbean (Dowling, 1967; Ortmann,1926; Watt, 1924; Zipf, 1949).1A classic demonstrationby Watt (1924) is displayedin Figure1. Althoughthe traditionaldefinitionis valid as far as it goes, I will argue that an alternativedefinitionis preferable.This alternativedefines pitch proximityas the resultof two more basic constraints.The first constraint affects a melody'sdistributionof pitch heights- in a word, its tessitura. 1. Dowling'sdemonstrationreliedon data reportedby Fucks(1962) and Merriam (1964).Thedatareportedby Merriam,in turn,was originallycollectedbyMerriam,Whinery and Fred(1956). Addresscorrespondenceto Paul von Hippel, School of Music, Ohio State University, 1866 CollegeRoad, Columbus,OH 43210. (e-mail:von-hippel.l@ohio-state.edu) 315 This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 316 Paulvon Hippel Fig. 1. In an analysisof 56 Liederby Schubert,Watt(1924) foundthat smallmelodicintervalsgreatlyoutnumberlargeones. Similarresults,less fullytabulated,wereobtainedfor the songs of two Native Americantribes- the Ojibway and the Lakota. The second constraintaffects a melody'sfreedomof movementwithin its tessitura- in a word, its mobility.These two constraints,on tessituraand mobility,limit a melody'schoice of intervals. At first glance, the proposedchange of definitionmay appeartrivial.I will demonstrate,however,that the new definitionoffersimportantadvantages overthe old one. Whereasthe old definitioncan predictonly the sizes of melodic intervals,the new definitionpredictstheir directionsas well. The old definitionpredictssmall intervalsregardlessof context, whereas the new definitionpredictssystematicvariationsin intervalsize. Finally, the new definitionfits a statisticalformalismthat, given the first several notes in a melody,assigns a probabilityto each of the pitches that could occurnext. This formalismandits corollarieswill be developedin the pages that follow. Constraint 1: Tessitura To begin, we can observeinformallythat everymelody is limitedin its range of pitches, and that most melodiesseem to favor the centerof their range.This preponderanceof moderatepitch heightscould have a variety of causes. On many instruments,pitches of moderateheight are easierto play.Evenwhen extremepitchescan be playedeasily,they tend to produce less definitepitch perceptions(Terhardt,Stoll, & Seewann,1982). Moreover, because melodies move primarilyby small interval,melodies must generallypass throughthe center of their range in order to reachthe ex- This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Redefining Pitch Proximity 317 tremes.Whateverthe cause,theredoes seem to be a tendencyfor melodies to use the fringesof their range sparingly.It is for this reason that musicians use the word "tessitura,"which "differsfrom range in that it does not take into accounta few isolatedpitchesof extraordinarilyhigh or low pitch" (Apel, 1969, p. 839). To make these observationsmore concrete,tests were conductedon a diversesamplecomprising176 indigenousfolk songs from four different continents:30 songs from a Chinesecollection (Chung-kuoyin yiieh yen chiu so [ChineseMusic ResearchInstitute],1959), 24 songs from a South Africancollection (Makeba, 1971), 42 songs chosen at random from a Native AmericanOjibway collection (Densmore,1910, 1913; computer data files availablein von Hippel, 1998), and 80 from a Europeancollection (Schaffrath,1995)- 4 songs chosen at random from each of 20 regions of Europe.One of the Europeanfolk songs is notated in Figure2, and a tally of the song'spitch heightsis displayedas a bar graphin Figure 3. The bargraph'smoundedshapeshows clearlythat most of this melody's pitchesoccurtowardthe middleof its range.Moreprecisely,the pitchheights in this melody- like those in the other sampledmelodies- approximatea normalor Gaussiandistribution.2 It is evidentfromFigure3 that the size of melodicintervalsis constrained by the pitch distribution.Giventhe pitchesused in this melody,the largest possibleintervalis 14 semitones- and that intervalcan be formedonly by straddlingtwo rarepitchesat opposite extremesof the range.In contrast, therearemanyways to formsmallerintervalsusingmoreabundantpitches nearthe middleof the range.To be more precise,we mightpoint out that, givena normaldistributionof pitchheights,abouttwo thirdsof a melody's Fig. 2. A German folk song ("Lasset uns schlingen dem Friihling Blumlein") from the Essen Folksong Collection (Schaffrath, 1995). As a simple test of normality, we can calculate skew and kurtosis values for each 2. of the 176 sampled melodies. Values of 0 would be consistent with a normal distribution, whereas a consistent tendency toward positive or negative values would indicate a departure from normality. According to a sign test, the skews of these melodies did not tend significantly toward positive or negative values (75 positive, 100 negative; %2=3.2, p = .07). The kurtoses did exhibit a significant tendency toward negative values (23 positive, 153 negative; x2 = 95, p < .0001), but the median kurtosis (-.7) did not seem large enough to warrant adjustments. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 318 Paulvon Hippel Fig. 3. The pitch heightstalliedhere come from the melody in Figure2. The distribution exhibitsan approximatelynormalshapein whichmost pitchesoccurnearthe centerof the melody'srange.The standarddeviationof this distributionis an index of tessitura. pitcheswill be within a standarddeviationof the meanpitch.The standard deviation of a melody'spitch heights- 3.5 semitones for the melody in Figure2- can thereforeserveas a usefulindex of tessitura.Medianvalues for the tessituraindex are 3.1 semitonesfor the Europeanfolk songs, 3.8 for the Chinese,4.0 for the South African,and 4.7 for the Ojibway.(The medianrangesare roughlyfour times as large:12 semitonesfor the European folk songs, 15 for the Chinese,17 for the SouthAfrican,and 16.5 for the Ojibway.) The effect of tessituraon intervalsize can be measuredby transforming eachof the sampledmelodiesinto a "scrambledtwin." A scrambledtwin is producedby simplyreorderinga melody'spitchesat random.The resulting twin is identicalto the originalmelodyin its pitchdistribution,but random in its pitch order- random,that is, in its intervals.The intervalspresentin a scrambledtwin arethe exclusiveresultof the pitchdistribution- a result, that is, of the scale, the relativeprevalenceof differentscale degrees,and the tessitura.If intervalsize is constrainedby tessitura,we would expect the scrambledtwins to exhibit a preponderanceof small intervals. For the purpose of this comparison,an intervalcan be defined as the semitonedistancebetweenany consecutivepitchesthat are not separated by a phraseboundaryor rest. To preservethe numberof intervalspresent in the originalmelodies,scrambledtwins can be constructedin such a way that the positions of rests and phrase boundariesare fixed, even as the positions of pitches are randomized.Scrambledtwins of this type were constructedfor each of the 176 sampledmelodies.Talliesof noncompound This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions RedefiningPitchProximity 319 Fig. 4. Noncompoundintervalsizes in folk songs from four differentcontinentsare comparedwith intervalsizes in correspondingsets of "scrambledtwins."Eachscrambledtwin was composedby rearrangingthe pitchesof the correspondingfolk song in a randomorder. The prevalenceof smallintervalsin the scrambledtwinscan resultonly fromconstraintson tessitura.The greaterprevalenceof smallintervalsin the originalfolk songs can be attributed to constraintson mobility. intervalsin the originalmelodiesand their scrambledtwins are displayed in Figure4. To begin by examining specific intervals, the distributions for the scrambledtwins displaysharppeaks at the consonantintervalsof 5 and 7 semitones(the perfectfourthand perfectfifth), as well as a deep troughat the dissonantintervalof 1 semitone (the minor second). These peaks and troughsare probablyan artifactof scale structure.Both the pitch content of common scales and the relativeprevalenceof certainscale degreestend to maximizethe possibilityof consonantintervalsand minimizethe possibilityof dissonantintervals(Huron, 1994; Smith, 1997). Settingasidethesedetailedobservations,Figure4 exhibitsa coarsergeneral pattern:In all foursetsof scrambledtwins,smallintervalsgreatlyoutnumber largeones.Thispatternis consistentwith the simpleclaimthatthe constraint on tessitura,by itself,would tendto producean excessof smallintervals. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Paul von Hippel 320 Constraint 2: Mobility Althoughsmall intervalsare prevalentin the scrambledtwins, they are considerablymore prevalentin the originalmelodies. The comparisonis summarizedin Table 1, which shows that the mean intervalsize for each repertoireis one to four semitonessmallerthan the mean intervalsize for that repertoire'sscrambledtwins. Whenthe 176 originalmelodiesareconsideredindividually,all but 5 of them have a smallermean intervalsize than the correspondingscrambledtwin. Theseresultsshow that, when the pitchdistributionis controlled,melodicintervalsremainconsiderablysmaller than would be expectedby chance. This simple analysiscorroboratesthe intuitionthat intervalsize is subject to a secondconstraintbeyondthat of tessitura.This secondconstraint limitsthe freedomwith which a melodycan move throughits tessitura- it is, in other words, a constrainton mobility.The effect of mobility constraintscan be describedby the observationthat, even allowing for the compact distributionof a melody'spitch heights, each pitch tends to be close to the pitch before.More formally,we would expect most melodies to have a positivecorrelationbetweenthe heightsof consecutivepitchesthat is, a positive lag-one autocorrelation. The strength of this autocorrelationmay serveas an index of constraintson mobility. For all but one of the 176 melodies in our sample, the lag-one autocorrelationbetweenconsecutivepitch heightsis positive.When pitch heightis measuredin semitones,the medianvalue of this autocorrelationis .46 for the Chinesefolk songs, .59 for the Europeanfolk songs, .59 for the SouthAfricanfolk songs, and .84 for the Ojibwayfolk songs. Notice that the Ojibwayfolk songs have the most constrainedmobility;this explains how they can have the widest tessituraof the four repertoires(medianindex, 4.7 semitones), yet still have the smallest mean interval size (1.6 semitones). Table 1 Mean Interval Sizes in Four Folk Song Repertoires and in Their Scrambled Twins Meanintervalsize (semitones) in ScrambledTwins FolkSongRepertoire in OriginalMelodies Chinese European South African Ojibway 23 2.2 2.3 1.6 3^8 3.7 4.3 5.5 MelodiesWithSmaller MeanIntervalSizesThan TheirScrambledTwins 26 79 24 42 out out out out of of of of This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 30 80 24 42 Redefining Pitch Proximity 321 To illustratethis index of mobility,consideragain the melody displayed in Figure1. In Figure5, we have plotted each pitch in this melody against the pitchthat succeedsit. The stronglag-oneautocorrelationbetweensuccessivepitchheights(.70) indicatesthat, evencomparedwith the restof the Europeansample,the mobilityof this folk song is highlyconstrained. Forecasting Pitch Heights Tests of the four sampledrepertoiresindicatethat the autocorrelation between successive pitch heights is approximatelylinear. That is, on a scatterplotlike Figure5, the points fit a straightline ratherthan a higher ordercurve.3The existenceof such a linear autocorrelationsuggeststhat the height of any pitch can be predicted,with some error,by a first-order autoregressionformula.In particular,if the first severalnotes in a melody of ru (?!, ..., PiA)havea meanof m semitonesand a lag-oneautocorrelation the most likelyheightfor the next pitch(P,)will fit the followingequation: P.-m=r1(PiA-m) (1) where,in practice,the predictedpitch height (P.)would be roundedto the nearestdiscretepitch. Put into words, this equationmay be interpretedin the followingterms. - m > 0), the next pitch tends to be someAftera high pitch (i.e., when Piml mean to the what lower (i.e., closer by a factorof rx).Similarly,aftera low pitch,the next pitch tendsto be somewhathigher.Both of these tendencies are strongerwhen the antecedentpitch is extreme(i.e., P,_xis distantfrom m) or when the constrainton mobilityis weak (i.e., rxis small). These phenomena,familiarto statisticiansas "regressiontoward the mean," are straightforwardresults of the constrainton tessitura.Simply put, after an extreme pitch- or, for that matter,before it- most of the availablepitches are quite a bit closer to the middle of the tessitura.For this reason,upwardintervals,especiallylargeones, tend to begin low and end high,whereasdownwardintervals,especiallylargeones, tend to begin high and end low. These tendenciesare illustratedin the two panels of Figure6, which shows that for the Germanfolk song in Figure1- as for all of the 176 sampledmelodies- intervals(as measuredin semitones)exhibit As an informal test of linearity (Darlington, 1990), we examined residual 3. scatterplots for eight randomly selected melodies (two from each of the four repertoires); these scatterplots exhibited no obvious signs of curvilinearity. As a more formal test (Darlington, 1990), we tried to fit quadratic curves to the scatterplots for each of the 176 melodies. In general, these curves explained only marginally more of the variance than could be explained by a straight line (median increment in R2= .01). It therefore seems that the correlation between successive pitch heights could be reasonably characterized as linear. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 322 Paulvon Hippel Fig. 5. The pitch successionsplotted here come from the melody in Figure3; the area of each point is proportionalto the numberof timesthat the correspondingpitchsuccession occurs in the melody.Each of the melody'spitchestends to be followed by a pitch that, consideringthe melody'stessitura,is nearby.Accordingly,the lag-oneautocorrelationbetween successivepitchheights(hererx= .70) can serveas an index of the melody'smobility. (Note. Musiciansoften use the terms"antecedent"and "consequent"in a specializedsense relatingto musicalrhetoric.In this paper,however,the terms simply referto successive pitches.) a negativecorrelationwith the heightof theirstartingpitch, and a positive correlationwith the heightof theirendingpitch. The tendenciesillustratedin Figure6 have a numberof straightforward corollaries.One corollaryis that the peak pitch in a phrase- which is, by definition,high- tends to be approachedand left by skip. A secondcorollaryis that aftera skip- whichtendsto landon an extremepitch- a melody is likelyto retreatby changingdirection.Althoughboth of thesetendencies have been claimed as independentprinciplesof melodic structure(Eitan, 1993, 1997; Meyer,1956; Nanino & Nanino, ca. 1600; Narmour,1990; Toch, 1948), recentanalysessuggestthat they are mereside effectsof constraintson tessitura(Huron, 1999; von Hippel & Huron, 1999). Assigning Probabilities to Pitches A testof the scatterplotin Figure5- andsimilartestsforthe othersampled melodies- indicatesthat the variancesaroundthe autoregressionline are This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions QJ i, ^ «5 G js as a§ iiiji .5 ^ ^ h « £ -a •&:§s im <u G -m _Ct Iflii S> 2 > .ti m «"S'S | g s 'o a a a Ï"S G g ^O'BbO .^ 2 ^ »h-S w §11111 S *O"g 3J Sa u §i "ri-if a s" §S .St:JSu-.g Sb b J3 8 2 3J$;5.8 filin .s ^ g a .2 « 1 B 121 .SPo H S'tJ.SP Ph o cd cr> "O (ii This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 324 Paul von Hippel roughly homogeneous. That is, regardless of the antecedent pitch height, the heights of consequent pitches are about equally variable.4 Given this property, our autoregression formula can do more than simply predict the pitch that is most likely to occur next- the formula can also assign a probability to the subsequent occurrence of any given pitch. In particular, if the first several notes of a melody (Pu ..., PM) fit a normal distribution with mean of m, a standard deviation of s, and an autocorrelation of ru the probabilities for the next note will fit a normal distribution. The mean of this probability distribution, as indicated above in equation (1), will be P,= fi(Pi-i- m) semitones, and the standard deviation will be Vl - rx2semitones (Brockwell &cDavis, 1996). Note that the width of the probability distribution (Vl-ra2) depends on both tessitura and mobility; in particular,pitches are predicted with greater certainty when the tessitura is narrow (s is small) or the mobility is highly constrained (rxis large). In practice, this continuous probability distribution will be broken into discrete bins corresponding to the available pitch classes. An example is displayed in Figure 7. Here the melody from Figure 2 has been interrupted at the end of its sixth measure, and a probability distribution has been generated for the following pitch. Because the mobility is constrained, the peak of the probability distribution is near the most recent pitch; moreover, because the most recent pitch is high in the tessitura, the peak of the probability distribution is somewhat lower. For melodies with a downward trend- for example, most Ojibway melodies (Densmore, 1913) - the next note will usually be lower than our formula predicts. But for a melody that maintains a consistent tessitura, the formula's predictions should be roughly correct- at least with respect to pitch height. In Figure 7, for example, it is encouraging to observe that the Fig. 7. Here the melody from Figure 2 has been interrupted at the end of its sixth measure, and our definition of pitch proximity has been used to generate a probability distribution for the next pitch. Because the mobility is constrained, the most likely pitches are predicted to be close to the most recent pitch. Moreover, because the most recent pitch is high in the tessitura, the most likely pitches are predicted to be somewhat lower. A simple test for homogeneity of variance is the rank correlation between the 4. predictions of the regression equation and the absolute values of the residuals (Madansky, 1988). For the 176 sampled melodies, the median value of this rank correlation is minuscule (r = -.05), indicating that the variances are close to homogeneous. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Redefining Pitch Proximity 325 second-highestprobability(18%) is assignedto the pitch A, which does in fact occurnext in the melody. Like prior definitionsof pitch proximity,the new definition takes no account for scale structure.It assigns probabilitiesbased entirelyon the heightsof pitches,regardlessof wherethose pitchesfit in a melody'sscale. In Figure7, for example,the definitionassignsthe highestprobability(19%) to the pitch At, which does not fit the fragment'sD-majorscale. If I were proposinga completemodel of melodic structure,ratherthan an isolated definitionfor pitch proximity,I would probablycombinethe bell curvein Figure7 with a seconddistributionrepresentingthe prevalenceof different scale degrees.The key-profiledistributionsexploredby Krumhansl(1990) and her colleaguesmight be suitablefor this purpose. Conclusion To review,researchershave traditionallydefined pitch proximity as a simpleabundanceof smallpitch intervals.I advocatereplacingthis definition with a more refined alternative.Statistically,the proposed revision definespitch proximityas a positivelag-one autocorrelationbetweensuccessivepitch heightsdrawnfrom a quasi-normaldistribution.More informally,the new definitionis based on the premisethat intervalsare constrainedboth by a melody'stessituraand by its mobility. This new definitionoffers severaladvantagesover the old one. 1. Whereasthe old definitioncan predictonly the sizes of melodic intervals,the new definitioncan predicttheir directionsas well. In general,the new definitionpredictsthat intervalswill retreat from the extremesof the tessituraand approachthe middle:intervalsthat start on high pitches will proceed downward, and intervalsthat start on low pitcheswill proceedupward. 2. Whereasthe old definitionpredictssmall intervalsregardlessof context, the new definitionpredictsthat intervalsizes will vary dependingon the melody's position in its tessitura. Near the middleof a melody'stessitura,the new definitionpredictssmall intervals,but nearthe extremes,the new definitionpredictsrelatively largeintervals. 3. The new definitionsubsumestwo tendenciesobservedby music theorists:thetendencyfor a melodicpeakto be approachedby skip andthetendencyfor a skipto be followedby a changeof direction. Althoughearlierwork has presentedthesetendenciesas independent rulesof melodicstructure,underthe new definitionthey are seenas emergingfrombasicconstraintson mobilityandtessitura. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 326 Paulvon Hippel 4. The new definitionfits a statisticalformalismthat, giventhe first severalnotes in a melody, assigns a probabilityto each of the pitchesthat could occur next. More specifically,the new definition predictsthat the probabilitiesfor the next pitch will fit a normaldistributioncenteredbetweenthe most recentpitch and the mean pitch of the melody.Both the peak and the width of this distributionare defined by statisticalmeasuresof tessitura and mobility. In sum, comparedwith the old definitionof pitch proximity,the predictions associatedwith the new definitionaremorepreciseanddetailed,more sensitiveto melodic context, and broaderin their implications.In other words, the new definitionenablesa sharperand deeperdescriptionof melodic structure. Although this definitionof pitch proximitywas developedto describe melodic structure,it might plausiblydescribemelodicpsychologyas well. A rule of pitch proximityis often used to account for such cognitivephenomenaas melodicexpectations,melodicpreferences,andauditorystreaming (Bregman,1990; Carlsen, 1981; Dowling, 1967). In these cognitive applications,just as in descriptionsof musical structure,pitch proximity has traditionallybeen defined simply in terms of intervalsize. In light of the presentresults,however,thereis good reasonto try an alternativedefinition.Perhapsmelodiccognition,likemelodicstructure,couldbe described more preciselyin termsof constraintson tessituraand mobility.5 References Apel,W. (1969). Harvarddictionaryof music.(2ndéd.). Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversity Press. Bregman,A. S. (1990). Auditoryscene analysis:The perceptualorganizationof sound. Cambridge,MA: The MIT Press. Brockwell,P.J., & Davis, R. A. (1996). Introductionto time seriesand forecasting.New York:SpringerVerlag. Carlsen,J. C. (1981). Somefactorswhichinfluencemelodicexpectancy.Psychomusicology, 1, 12-29. Chung-kuoyin yiieh yen chiu so [ChineseMusic ResearchInstitute].(1959). Chung-kuo min ke Chung-kuoyin yiiehyen chiuso pien [Chinesefolk songs].Beijing:Yinyiiehch'u pan she. Darlington,R. B. (1990). Regressionand linearmodels.New York:McGraw-Hill. Densmore,F.(1910).Chippewa[i.e., Ojibway]music.Washington,DC:Government Printing Office. Densmore,F. (1913). Chippewa[i.e., Ojibway]music- II. Washington,DC: Government PrintingOffice. 5. I thankDavid Huron,David Temperley,actioneditorAdrianHoutsma,and two anonymousreviewersfor thoughtfulcommentson draftsof this paper. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Redefining Pitch Proximity 327 Dowling, W. J. (1967). Rhythmic fission and the perceptual organization of tone sequences. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA. Eitan, Z. (1993). Melodic contour and musical style: A quantitative study. In M. G. Boroda (Ed.), Fundamentals of musical language: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 1-68). Bochum, Germany: Brockmeyer. Eitan, Z. (1997). Highpoints: A study of melodic peaks. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Fucks, W. (1962). Mathematical analysis of formal structure of music. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 8, S225-S228. Huron, D. (1994). Interval-classcontent in equally tempered pitch-class sets: Common scales exhibit optimum tonal consonance. Music Perception, 11(3), 289-305. Huron, D. (1999). Zohar Eitan: Highpoints: A study of melodic peaks [Review of the book]. Music Perception, 16(2), 257-264. Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Madansky, A. (1988). Prescriptions for working statisticians. New York: Springer-Verlag. Makeba, M. (1971). The world of African song. Chicago: Quadrangle Press. Merriam, A. (1964). Anthropology of music. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. Merriam, A. P., Whinery, S., 8c Fred, B. G. (1956). Songs of a Rada community in Trinidad. Anthropos, 51, 157-174. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Nanino, G. M., ÔCNanino, G. B. (ca. 1600). Regole di contrappunto [Rules of counterpoint]. Unpublished manuscript, Civico Museo Bibliografico Musicale, Bologna, Italy. Narmour, E. (1990). The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures: The implication-realization model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Ortmann, O. R. (1926). On the melodic relativity ot tones. Psychological Monographs, 35(162). * Schaffrath, H. (1995). The Essen folksong collection in the Humdrum *kernformat [ComResearch in the Hufor Assisted CA: Center Menlo Park, Computer puter database]. manities. Smith, A. B. (1997). A "cumulative" method of quantifying tonal consonance in musical key contexts. Music Perception, 15(2), 175-188. Terhardt, E., Stoll, G., & Seewann, M. (1982). Pitch of complex signals according to virtual-pitch theory: Tests, examples, and predictions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71(3), 679-688. Toch, E. (1948). The shaping forces in music: An inquiry into harmony, melody, counterpoint, form. New York: Criterion Music. von Hippel, P. (1998). 42 Ojibway folksongs in the Humdrum * *kem representation: Electronic transcriptions from the Densmore collection [Computer database]. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University, Cognitive and Systematic Musicology Laboratory (www.music-cog.ohio-state.edu/~pvh). von Hippel, P., & Huron, D. (1999). Why do skips precede reversals?The effect of tessitura on melodic structure. Manuscript submitted for publication. Watt, H. J. (1924). Functions of the size of interval in the songs of Schubert and of the Chippewa [i.e., Ojibway] and Teton Sioux [i.e., Lakota] Indians. British Journal of Psychology, 14(4), 370-386. Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Press. This content downloaded from 140.254.87.149 on Thu, 11 Jun 2015 01:29:54 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions