Flower Constancy: Definition, Cause, and Measurement Nickolas M. Waser The American Naturalist, Vol. 127, No. 5. (May, 1986), pp. 593-603. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0003-0147%28198605%29127%3A5%3C593%3AFCDCAM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-J The American Naturalist is currently published by The University of Chicago Press. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/ucpress.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers, and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. http://www.jstor.org Thu Dec 20 18:04:01 2007 Vol. 127, No. 5 The American Naturalist May 1986 FLOWER CONSTANCY: DEFINITION, CAUSE, AND MEASUREMENT N I C K O L M. A ~ WASER Department of Biology. University of California, Riverside. Cal~fornia93521 Slrbr~ittedMro.c.11 9 , 1985: Acc.eptccl Scpter~ihcr13. 1985 Pollinating animals sometimes restrict their visits to flowers of a single species or morph within a species, even when rewarding alternative flowers must be bypassed in the process. The tendency to specialize in this way, common in some social bees, has been referred to asflower cotlJtancy (Plateau 1901; Clements and Long 1923; Grant 1950; Free 1966, 19700; Levin and Anderson 1970). The striking features of constancy are that individual pollinators, even members of the same bee colony, specialize on different flower types, and each may switch its specialty from time to time (Grant 1950; Free 1966; Heinrich 1976, 19790; Wells and Wells 1983; Wells et al. 1983). These features indicate that flowcrs passed over by an individual at a given time are indeed rewarding, since they are acceptable to conspecific individuals or to the same individual at another time. It is useful to distinguish constancy from two other kinds of specialization that might occur when there is access to several flower types. A pollinator may specialize because it has fixed floral affinities, as do oligotropic or oligolectic solitary bees (see Faegri and van der Pijl 1979; Schemske 1983). 1 refer to :his as $xed preference. Alternatively, a species without fixed affinities may speci- I'ize on flowers that are abundant and rewarding. Such behavior is predicted by simple optimal-diet theory (e.g., MacArthur and Pianka 1966). It should occi~rwhen the most rewarding flowers are sufficiently abundant and superior that the highest reward-intake rate is achieved by skipping inferior flowers, even though this elevates travel costs relative to a generalized diet. I refer to this as lrrbile pr.c.f>r.ence. There is good evidence that pollinators such as bumblebees and temperate hummingbirds choose flowers partly on the basis of the energetic value of rewards the flowers contain (Heinrich 1976; Pleasants 1981; Gass and Montgomerie 1981; Pyke 1981; Waser 19836). Some workers have equated constancy with fixed preference (Grant 1950; Linsley and MacSwain 1958; Faegri and van der Pijl 1979), others with labile preference (see below), but I advocate reserving the term for a third kind of specialization (see also Bateman 1951; Levin and Anderson 1970; Wells and Wells 1983). Suppose that a pollinator can remember how to recognize or manipulate only one or a small number of flower types or requires a long time to learn to recognize or manipulate each type. Such behavioral constraints favor specializaAm. Nat. 1986. Vol. 127. pp. 593-603. S 1986 by The U n ~ v s ~ h of ~ t Ch~cago. y 0003-0147.'86.'2705-0008$02.00 A l l rlphtr lsaelvsd 5 94 T H E AMERICAN NATURALIST tion because they elevate mean handling time per flower when several types are visited. By skipping all but one or a few types the pollinator saves handling costs, since it does not invest in further learning or successive relearning. Skipping is the best strategy if reduced handling time more than offsets increased travel costs (McNair 1981; Stanton 1984). In this view constancy derives from something intrinsic in the pollinator, namely, limitations of its nervous system. Fixed preference also derives from intrinsic physiological and morphological limitations, but labile preference derives from something largely extrinsic to the pollinator, namely, abundances of flowers and the rewards they contain. Unless the three kinds of specialization occur in some combination (which is not unlikely), each should be distinguishable by its behavioral "signature" (see also Wells et al. 1983). With constancy, the specialty should differ among conspecific individuals depending on the sequence of acceptable flowers each has encountered, since encounter history could determine which type is learned first (see Heinrich 19796). Moreover, the behavior should occur even when available flower types all offer similar, energetically acceptable rewards (as defined operationally in the first paragraph of this paper), that is, when simple diet theory predicts generalization. With fixed preference, conspecifics should have concordant and unchanging specialties. With labile preference, individuals should have concordant specialties that change predictably according to floral abundances and rewards, and specialization should occur only when rewards differ substantially among flowers (see MacArthur and Pianka 1966). After elaborating on memory and learning constraints that may cause constancy, I discuss definitions of constancy and preference in terms of flower-toflower movements, a corresponding quantitative constancy index, and suitable empirical measures of pollinator behavior. I cite some experimental evidence suggesting that constancy is indeed caused by behavioral constraints, and I mention recent examples of confusion between constancy and preference and some possible consequences of each behavior for plant reproduction. BEHAVIORAL CONSTRAINTS THAT FAVOR CONSTANCY Darwin seems to have been the first to propose that behavioral constraints are involved in constancy: That insects should visit the flowers of the same species for as long as they can. is of great importance to the plant. as it favours the cross-fertilisation of distinct individuals of the same species; but no one will suppose that insects act in this manner for the good of the plant. The cause probably lies in insects being thus enabled to work quicker: they have just learned how to stand in the best position on the flower, and how far and in what direction to insert their proboscides. (1876. p. 419.) Here Darwin refers to morphological differences among flowers, but the idea can be extended to differences in other traits involved in flower recognition or manipulation, such as color, odor, or the type of floral reward (e.g., nectar vs. pollen). There is good evidence for constancy based solely on color (for reviews, see Waser 1983u,b). I have alluded to two kinds of behavioral constraint that favor constancy (cf. Pietrewicz and Kamil 1981). One possibility, the "memory hypothesis," is that FLOWER CONSTANCY 595 pollinators are limited in their ability to remember simultaneously how to recognize or manipulate more than a few flower types. This is similar to one view of the search image (Dawkins 1971; Pietrewicz and Kamil 1981; Stanton 1984). Another possibility, the "learning hypothesis," is that pollinators require a substantial time to learn to recognize and handle each type. Most discussion of what causes constancy (Heinrich 1976, 19796; Laverty 1980) invokes the learning hypothesis, and it is certain that learning to deal with flowers takes time. There is also evidence, however, that bees (Kugler 1943; Heinrich et al. 1977; Gould 1985), butterflies (Stanton 1984), and birds (Cowie and Krebs 1979; Pietrewicz and Kamil 1981) have short "memory windows" (Krebs et al. 1981). This is just the constraint required by the memory hypothesis. It means that information, for example about how to manipulate one flower type, can be lost because it is replaced by information about a new flower type or sin~plybecause time has passed. There is another reason to suspect that memory constraints are important for constancy. With large memory capacity all flower types could be learned eventually. The total time required to learn five types (for example) should be on the order of hours at most, judging from learning experiments with hummingbirds and bees (Weaver 1956; Menzel et al. 1974; Heinrich 1976, 19796; Heinrich et al. 1977; Goldsmith and Goldsmith 1979; Laverty 1980; Waser 19836). This is much shorter than the expected pollinator life span (weeks to years; Heinrich 1 9 7 9 ~Calder ; et al. 1983) or the flowering of plants (usually weeks; Waser 1978 and references therein). A pollinator with a large memory should not be constant if it experiences flower types for a period much longer than that needed for learning. since the learning investment for each type would be made only once and amply repaid by savings in travel costs over a long period. The memory and learning hypotheses are not mutually exclusive. Indeed. the latter can be considered a special case of the former, in which a flower type must be learned only once rather than relearned continually. In some cases, the two hypotheses do yield different predictions. For example. if initial learning alone is involved, pollinators that encounter several flower types should become less constant and increasingly proficient at using all types. If limited memory is involved, pollinators should not become less constant, and if there is any variation, the least constant individuals should be least proficient and forage most slowly (see also Stanton 1984). Some predictions are shared by the two hypotheses, at least when one is dealing with naive pollinators. For example, constancy should decrease as travel costs increase and outweigh the extra learning cost associated with inconstant foraging. Thus, constancy should depend on flower density (for a hint of this, see Grant 1949, p. 89; Brown and Clegg 1984). In addition, constancy should increase as a visitor encounters flowers that are increasingly dissimilar in morphology or color. This last prediction is upheld by the experimental results discussed later. Specialization should be discernible in pollinator movements between flowers or plants. Imagine the simplest situation in which a pollinator at each of two flower T H E AMERICAN NATURALIST FIG. 1.-Notation used in Bateman's index and table 2 for the frequencies of all possible transitions between flowers of two types. 1 and 2. Constant transitions are those within types. i.e., A and D. Inconstant transitions are those between types. i.e.. B and C . types has certain probabilities of next visiting type 1 and type 2. Actual movements can be cast into a matrix of transition frequencies (fig. I ) that reflects these probabilities, and the sequence of visits can be thought of as a Markov process (Straw 1972). Bateman (1951) proposed a constancy index that depends on the tendency for all transitions to occur between like flowers (see also Levin and Anderson 1970; Straw 1972). A pollinator leaving any flower has equal access to flowers of types 1 and 2. When transition frequencies are known (see fig. l), Bateman's index is CONS = [(AD)'!? - (BC)l'?]/ [(AD)[!" (BC)'!?] . Ranging from - 1 (complete inconstancy; all transitions between unlike flowers) to 0 (random transitions) to + 1 (complete constancy; all transitions between like flowers), this index is identical to Yule's (1912) "measure of colligation" for the analysis of contingency tables (discussed further in Bateman 1951; see also Fisher and Bailey 1949). The index has the interesting property of being insensitive to changes in transition frequencies A and C by a constant multiple, that is, to a fixed change in the probability of approaching flower type 1 regardless of the current flower. A reasonable interpretation is that this change represents increased preference for type 1. By this definition, Bateman's index is insensitive to preference changes; it is altered only by unequal changes in transition frequencies between like and unlike flowers in the same row or column. If A increases proportionately more than C , for example, CONS will increase. An interpretation is that the chance of visiting type 1 is greater if the pollinator is already at that type. This behavior is in accordance with constancy; thus, the index seems reasonable. A broader definition of preference involves any change in total transitions to one or another flower type (i.e., in the column totals of fig. 1; Fisher and Bailey 1949, p. 227; Levin and Anderson 1970; Chesson 1983), rather than a strictly proportional change in each column element. I will not pursue different definitions further, but I use the ratio of column totals to estimate preference in an example mentioned later. FLOWER CONSTANCY 597 EMPIRICAL MEASUREMENT OF CONSTANCY To measure constancy quantitatively requires information about movements between flowers that can be evaluated with an index such as Bateman's. This information cannot be gleaned from the purity of pollen loads carried by pollinators, a traditional constancy measure (Clements and Long 1923; Linsley and MacSwain 1958; Free 1963, 1970h; Kislev et al. 1972; Grace and Nelson 1981). Such an indirect measure does not take into account floral availabilities and thus does not serve even to demonstrate specialization, much less to distinguish among the various kinds. For example, a pure load may mean simply that the pollinator foraged in an area containing only one flower type or that it visited several types but actively collected pollen from only one (Free 19700). Observing pollinator movements in nature (Bennett 1883; Christy 1883; Plateau 1901; McNaughton and Harper 1960; Heinrich 1976) is an obvious, direct way of assessing choice, but there is a drawback. Given highly nonrandom spatial mixing of flower types and complex pollinator movements, it is difficult to generate accurate expectations of encounter frequencies with different flowers (see Stanton 1984). Known encounter rates are needed if one hopes to ascribe a positive value of Bateman's index to something other than a patchy distribution of plant species. Field observations may suffice to infer the presence of constancy, however (e.g., Stucky 1984). One way to measure constancy quantitatively is to control flower availabilities experimentally. This approach was pioneered by Clements and Long (1923), who presented pollinators with "bouquets" of different flowers (see also Jones 1978; Thomson 1981). Others have arranged artificial flowers into regular arrays and exposed them to pollinators in the laboratory (Manning 1957; Waddington and Holden 1979; Real 1981; Wells et al. 1983). Large arrays allow the availabilities of flowers to be controlled and all types of movements between the flowers to be scored simultaneously, thus generating results appropriate for quantitative indexes of constancy and preference. A R R A Y EXPERIMENTS I have conducted several array experiments in the field to-test the prediction derived above that constancy increases as a visitor encounters increasingly dissimilar flowers. Each array contained two flower types (table 1). In each experiment there were two or three arrays that differed in how distinct their constituent flowers were in color andlor morphology (table 2, "Major Differences" column). Good arguments can be made that my ranking of how strongly pairs of flowers differed was consistent with what a bee would experience. In April 1979 1 scored visits of nectar-collecting solitary bees (Diadasiu sp.; Anthophoridae) to annual wild flowers in Organ Pipe Cactus National Monument, Arizona. There were two arrays separated by 2 m, each of which measured 1 m by 1 m and contained 100 vases, spaced 10 cm apart, in which 50 flowers of two species were alternated. The flowers were species visited naturally by the bees in adjacent areas. TABLE l P L , ~ NUSED T ~ I N ARRAY EXPERIMENTS Site o r Source Arizona Costa Rica Species Morphology' Color Hriplopclpplrs spinlilosis Gaillardia ciriionica Clzrietznctis stei,ioides Digitalis purplrreri Fllc.Iisicr cirhoreacens Agercltirla sp. composite. disk and ray composite, disk and ray composite, disk only broad tube narrow tube composite. disk only small. short petals large, longer petals yellow yellow white purple or white purple white dark yellow+ pale yellow " "Disk and ray" refers to floret type. t These are flower colors, but vegetation color also differed within each Brnssic.a species TABLE 2 Hs + Ga 36 19 18 25 .24 None 12 9.2 fl 61 4 4 30 .83 C 12 9.3 fl Dp(purple) + Dp(white) vs. Dp(purple) + FA or VS. Dp(white) + A sp. 141 104 105 125 .I2 C 39 13.3 inf 139 47 42 31 .19 M 23 12.3 inf 117 14 14 72 .74 M 30 8.3 inf + Bn(yellow) or Bo(red) + Bo(green) 72 29 33 51 .32 C 26 NA 55 6 9 17 .62 C 26 NA Bol 185 14 15 87 .80 C+M 26 NA + Bn(yellow) VS. Bn + Bo# 23 7 7 7 .29 C 8 NA 44 3 4 2 .46 C+M 8 NA VS. Cs + Ga Bn(white) vs. Bn Bn(white) + ' Abbreviations for species names: H s . Hnplopclppus spinlrlosis; Ga. Gnillclrdiri ariioriiccl: Cs, Ch~enclctisstevioides; Dp, Digitcilis plrrplrrecl: Fa, Flcchsicl rirhorrscrns: A sp.. Agercitina sp.: Bn. Brcissicci nriplrs: Bo, Brua~iccioleraceel. Digitalis prrtplrreci had two flower colors and Brrissiccl spp. had several vegetation colors: these are specified in parentheses. t Species listed first in a mixture is "type 1" (see fig. 1 ) in calculations of transition frequencies. j: Major characteristics (M = morphology. C = color) distinguishing flowers in an array. $ Mean number of flowers (fl) in inflorescences (inf) visited if known. This comparison involved honeybees. # This comparison involved solitary bees. ' FLOWER CONSTANCY 599 In March 1981 1 observed nectar-collecting bumblebees, Bornbus ephippiut~ls, at Cerro de la Muerte, Costa Rica. There were three arrays spaced 3 m apart; each measured 60 cm by 40 cm and contained 24 vases, spaced 10 cm apart, in which 12 inflorescences of two species were alternated. I pruned inflorescences to produce floral "flags" of about the same size ( 5 flowers of Digitulis prirprirea, 10 flowers or heads of other species). I scored movements between inflorescences, since each contained several flowers. Again, the flowers were species visited naturally by the bees in adjacent areas. Finally, I analyzed two experiments from Bateman (1951), in which honeybees and unidentified solitary bees visited mustards in a garden. Bateman did not specify interplant spacing or whether bees collected nectar or pollen. Only movements between plants were considered. In all pairwise comparisons of arrays exposed simultaneously to pollinators, constancy was greater in the array containing flowers of dissimilar color andlor morphology than in the array containing more-similar flowers (table 2; N = 6, one-tailed sign test, P = .016). This is the result predicted if the behavior is caused by memory or learning constraints. DISCUSSION Given the widespread interest in constancy among crop scientists and ecologists, it is surprising that the five studies cited earlier and a few noted by Clements and Long (1923) appear to be the only attempts to measure constancy using direct behavioral observation in the field. Array experiments are equally rare; those discussed here, few as they are, are all that I know of. Arrays allow the simultaneous scoring of all possible transitions between flower types whose accessibility is controlled, exactly what is required for an index such as Bateman's. It is more difficult to score all transitions or to achieve controlled accessibility with artificial bouquets, and bouquet studies to date do not provide information suitable for measuring constancy quantitatively. For example, Jones (1978) used bouquets in a way that allowed for all possible transitions between flowers, but not at one time and place. Thomson (1981) constructed bouquets of several species, presented them to bumblebees naturally foraging at flowers of one species, and considered movements onto that species in the bouquet to represent constancy. He scored only certain transitions, however, corresponding to one row of figure 1. Since either constancy or preference will influence row elements, the behavior Thomson recorded cannot be unambiguously ascribed to constancy. Constancy and labile preference have not been distinguished in several recent laboratory studies. Bumblebee choice based on reward quality is called constancy by Heinrich et al. (1977). Waddington and Holden's (1979) constancy model and test also involved preference for a superior reward. Laboratory studies have not been designed to measure constancy quantitatively, although some have certainly demonstrated that it occurs (Wells and Wells 1983; Wells et al. 1983). Distinguishing constancy from preference is of value if one is interested in plant reproduction. It has been argued that plants can compete for pollination by the usurpation of pollinator visits or through interspecific pollen transfer (Waser 600 T H E AMERICAN NATURALIST 19830; Campbell and Motten 1985). Interspecific pollen transfer might lead to stigma clogging, disruption by foreign pollen, or the loss of pollen deposited on foreign flowers, each of which could reduce fitness. The potential for each mechanism of interaction can be inferred from records of pollinator movements. For example, Delphinilltn nelsonii and Ipotnopsis aggregata compete for hummingbird pollination, and hummingbirds visiting arrays (Waser 1978) show constancy of only 0.04 according to Bateman's index. Hummingbirds also show moderate preference: the ratio of total transitions to each type (Chesson 1983) is 1.57 in favor of I. aggregata. Thus, both species might suffer from interspecific pollen transfer, and D. rielsonii might suffer more because I. aggregata is more attractive, a conclusion supported by observations in natural populations and experiments (Waser 1978; Kohn and Waser 1985; R . Mitchell, pers. comm.). I hope to have demonstrated the value of attempting to define constancy in more mechanistic and quantitative terms and the value of attempting to formulate testable hypotheses about why foragers exhibit such behavior. The experimental evidence at hand is consistent with the hypothesis that constancy is related to limited ability to learn or remember how to recognize several flower types at once or how to deal with them efficiently. Thus, the evidence suggests that constancy is not a highly evolved adaptation as has been implied in the pollination literature (Grant 1949, 1950; Levin 1978; Faegri and van der Pijl 1979); rather, it represents an imperfect adaptation attributable to behavioral constraints. SUMMARY A pollinator that restricts its visits to one flower type, even when other rewarding types are accessible, can be said to exhibit flower constancy. This usage distinguishes constancy from fixed preference or labile preference for the most rewarding flower type; I discuss a quantitative constancy index that is insensitive to preference changes. Because a constant visitor avoids flowers with acceptable rewards, the behavior is inefficient unless there are constraints such as an inability to learn quickly or to remember simultaneously how to deal with many flower types. If such constraints are the basis for constancy, it should be most pronounced when flowers in a mixture differ strongly in morphology or color. I observed bees foraging in outdoor flower arrays and found that constancy always increased with increasing differences among flower types; similar results can be gleaned from one other study. The available experimental evidence thus suggests that constancy reflects behavioral constraints. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS For comments and ideas I thank W. S . Armbruster, D. Campbell, P. Feinsinger, A. Montalvo, C . Osenberg, and especially my most thoughtful and constant critic, M. Price. She and students from two graduate classes helped in the field, and National Science Foundation grants DEB 81-02774 and BSR 83-13522 provided financial support. FLOWER CONSTANCY LITERATURE CITED Bateman, A. J. 1951. The taxonomic discrimination of bees. Heredity 5:271-278. Bennett. A. W . 1883. On the constancy of insects in their visits to flowers. Zool. J . Linn. Soc. 17: 175-185. Brown, B. A , , and M. T . Clegg. 1984. Influence of flower color polymorphism on genetic transmission in a natural population of the common morning glory. 1pornoc.o prrrprrr.eir. Evolution 38: 796-803. Calder, W. A , , N . M. Waser, S. M. Hiebert. D. W. Inouye, and S. Miller. 1983. Site-fidelity, longevity, and population dynamics of broad-tailed hummingbirds: a ten-year study. Oecologia (Berl.) 56:359-364. Campbell, D. R.. and A. F. Motten. 1985. The mechanism of competition for pollination between two forest herbs. Ecology 66:554-563. Chesson, J . 1983. The estimation and analysis of preference and its relationship to foraging models. Ecology 64: 1297-1304. Christy, R. M. 1883. On the methodic habits of insects when visiting flowers. Zool. J . Linn. Soc. 17:186-195. Clements. F. E . , and F. L. Long. 1923. Experimental pollination: an outline of the ecology of flowers and insects. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Publ. 336. Cowie, R. J . , and J . R. Krebs. 1979. Optimal foraging in patchy environments. Pages 183-205 itz R. M. Anderson. B. D. Turner, and L . R. Taylor, eds. Population dynamics. Blackwell, Oxford. Darwin. C. 1876. On the effects of cross and self fertilisation in the vegetable kingdom. John Murray, London. Dawkins. M. 1971. Perceptual changes in chicks: another look at the "search image" concept. Anim. Behav. 19566-574. Faegri, K . , and L. van der PijI. 1979. The principles of pollination ecology. 3d rev. ed. Pergamon, Oxford. Fisher, R. A , , and T. J. Bailey. 1949. The estimation of linkage with differential viability. Heredity 3:2 15-228. Free, J. B. 1963. The flower constancy of honeybees. J . Anim. Ecol. 32: 119-13 I . -. 1966. The foraging behaviour of bees and its effect on the isolation and speciation of plants. Pages 76-91 irz J . G. Hawkes, ed. Reproductive biology and taxonomy of vascular plants. Pergamon, Oxford. -. 1970rr. Insect pollination of crops. Academic Press. London. ----. 1970b. The flower constancy of bumblebees. J . Anim. Ecol. 39:395-402. Gass, C . L., and R. D. Montgomerie. 1981. Hummingbird foraging behavior: decision-making and energy regulation. Pages 159-194 irl Kamil and Sargent 1981. Goldsmith. T . H . . and K . M. Goldsmith. 1979. Discrimination of colors by the black-chinned humnle.rrrtzdr,i). J . Comp. Physiol. A, Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol., mingbird (A~~clzilochrrs 130:209-220. Gould, J. L. 1985. How bees remember flower shapes. Science (Wash., D . C . ) 227: 1492- 1494. Grace. J., and M. Nelson. 1981. Insects and their pollen loads at a hybrid Her~trclerrr~z site. New Phytol. 87:413-423. Grant, V . 1949. Pollination systems as isolating mechanisms in angiosperms. Evolution 3:82-97. -. 1950. The flower constancy of bees. Bot. Rev. 16:379-398. Heinrich, B. 1976. The foraging specializations of individual bumblebees. Ecol. Monogr. 46: 105-178. -. 19790. Bumblebee economics. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass. ----. 1979b. "Majoring" and "minoring" by foraging bumblebees, Botnhrrs i.tr,ycirzs: an experimental analysis. Ecology 60:245-255. Heinrich, B., P. R. Mudge, and P. G . Deringis. 1977. Laboratory analysis of flower constancy in foraging bumblebees: Botnhrr.c tert~ot.iri.sand B. rerricoln. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2:247-265. Jones, C . E . 1978. Pollinator constancy as a pre-pollination isolating mechanism between sympatric species of Cerc,idirrrn. Evolution 32:199-198. Kamil. A. C . , and T. D. Sargent, eds. 1981. Foraging behavior: ecological, ethological. and psychological approaches. Garland STPM Press. New York. 602 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Kislev, M. E.. Z. Kraviz. and J . Lorch. 1972. A study of hawkmoth pollination by a palynological analysis of the proboscis. Isr. J . Bot. 21:57-75. Kohn, J . R., and N . M. Waser. 1985. The effect of D r l p h i r ~ i r r mnelsonii pollen on seed set in I p o n ~ o p s i s crggregutcr, a competitor for hummingbird pollination. Am. J . Bot. 72: 1144-1 148. Krebs, J . R., A. I. Houston, and E . L. Charnov. 1981. Some recent developments in optimal foraging. Pages 3-18 in Kamil and Sargent 1981. Kugler. H . 1943. Hummeln als Bliitenbesucher. Ergeb. Biol. 19:143-323. Laverty. T. M. 1980. The flower-visiting behaviour of bumblebees: floral complexity and learning. Can. J . Zool. 58:1324-1335. Levin, D. A. 1978. Pollinator behavior and the breeding structure of plant populations. Pages 133-150 in A. J . Richards. ed. The pollination of flowers by insects. Academic Press, London. Levin, D. A , . and W. W. Anderson. 1970. Competition for pollinators between simultaneously flowering species. Am. Nat. 104:455-467. Linsley, E . G . , and J . W . MacSwain. 1958. The significance of floral constancy among bees of the genus Diudasin (Hymenoptera. Anthophoridae). Evolution 12:219-223. MacArthur, R. H., and E. R. Pianka. 1966. On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am. Nat. 100:603-609. Manning, A. 1957. Some evolutionary aspects of the flower constancy of bees. Proc. R. Phys. Soc. Edinb. 25:67-71. McNair, J . N . 1981. A stochastic foraging model with predator training effects. 11. Optimal diets. Theor. Popul. Biol. 19: 147-162. McNaughton, I. H . , and J . L. Harper. 1960. The comparative biology of closely related species living in the same area. 1. External breeding barriers between Pnpcrver species. New Phytol. 59: 15-26. Menzel, R . , J . Erber. and T. Masuhr. 1974. Learning and memory in the honeybee. Pages 195-217 it1 L. Barton Browne, ed. Experimental analysis of insect behaviour. Springer. New York. Pietrewicz, A. T . , and T. D. Kamil. 1981. Search images and the detection of cryptic prey: an operant approach. Pages 31 1-331 in Kamil and Sargent 1981. Plateau, F. 1901. Observations sur le phenomene de la constance chez quelques hymenopteres. Ann. Soc. Entomol. Belg. 45:56-83. Pleasants, J . M. 1981. Bumblebee responses to variation in nectar availability. Ecology 62:1648-1661. Pyke, G . H . 1981. Optimal foraging in nectar-feeding animals and coevolution with their plants. Pages 19-48 in Kamil and Sargent 198 1. Real, L . A. 1981. Uncertainty in plant-pollinator interaction: the foraging behavior of bees and wasps on artificial flowers. Ecology 62:20-26. Schemske, D. W. 1983. Limits to specialization and coevolution in plant-animal mutualisms. Pages 67109 in M. H . Nitecki, ed. Coevolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Stanton. M. L. 1984. Short-term learning and the searching accuracy of egg-laying butterflies. Anim. Behav. 32:33-40. Straw. R. M. 1972. A Markov model for pollinator constancy and competition. Am. Nat. 106:597-620. Stucky, J . M. 1984. Forager attraction by sympatric Ipornoen Ilederrrcen and I. prrrprtrea (Convolvulaceae) and corresponding forager behavior and energetics. Am. J . Bot. 71:1237-1244. Thomson, J . D. 1981. Field measures of flower constancy in bumblebees. Am. Midl. Nat. 105:377-380. Waddington, K. D., and L. R. Holden. 1979. Optimal foraging: on flower selection by bees. Am. Nat. 114: 179-196. Waser, N . M. 1978. Competition for hummingbird pollination and sequential flowering in two Colorado wildflowers. Ecology 59:934-944. 19830. Competition for pollination and floral character differences among sympatric plant species: a review of evidence. Pages 277-293 in C. E . Jones and R. J . Little, eds. Handbook of experimental pollination biology. Scientific and Academic Editions. New York. -. 1983b. The adaptive nature of floral traits: ideas and evidence. Pages 241-285 in L. A. Real, ed. Pollination biology. Academic Press, New York. Weaver, N . 1956. The foraging behavior of honeybees on hairy vetch: foraging methods and learning to forage. lnsectes Soc. 3:537-549. FLOWER CONSTANCY 603 Wells, H . . and P. H. Wells. 1983. Honeybee foraging: optimal diet. minimal uncertainty, o r individual constancy behaviour? J . Anim. Ecol. 521829-836. Wells, H . , P. H . Wells, and D. M. Smith. 1983. Ethological isolation in plants. 1. Colour selection by honeybees. J . Apic. Res. 22:33-44. Yule, G. U . 1912. On the methods of measuring association between two attributes. J. K. Stat. Soc., ser. A 75:579-642.