STRESSED OUT WITH STRESS Perceptual recognition of acoustic correlates of stress in Tagalog Sergey B. Klimenko Maria Paz C. San Juan Jem R. Javier University of the Philippines Unit 308, 137 Matatag St., Central, Quezon City 1101 +63-905-331-8047 Translation Division, The Church of Jesus Christ of Latter-Day Saints; University of the Philippines 9 Linaluz St., San Carlos Heights, Binangonan, Rizal +63-928-503-1712 The Department of Linguistics University of the Philippines 27 Geoffrey St., Gabriel Subd. II, Hulong Duhat, Malabon City 1470 +63-923-295-2012 venom4000@gmail.com venom_2000@mail.ru sanjuanpc@ldschurch.org ABSTRACT Prosodic properties are features of human speech sound that can be assigned to units larger than a segment like syllables and phrases. Earlier studies have established that not all components of the speech signal are perceived to be distinctive by speakerhearers. It is not enough therefore to establish the acoustic correlates of “stress” if the same cannot be so identified through simple hearing. This study aims to establish by experiment and statistical methods the relative contribution of “stress” variables (pitch, length, and intensity) in distinguishing one member of minimal pairs or set from the other. Through GoldWave and PRAAT programs, the variables are modified and sets of synthesized utterances are evaluated by a sample of native speakers. By comparing the results of this experiment with those acoustic measurements the researchers intend to determine with confidence the variable or combination of variables that dictates what has been widely referred to as stress. Keywords Stress, acoustic correlates, pitch, intensity, duration, Tagalog 1. INTRODUCTION Prosodic properties are features of human speech sound that can be assigned to units larger than a segment like syllables and phrases. Also termed suprasegmentals, these prosodic properties are distinct in different languages as much as speech sound patterns (Dobrovolsky and Czaykowska-Higgins, 2001). This paper aims to establish by experiment and statistical methods the relative contribution of “stress” variables (pitch, length, and intensity) in distinguishing one member of minimal pairs or sets from the other. Brichoux (1972: 6) believes that the phonemic contrastiveness of stress placement in Tagalog, if not in many Philippine-type languages, “deals only with a subjective phenomenon based on auditory impressions or native reaction.” This is the reason why he differentiates the two classic definitions of stress as (1) perceived prominence of a syllable within a word, judged by the native speakers; and (2) as Ladefoged (2003: 90) aptly states: “the auditory/acoustic consequences of a syllable jemroquejavier@gmail.com jemroquejavier@yahoo.com having received stress…[which] are likely to be some combination of increased pitch, length, and loudness, with the first two playing the greatest roles.” 2. SOME STRESSFUL STUDIES For many decades now, linguists have still been debating on the nature of stress. Bloomfield (1917) defines this (and he uses the word ‘accent’) as involving greater stress and higher pitch. Furthermore, as he describes the stress of the English language, Bloomfield (1933) distinguishes the stressed syllable as the one being pronounced louder than the other syllables within a word. Pike (1947: 250) agrees with this claim, stating that stress is “a degree of intensity upon some syllable which makes it more prominent or louder than an unstressed syllable.” Nyqvist (1962) says essentially the same thing: that what sets a stressed syllable apart from the unstressed one/s is the greater loudness of the stressed syllable. On the other hand, there are also linguists such as Berger (1955) who suppose that intensity or loudness may not be the main contributor of stress. He suggests that “a sound may be more prominent…by timbre, length, or articulatory energy without being louder in any physical sense.” Fischer-Jørgensen (1961) agrees with Berger and even goes farther by stating that intensity may be a trivial factor in the definition of stress. The claims made may be because the definition of stress in the past had been based on the studies made on the English language. Fischer-Jørgensen (1961: 133) suggests that to really find the acoustic correlates of stress, “it would be of great interest to undertake studies of stress in different languages.” As a reply to Fischer-Jørgensen’s challenge of studying different languages to firmly establish the definition of stress, Brichoux (1972) studies the spectrographic acoustic correlates of stress in Tagalog, i.e., the relative amplitude (or loudness or intensity), relative frequency (or pitch), and relative length (or duration) of the said language. Using a tape recorder and the Sonagraph (trademark and name used by the Kay Elemetrics Company for their sound spectrograph), Brichoux finds out that amplitude is not what signals the stress in Tagalog: “Acoustically speaking, it may very well be that stress will always be found to consist in a number of factors, of which intensity may be one, but certainly not demonstrably the most important, factor. Stress is a universal when all levels of language are considered, but its acoustic parameters may be better described as bundles of ‘features’: intensity, relative frequency, duration, vowel quality (envelopes), or degree and kind of change in one or some of these phenomena.” (Brichoux, 1972: 18-19) Brichoux (1972) also calls for research studies using synthetic speech in order to determine which acoustic correlates are perceived by listeners as stress; this challenge is what stimulates the researchers to undertake this research. Before the publication of Brichoux’s 1972 study on the acoustic correlates of stress in Tagalog, Gonzalez (1970) has already studied the acoustic correlates of accent, rhythm (syllable duration), and intonation (syntactic accent) in the same language. Using Pitch Extractor and Trans-Pitchmeter, computer programs written at the Phonology Laboratory of the University of California in Berkeley, Gonzalez discerns that it is the higher fundamental frequency (pitch), greater amplitude, and longer duration that acoustically signal the accent in Tagalog. However, for Gonzalez (1970: 556), the three acoustic correlates may not always be present all at the same time: “the speaker at times uses only one of three correlates in the actual phonetic realization of his utterances, thus showing the phenomenon of ‘trading effect.’” Another groundbreaking study was made by Tsukenazu Moriguchi in his 1977 study entitled, “Some remarks on Tagalog stress: Physical accents vs. psychological accents.” According to this study, only duration functions as a phoneme. Furthermore, the stressed syllable is “(1) the one preceded by a syllable with short duration, or (2) the syllable in the initial position, if all syllables are equal in duration.” (p. 79) multitrack audio/video mixing software. The company is named after the GoldWave Digital Audio Editor, which was released in April 1993. GoldWave has been used for historic recording analysis in the moon landing, as reported on BBC and the Houston Chronicle. It is used in dozens of schools and universities around the world and is mentioned in numerous research papers covering a wide variety of topics, including neuropsychology, bioacoustic signals, seismic analysis, and engine and animal identification. GoldWave is very accessible and has become popular in the visually impaired community and has applications for the study of hearing impairment and perception. [About GoldWave (GoldWave, 2010)] 3.3 (Par)oxytonic Words Measured The study focuses on the analysis of stress in Tagalog minimal pairs in isolation. The informant (33 years old, native resident of Metro Manila and native speaker of Tagalog) was asked to pronounce in triplets twelve pairs of Tagalog disyllabic words which phonologically differ only in their stress position: (1) aso: ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] ‘dog’ – [Ɂʌ.'sɔ] ‘soot’ (2) balat: ['bʌ.lʌt] ‘birthmark’ – [bʌ.'lʌt] ‘skin’ (3) bukas: ['bʊ.kʌs] ‘tomorrow’ – [bʊ.'kʌs] ‘open’ (4) gabi: ['gʌ.bɪ] ‘taro’ – [gʌ.'bɪ] ‘night’ (5) gutom: ['gʊ.tɔm] ‘hunger’ – [gʊ.'tɔm] ‘hungry’ (6) hamon: ['hʌ.mɔn] ‘challenge’ – [hʌ.'mɔn] ‘ham’ (7) hapon: ['hʌ.pɔn] ‘afternoon’ – [hʌ.'pɔn] ‘Japanese’ (8) pako: ['pʌ.kɔɁ] ‘nail’ – [pʌ.'kɔɁ] ‘species of medicinal plant’ (9) paso: ['pʌ.sɔɁ] ‘burn’ – [pʌ.'sɔɁ] ‘flower pot’ (10) puno: ['pʊ.nɔɁ] ‘tree’ – [pʊ.'nɔɁ] ‘full’ (11) saya: ['sʌ.jʌ] ‘dress’ – [sʌ.'jʌ] ‘happy’ (12) tubo: ['tʊ.bɔ] ‘tube’ – [tʊ.'bɔ] ‘sugar cane’ The pairs of Tagalog disyllabic words which differ in stress placement were taken from Ramos’s 1971 study on Tagalog structures. With the view of measurement and further manipulation only middle members were taken into consideration in every triplet. Duration, pitch, and intensity of both vowels in every word were measured with the help of Praat (see Figure 1). 3. DATA AND ANALYSES 3.1 Praat Praat is computer software developed by Paul Boersma and David Weenink of the Institute of Phonetic Sciences, University of Amsterdam. The program aids the users in investigating a large number of processes such as: (1) making spectrograms; (2) making pitch analyses; (3) looking at how the ear analyzes sounds; (4) synthesizing speech in articulatory terms; (5) using neural nets; and (6) describing phonetic events by means of optimality theory; among others (Ladefoged, 2003: 84). 3.2 GoldWave GoldWave is computer software manufactured by GoldWave Inc. a privately owned, Canadian based corporation. GoldWave offers affordable, professional quality, digital audio editing software and Figure 1: Sound wave and spectrogram of ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] in Praat. The measurements are given in the following table: Table 1. Measurements of duration, pitch, and intensity on the words elicited. WORD DUR1 (ms) DUR2 (ms) PITCH1 (Hz) PITCH2 (Hz) INT1 (dB) INT2 (dB) DUR2 (ms) PITCH1 (Hz) ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] 144 211 [Ɂʌ.'sɔ] 54 218 ['bʌ.lʌt] 161 [bʌ.'lʌt] 169 ms, respectively. The mean values of duration for stressed and unstressed penult vowels are around 127 ms and 49 ms, respectively. Please see graph below: PITCH2 (Hz) INT1 (dB) INT2 (dB) 123 85 83 67 87 100 75 75 117 120 90 88 83 47 125 81 103 77 85 0.12 ['bʊ.kʌs] 106 140 128 110 79 88 0.10 [bʊ.'kʌs] 46 155 82 113 65 85 0.08 ['gʌ.bɪ] 133 264 104 82 83 71 [gʌ.'bɪ] 51 267 89 98 75 78 ['gʊ.tɔm] 108 99 119 105 79 82 [gʊ.'tɔm] 40 91 91 115 66 77 ['hʌ.mɔn] 112 123 136 116 86 77 [hʌ.'mɔn] 67 122 104 127 78 80 ['hʌ.pɔn] 117 84 130 94 86 69 [hʌ.'pɔn] 53 132 96 119 75 77 ['pʌ.kɔɁ] 109 134 110 90 85 76 [pʌ.'kɔɁ] 34 128 85 104 71 78 ['pʌ.sɔɁ] 140 131 134 76 87 71 Intensities in dB of the First and Second Syllables of the 12 Minimal Pairs Density DUR1 (ms) WORD 0.04 0.02 0.00 65 70 75 80 85 90 Data INT1 and INT2, intensities of the first and second syllables of the 12 paroxytonic words INT3 and INT4, intensities of the first and second syllables of the 12 oxytonic words Figure 2. Histogram of intensity. Pitches in Hz of the First and Second Syllables of the 12 Minimal Pairs 0.06 43 145 87 120 70 82 ['pʊ.nɔɁ] 151 144 136 98 77 74 0.04 [pʊ.'nɔɁ] 58 149 85 116 69 79 ['sʌ.jʌ] 133 274 133 78 87 79 [sʌ.'jʌ] 65 291 98 116 78 85 ['tʊ.bɔ] 121 246 140 92 76 72 [tʊ.'bɔ] 34 205 98 105 70 76 Density [pʌ.'sɔɁ] Variable PITCH1 PITCH2 PITCH 3 PITCH 4 Mean StDev N 126.1 11.13 12 93 12.45 12 90.25 7.225 12 111.3 9.089 12 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.00 80 100 120 140 Data PIT1 and PIT2, pitches of the first and second syllables of the 12 paroxytonic words PIT3 and PIT4, pitches of the first and second syllables of the 12 oxytonic words Figure 3. Histogram of pitch. It can be seen from the graphs that the mean of DUR1 is less than DUR2 but DUR1 is more compact and DUR2 more dispersed. The same pattern can be seen from DUR3 and DUR4 graphs. Furthermore, DUR2 and DUR4 appear to be almost identical. These are the patterns that Moriguchi (1977) has hypothesized regarding duration when he talked about the Tagalog stress. See Figure 4. Durations in Milliseconds of the First and Second Syllables Pitch, showing more regularity, is higher on the stressed vowel than on the unstressed one in all words, paroxytonic and oxytonic. The mean difference in pitch between two syllables is about -33 Hz for paroxytonic and about +21 Hz for oxytonic words. See Figure 3 below. of the 12 Minimal Pairs 0.04 Variable DUR1 DUR2 DUR3 DUR4 0.03 Density Duration exhibits somewhat more sophisticated pattern: while length of the ultima vowel in both words in most of the pairs shows considerable degree of uniformity, length of the penult vowel in oxytonic words is significantly shorter than that of the penult vowel in paroxytonic words. The mean values of duration for unstressed and stressed ultima vowels are around 163 ms and Mean StDev N 83 4.221 12 75.75 6.312 12 72.42 4.522 12 79.75 3.646 12 0.06 0.05 Intensity manifests the least clear pattern for paroxytonic and oxytonic words. The ultima vowel has stronger intensity relative to that of the penult vowel in all oxytonic words but [Ɂʌ.'sɔ] in the case of which intensity of both the vowels is the same (75 dB). The penult vowel has stronger intensity relative to that of the ultima vowel in ten out of the twelve paroxytonic words. In ['bʊ.kʌs] and ['gʊ.tɔm] the unstressed vowel is louder than the stressed one (79 db vs. 88 dB, and 79 dB vs. 82 dB, in ['bʊ.kʌs] and ['gʊ.tɔm], respectively). The mean difference in intensity between two syllables is about -9.1 dB for paroxytonic words (exclusive of ['bʊ.kʌs] and ['gʊ.tɔm]), and about +14.8 dB (exclusive of [Ɂʌ.'sɔ]) or +13.5 dB (inclusive of [Ɂʌ.'sɔ]) in oxytonic words. Variable INT1 INT2 INT3 INT4 Mean StDev N 127.9 18.44 12 163.9 66.41 12 49.33 10.81 12 169 62.26 12 0.02 0.01 0.00 50 100 150 200 Data 250 300 DUR1 and DUR2, durations of the first and second syllables of the 12 paroxytonic words DUR3 and DUR4, durations of the first and second syllables of the 12 oxytonic words Figure 4. Histogram of duration. Thus, the initial data reveal the following: (1) there is a correlation between duration and stress in every case, with stressed penult vowel being notably longer than unstressed penult vowel, and unstressed and stressed ultima vowels being of relatively the same duration; (2) there is a strong one-to-one correlation between pitch and stress, with pitch of stressed vowel being higher than that of unstressed one, irrespective of their position in a word; and (3) there is somewhat loose correlation between intensity and stress, with intensity of stressed vowel being acoustically stronger than that of unstressed one, irrespectively of their position, only in nine of 12 pairs; in the remaining three pairs, one of the words does not show any correlation between intensity and stress. To determine by statistical methods if the stress variables (duration, pitch and intensity) manifest significant correlation with stress, the paired T-test in Minitab was employed. Minitab is a “statistics package” developed by Barbara F. Ryan, Thomas A. Ryan, Jr., and Brian L. Joiner of the Pennsylvania State University in 1972 (Meyer and Krueger, 2004). After the statistical tests it can be concluded that stress variables are significant and therefore have important contribution to the perception of stress if the PValue associated with the computed T-Value is equal or less than 0.05. Below are the results of the paired t-tests for duration, pitch and intensity. Table 2. Paired t-test results for duration, pitch and intensity for the 12 paroxytonic words Stress Variable Mean P-Value 0.082 Duration Penult 127.7 ms Ultima 163.9 ms 0.000 Pitch Penult 126.08 Hz Ultima 93.00 Hz 0.009 Intensity Penult 83.00 dB Ultima 75.75 dB . From the P-Values above it can be seen that both pitch and intensity manifest significant correlation with stress. Furthermore, pitch and intensity tend to be higher on the penult, where the stress is, rather than the ultima, for the given set of paroxytonic words. The current data however, do not show enough proof that duration is significant at 0.05. Going back to the raw data it is noticeable that there is no consistent pattern in duration unlike what can be seen in pitch and intensity. However, it is too early to discount the effect of duration in the perception of stress at this point because although duration is not significant at alpha=0.05, it is significant at alpha=0.10. Table 3. Paired T-test results for duration, pitch and intensity for the 12 oxytonic words Stress Variable Mean P-Value Duration Penult Ultima Pitch Penult Ultima Intensity Penult Ultima 0.000 49.3 ms 169.0 ms 0.000 90.25 Hz 111.33 Hz 0.001 72.42 dB 79.75 dB For the 12 oxytonic words, results show that duration, pitch, and intensity very clearly manifest significant correlation with stress. Duration, pitch, and intensity show a strong consistent pattern to be higher in value in the stressed syllable, the ultima. Among the three variables, duration shows the most distinct pattern. Duration on the stressed syllable on the average is 2.42 times more than the unstressed syllable while pitch and intensity is only about 0.26 and 0.11 respectively. Having obtained these facts, the following hypothesis, consisting of three suppositions, can be formulated: (1) stress falls on ultimate vowel if that vowel is preceded by a relatively shorter one and it falls on the penult vowel if that vowel has a duration as long as the ultima; (2) pitch is of secondary significance for stress in Tagalog, or else tends to be higher on stressed vowel, but is not significant for stress (the assumption that being stressed for a Tagalog vowel necessarily means having relatively higher pitch would be implausible, since pitch is operative in intonation patterns formation, and it will differ for the same words pronounced as a question, exclamation, etc., which are not represented in this study); and (3) since there are minimal pairs that do not differ in intensity pattern, stress is of loose dependence on intensity, i.e., intensity may also be of secondary significance just like the pitch. Note that the first part of the hypothesis is in tune with the stand of Moriguchi’s 1977 study on stress in Tagalog. 3.4 Sound Synthesis The The files used in the perception test were prepared with the help of the Praat toolkit and the GoldWave software. For the purpose of sound modification, the researchers took the first word (paroxytonic) of each pair, and adjusted alternately its duration, pitch, and intensity (the former two were manipulated in Praat, while intensity in GoldWave). For each of the stress correlates, the value of the penult vowel in the first (paroxytonic) word was modified to the value of the penult vowel in the second (oxytonic) word. The value of the ultimate vowel in the first (paroxytonic) word was modified to the value of the ultimate vowel in the second (oxytonic) word. While an individual stress correlate is modified, the other two are kept intact. For instance, the word ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] in terms of duration was manipulated in such a way that the length of the penult vowel (144 ms) in the paroxytonic word was changed to 54 ms (duration of the penult vowel of the oxytonic word), and that of the ultimate vowel (211 ms) in the paroxytonic word to 218 ms (duration of the ultimate vowel in the paroxytonic word), while the pitch and intensity of the vowels in this word were left unmodified (Fig. 2 and Fig. 3). By this the researchers intended to single out significance of each separate correlate for stress recognition during the perception test conducted later: if a paroxytonic word with a modified acoustic property is perceived or “heard” as an oxytonic word of the same pair, then this property is to be considered significant for stress. To determine if the acoustic correlate is perceived or heard by the respondents, they must have noticed a change in the meaning of the word. As a result of the data manipulation stage of this study, for each minimal pair three files of every modified paroxytonic word were obtained: one for each of manipulated duration, pitch, and intensity. 21 3 Ilokano 1 Hiligaynon 3 43 1 Kapangpangan 1 Ilokano 1 Total 51 Pangasinan 1 Kapangpangan 1 Tagalog 37 Tagalog 33 Total 51 Total 51 Distribution of the Native Languages Spoken by the 51 Respondents in the Sample Bicol Chabacano English Fookien Hiligaynon Ilokano Kapangpangan Pangasinan Category Bicol Chabacano English Fookien Hiligaynon Ilokano Kapangpangan Pangasinan Tagalog Tagalog Figure 7. Pie graph of the native languages of the respondents. Figure 5. ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] on the pitch and duration manipulation screen of Praat. The answers were tabulated and analyzed using paired T-test in MiniTab. The researchers ran the test in three batches: Batch 1, for all the respondents in the sample (n=51); Batch 2, for those whose native and home language is Tagalog (n=29); and Batch 3, per minimal pair for the respondents who correctly identified both the unaltered sounds. For example, paired T-test was computed for the minimal pair (['bʌ.lʌt] ‘birthmark’ – [bʌ.'lʌt] ‘skin’) using only the responses from the 48 respondents who correctly identified the unaltered pair during the survey, for the pair (['hʌ.mɔn] ‘challenge’ – [hʌ.'mɔn] ‘ham’) 29 respondents and so on. Only the minimal pairs (['Ɂʌ.sɔ] ‘dog’ – [Ɂʌ.'sɔ] ‘soot’) and (['pʌ.sɔɁ] ‘burn’ – [pʌ.'sɔɁ] ‘flower pot’) were correctly identified by all respondents. The summary of the results of the paired T-tests is below: Figure 6. Manipulating intensity using GoldWave. Table 5. Paired T-test results Respondents 3.5 Perceptual experiment Fifty-one students of the University of the Philippines from 34 towns and cities around the archipelago participated in the perceptual experiment. Below are some important details about the respondents: Native Language n= Balat aso hamon bukas gabi Table 4. Profile of the respondents First Language at Home Batch 1 All respondents (n-51) Minimal Pairs Sex n= Age n= n= F 40 16 8 Bicol 2 Bicol 1 M 11 17 11 Chabacano 1 Cebuano 1 Total 51 18 14 English 4 Chabacano 1 19 9 Fookien 1 English 8 20 5 Hilgaynon 3 Fookien 2 gutom tubo hapon Significant Stress Correlates and their P-Values duration* pitch (0.024) duration* pitch (0.024) duration (0.006) pitch (0.002) duration* duration* pitch (0.024) duration* pitch (0.001) duration* intensity (0.010) pitch* duration (0.051) intensity (0.032) pitch (0.007) Respondents Minimal Pairs pako paso puno saya Batch 2: Tagalog as the only mother tongue and language at home (n-29) Balat aso hamon bukas gabi gutom tubo hapon pako paso puno saya Batch 3: Respondents who correctly answered on both minimal pairs balat (n=48) aso (n=51) hamon (n=29) bukas (n=29) gabi (n=29) gutom (n=29) tubo (n=29) hapon (n=47) pako (n=50) paso (n=51) puno (n=50) saya (n=50) Significant Stress Correlates and their P-Values duration* duration* duration* pitch* duration* pitch (0.013) duration* duration* duration (0.032) duration* duration* duration* pitch (0.32) duration* intensity (0.030) pitch (0.023) --duration* duration* duration (0.001) pitch (0.003) duration* pitch (0.023) duration* pitch (0.024) duration* pitch (0.024) duration (0.019) pitch (0.003) duration* duration* pitch (0.044) duration* pitch (0.001) duration* pitch (0.003) duration (0.051) pitch (0.024) duration* duration* duration* pitch* duration* pitch (0.013) * P-Value= 0.000 4. FURTHER INTERPRETATIONS The data obtained by the measurements can be used for building hypotheses on the subject matter, i.e., what acoustic features of vowels are relevant for stress in Tagalog words. In the perception experiment, intensity is significant only for the minimal pairs (['tʊ.bɔ] ‘tube’ – [tʊ.'bɔ] ‘sugar cane’) and (['hʌ.pɔn] ‘afternoon’ – [hʌ.'pɔn] ‘Japanese’) among Batches 1 and 2 but not among Batch 3. It is also interesting to point out that in the perception experiment, the respondents whose native and home language is Tagalog perceived the change in stress less when pitch was changed than do the non-native Tagalog speakers. The native Tagalog speakers collectively did not perceive the change in stress when pitch was changed in the following five minimal pairs: ['bʌ.lʌt] ‘birthmark’ – [bʌ.'lʌt] ‘skin’, ['Ɂʌ.sɔ] ‘dog’ – [Ɂʌ.'sɔ] ‘soot’, ['hʌ.mɔn] ‘challenge’ – [hʌ.'mɔn] ‘ham’, ['gʌ.bɪ] ‘taro’ – [gʌ.'bɪ] ‘night’, and ['hʌ.pɔn] ‘afternoon’ – [hʌ.'pɔn] ‘Japanese’, while the non-native Tagalog respondents did. In the perception experiment, duration was shown to be the most significant among the three variables of stress. This is true for all the minimal pairs except for (['hʌ.pɔn] ‘afternoon’ – [hʌ.'pɔn] ‘Japanese’). Pitch comes in second in significance. This variable is significant in 4 out of 12 minimal pairs as perceived by the 29 respondents whose native and home language is Tagalog (Batch 2) and 9 out of 12 of the minimal pairs as perceived by all of the respondents and by those who correctly recognized the minimal pairs (Batches 1 and 3). In the analysis of the stress components of the minimal pairs and the results of perception experiment duration has shown the most significant contribution in the perception of stress and pitch second. Although statistical tests show all three stress variables are significant, the perception experiment validated only duration and pitch. 5. REFERENCES [1] Berger, M. D. (1955). Vowel distribution and accentual prominence in Modern English. Word 11:361-376. [2] Bloomfield, Leonard. (1917). Tagalog texts. University of Illinois studies in language and literature 3:parts 1, 2, and 3. [3] _________________. (1933). Language. New York: Henry Holt and Company, Inc. [4] Boersma, Paul and David Weenink. (2010). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1.25) [Computer program]. Retrieved 20 January 2010 from the Praat website: http://www.praat.org/ [5] Brichoux, Robert. (1972). Acoustic correlates of stress in Tagalog: spectrographic studies of relative amplitude, relative frequency, and length. Pullerton: California State College. [6] Dobrovolsky and Czaykowska-Higgins. (2001). Phonology: The function and patterning of sounds. In W. O’Grady and J. Archibald (Eds.), Contemporary linguistics An introduction (44th ed.). Boston: Bedford/St. Martin’s. [7] Fischer-Jørgensen, Eli. (1961). What can the new techniques of acoustic phonetics contribute to linguistics? In Sol Saporta (Ed.), Psycholinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. [8] GoldWave. (2010). About GoldWave. Retrieved 28 January 2010 from the GoldWave website: http://www.goldwave.com/about.php [9] Gonzalez, Andrew. (1970). Acoustic correlates of accent, rhythm, and intonation in Tagalog. Phonetica (22):11-44. [10] Ladefoged, Peter. (2003). Phonetic Data Analysis: An Introduction to Fieldwork and Instrumental Techniques. MA: Blackwell Publishing Ltd. [11] Meyer, Ruth K. and David D. Krueger. (2004). A Minitab guide to statistics (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall Publishing. [12] Moriguchi, Tsunekazu. (1977). Some remarks on Tagalog stress: physical accents vs. psychological accents. 東南アジア研究 15(1):79-94. Kyoto University: Departmental Bulletin Paper. [13] Nyqvist, Alvar. (1962). Stress intonation, accent, prominence in disyllabic double stress compounds in educated Southern English. Proceedings of the Fourth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences 710-713. The Hague: Mouton. [14] Pike, Kenneth L. (1947). Phonemics. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. [15] Ramos, Teresita V. (1971). Tagalog structures. In Howard P McKaughan, PALI language texts: Philippines. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.