NEGOTIATING A CHANGE IN THE ARGUMENTATION COURSE: TEACHING COOPERATIVE ARGUMENT Argumentation & Advocacy, Winter 2000, Vol. 36 Issue 3, 105-119 David E. Williams, Associate Professor at Texas Tech University and . Brian R. McGee, Texas Tech University. While less common than was once the case, the argumentation or argumentation and debate course in American colleges and universities is often taught as a scaled-down version of a tutorial in intercollegiate debate. Although textbooks that de-emphasize traditional debate formats have increased in popularity, many instructors still provide students with basic instruction in policy or value debate, where they are introduced to a version of the National Debate Tournament (NDT), Cross Examination Debate Association (CEDA), or parliamentary debate format. Students are then encouraged to compete with each other in debate rounds where a win-loss decision is announced and a grade is issued. This approach to the course seems sensible enough. Intercollegiate debate has a long history of providing valuable experience to student competitors (e.g., Colbert & Biggers, 1985; Colbert, 1987; Matlon & Keele, 1984), and non-forensics students would seemingly benefit from this instruction as well. But the argumentation course would be bolstered by a dual focus on possibilities for argumentation that will de-emphasize the competitive component of the course without seeking to eliminate that component entirely. The competitive element of debate instruction should be balanced with recognition of the need to foster competencies in cooperation, compromise and consensus building that may not be adequately addressed by the traditional debate formats. We will suggest that by including a unit on negotiation, argumentation instructors can provide their students with instruction and activities of clear potential benefit for public-sphere deliberation and professional development. While many of the research, argument construction, and refutation skills used in negotiation have analogues to those required for debate, a negotiation unit emphasizes for students how argumentation is potentially consistent with cooperation and outcomes of mutual benefit. As Gerald I. Nierenberg (1995) states in the introduction to his book, The Art of Negotiating. A competitive spirit is necessary but does not have to be divisive. In fact quite the opposite will happen... instead of creating a rift, each negotiator's competence will enhance the other's, and result in the achievement of a common goal. Competition then becomes a cooperative effort. (viii) This cooperative perspective requires a set of argumentation skills that is not typically emphasized in the traditional argumentation course. The move toward viewing argument as a cooperative enterprise is anything but new and is consistent with more recent challenges to the competitive focus of debate education. Gordon Mitchell (1998) recently criticized the "purely preparatory pedagogy" of current intercollegiate debate practice. His essay suggested that a focus on learning the intricacies of academic debate as a means for preparing students for future activities can be limiting. His call was to move beyond tournament competition and use argumentation in the public sphere (e.g., public debates, debate outreach, and public advocacy). Mitchell noted that "approaches that are purely oriented toward preparation place students and teachers squarely in the proverbial pedagogical bullpen, a peripheral space marked off from the field of social action" (43). Mitchell's concern was with the practice of intercollegiate debate tournaments, but his concern is also applicable to the argumentation course. An exclusive focus on in-class debates can limit students' perception of the versatility of skills they are developing. While the negotiation process might be viewed by some as still competitive, it offers an account of argumentative practice with a more cooperative framework and purpose, where a mutually satisfactory outcome is more likely. If instruction in argumentation is to include a public-sphere component, then the development of cooperative argumentation skills will enhance the ability of participants in public for a to take seriously a variety of perspectives in exchanges with auditors. Although the negotiation exercise by itself does not necessarily take argumentation instruction out of the classroom, it does move the focus beyond formal debate and competition. What follows is a review of the literature dealing with competitive and cooperative approaches to argumentation. The defense of cooperative argumentation serves as the theoretical foundation for incorporating negotiation into the course design. The essay then reviews some primary concepts concerning the role of argumentation in negotiation and explains how to conduct the negotiation unit. COOPERATIVE ARGUMENTATION Deborah Tannen (1998) is one of many scholars who has argued that U.S. public culture has become increasingly strident and adversarial. From this perspective, public argument today is singularly mean-spirited, nasty, and unpleasant, with partisan advocates defending their own positions and denouncing those of their opponents with the zeal of what Hoffer (1942/1963) once called the "true believer." While "meaningful incivility" and the passionate defense of dearly held positions are not without defenders (e.g., Darsey, 1997), the prevailing tendency is to denounce the competitive spirit of vituperative partisan disputes. The complaints about partisanship in the gun control debate following the Littleton, Colorado, tragedy are only one manifestation of this tendency. This critique of contemporary public culture is reminiscent of an earlier debate in communication studies over the Vietnam War-era "rhetoric of confrontation," when rhetoricians struggled to respond to rhetorical forms and argument strategies for which the rhetorical theory of the time seemed inadequate (e.g., Booth, 1974; Haiman, 1967; Windt, 1972). While sometimes sympathetic with the motives of those employing confrontational rhetoric, scholars like Scott and Smith (1969) admitted that "academic rhetorics have been for the most part instruments of established society, presupposing the 'goods' of order, civility, reason, decorum, and civil or theocratic law" (7). In other words, rhetoricians have been (and largely still are) likely to defend decorum and reasonable deliberation-what Matthew Arnold (1932) would have called "sweetness and light"-and to label confrontational tactics as irrational or worse. For example, Windt condemned the radicalism of the anti-war Yippies as no better than "those who conducted the [Vietnam] war" by using napalm against the civilian Vietnamese population (Windt, 1972, 14). Other exemplars of a disciplinary disdain for confrontational strategies include Rothwell's (1971) dislike of verbal obscenity in public discourse and Baskerville's (1963) description of the "far right" as "irresponsible" and the domain of "crackpots." (198-199) A quick review of argumentation and debate textbooks in communication studies published over the past three decades initially suggests that textbook writers have shared the disciplinary preference for cooperative, reasoned deliberation over competitive, confrontational, winner-take-all argumentation. Ziegelmueller, Kay, and Dause (1990) maintain that adhering to certain "fundamental principles of argumentation" will lead to "enlightened understanding and decision-making" (12). Freeley (1990) describes debate as involving "reasoned arguments for and against a given proposition" and leading to "reasoned decisions" (3). While some textbook authors define debate primarily as "competitive advocacy" (Klopf & Cambra, 1979, 3; see Bartanen & Frank, 1991), other textbooks describe debate as also being a cooperative endeavor (e.g., Colburn, 1972; Patterson & Zarefsky, 1983; Thompson, 1971). For example, Patterson and Zarefsky maintain that argument inherently requires the "common goal of making the best decision," since those who are certain they are right would never bother to enter into an argument in the first place (1983, 7; see Brockriede, 1975). Whether the emphasis of the argumentation and debate textbook is on cooperation or competition, a feature shared by almost all such texts is their focus on debate as helping interlocutors to "decide rationally," where to be "rational" means that we must reject "emotional decision[s]" and "personal opinion" in favor of "hard evidence" (Pfan, Thomas, & Ulrich, 1987, 3-4).(n1) While debate presumes some sort of meaningful disagreement, debaters follow an orderly procedure intended to produce an outcome consistent with well-known rules for adjudication. This procedure includes the directive that debaters not commit fallacies of language, evidence, and reasoning, especially the argumentum ad hominem, or attack on the person. In describing the benefits of debate, Freeley (1990) makes reference to the relatively civil examples of U.S. Senators and other government officials, rather than to more confrontational social movement activists. In short, argumentation and debate textbooks appear to suggest a distinct preference for decorousness, where emotion is divorced from reason; disagreement stops short of negative assessments of the character of the opponent; and competition does not become the ultimate goal of the debaters, who must cooperate if their debate is to be productive. The decline in sales of traditional argumentation and debate textbooks that concentrate solely on tournament debating may provide continuing evidence of a general disciplinary preference for reduced emphasis on competition. Of course, not all scholars believe that the traditional argumentation and debate textbook or course should marginalize competition and valorize cooperation. Many students of argumentation during the twentieth century have defended the competitive character of intercollegiate and/or public debate against charges that competition is anti-educational and unproductive, maintaining instead that competition motivates some students to excel who otherwise would not and that competition can be used as an indicator of educational progress (e.g., Simerly & McGee, 1991; Wood & Goodnight, 1990). Some academics are willing to embrace the competitive elements of intercollegiate debate and do not believe that competition is incompatible with the thoughtful examination of diverse arguments. As Baird (1928) responded to critics of competitiveness in academic debate, if "competition in debate will endanger those [liberal] educational values, then the debating game had better be abandoned. But there is no evidence to prove that those educational values are seriously threatened" (25). For Baird, competition was necessary to inspire the "vigorous" and "thorough" preparation of college students for debates, with a resulting improvement in the quality of deliberation. McKean (1934), in another early defense of competitive debate against proponents of cooperative approaches, insisted that some questions do not allow for a resolution that will satisfy everyone. On these questions, he maintained, there must be winners and losers, since a palatable middle position will not exist in those cases where an intelligent person "ought never to compromise, ought never to give way an inch" (McKean, 1934, 234). Further, despite the language of cooperation often employed in some texts, the commitment of many textbook authors to cooperative approaches to argumentation is questionable at best. Gehrke's (1998) review of argumentation and debate texts reveals that such works often frame "argument within competitive and often combative metaphors" (77). For example, Ehninger and Brockriede's (1963) classic text describes testing beliefs by seeing how they withstand "the attacks of an informed opponent," (15) using what Lakoff and Johnson (1980) have called the "argument is war" metaphor. (4) Such metaphors, which are common both in popular discourse and argumentation pedagogy, emphasize competition and marginalize efforts to depict argument as a cooperative, shared search for answers to common problems. The description of argument in violent terms as an activity in which there are winners who do violence and losers who have violence done to them encourages incivility and competition at the expense of cooperation, rather than suggesting that competition and cooperation can coexist in public argument. While civility is not necessary coterminous with cooperation, the war metaphor encourages the association of competition with verbal aggression and hostility. Perhaps the most explicit recent critique in print of the traditional argumentation and debate course's emphasis on competition has come from Josina Makau (1990, 1992a, 1992b, 1996). Relying in part on the claim that patriarchal, Western forms of pedagogy encourage "competition more than cooperation among students" (Wood, 1993, 372; see Harnack, 1951), Makau maintains that traditional argumentation and debate instruction encourages a competitive approach antithetical to critical thinking and reasoned deliberation. As Makau explains, "traditional competitive debate instruction encourages students to view one another as rivals. Students are taught to seek power over one another, to use communication to gain advantage over others" (1996, 139). Because often "competitors' desire to win overshadows their desire to contribute to a just outcome," such competition "sometimes discourages, rather than fosters, reasoned decision making" (1990, 49). Instruction in competitive argumentation typically precludes attempts to find common ground and mutually satisfactory answers to important questions, since competition encourages students to value winning over a productive dialogue. As an alternative, Makau would prefer a classroom experience in which students and instructors eliminated "all traces of competition" (1992a, 9), with students sharing evidence and instructors using "non-competitive grading strategies" (1992b, 82). The cooperative alternative that Makau describes has interlocutors "view opposing advocates as colleagues potentially capable of enlightening them" (1990, 49). Only with the adoption of a cooperative approach would seeking the "best assessments or decisions in any given situation" be possible, since competition interferes with the search for the best decisions (1990, 49). Much about Makau's perspective is commendable. At its worst, competition does intimidate some prospective debaters, dissuading them from engaging in an activity frequently associated with the health and viability of democratic institutions. Her focus on creating a safe classroom space in the argumentation and debate course by avoiding the use of win-loss decisions and urging mutual respect among interlocutors is likely to encourage students to take the enormous risks involved in staking out controversial positions in front of their peers. However, contra Makau, we are not convinced that competition is without value as a unique source of motivation or an indicator of academic progress. It is uncertain that competition devalues "losers" and glorifies "winners" when winning students know they may lose a debate to the same opponent(s) at the next tournament or in-class debate, while losing students realize the next tournament or inclass debate will give them another opportunity to improve. Ideally at least, students should understand that the win-loss decision is a round-specific judgment made by a fallible judge or instructor, rather than some assessment of a student's value as an individual. Further, the frequent willingness of students in competitive debate to make concessions and to come to mutual agreement on key points, especially in crossexamination periods, suggests that sharing power and cooperation are not wholly absent from competitive debate rounds and are compatible with the traditional debate format. When advocates in competitive debates cooperate, as they often do, positing an either-or choice between competition and cooperation seems unsatisfactory. The example of competitive NDT and CEDA debate is illustrative, since a debater who repeatedly fails to cooperate with an opponent's requests for information and/or willfully deceives her or his opponents for competitive gain eventually will earn an unpleasant reputation or academic sanction that, ironically, interferes with future competitive success. In short, we believe that the traditional argumentation course may overemphasize competition, but we are not convinced that the idea of competitive argument should be banished from the classroom. Instead, the traditional debate experience should be complemented by a more cooperative exercise or unit in negotiation. In addressing the controversy in his era over cooperative discussion versus competitive debate events, Nichols (1942) wisely observed that, in a world in which competition and cooperation are often interrelated, training in a variety of argumentation contexts that emphasize competition and cooperation in varying ways and degrees is desirable. In a culture that often values competition over cooperation, the very real dangers of an excessive emphasis on competition as described by Makau justify providing more opportunities for students in argumentation and debate classes to develop cooperative argumentation skills. Exercises in negotiation are well suited to developing such skills. The theoretical justification for a negotiation exercise is hardly new. Older argumentation and debate texts frequently included extensive development of "discussion," a type of argumentation that sometimes was distinguished from debate and was typically characterized as the cooperative search for consensus or, more modestly, a livable compromise (see, e.g., Gulley, 1942). As described by Ehninger and Brockriede (1963), discussion is an "intelligent, purposeful interchange of ideas carried on in a conversational pattern," where "the atmosphere is informal and permissive" and a final decision is made by the interlocutors themselves, as opposed to decisions following debates that are made by a third party or parties (9). Negotiation, a specific form of discussion, employs many argumentation strategies that are shared with formal debate, and negotiation, if it is to satisfy all participants, ultimately must have a cooperative purpose, since the mutual gain of all participants is the optimal outcome of negotiation. A negotiation exercise, therefore, can be an important vehicle for encouraging students to develop cooperative argumentation skills, as explained below. According to Trompson's (1944) description of discussion over five decades ago, discussion is an approach to "thinking cooperatively toward the solution of a common problem," (289) and the negotiation exercise seems consistent with this spirit.(n2) Incorporating negotiation into the argumentation and debate course design begins with a review of the role argumentation plays in negotiation. ARGUMENTATION IN NEGOTIATION To illustrate the distinction between competitive and cooperative argumentation where negotiation is concerned, Walton and McKersie's (1965) typology of negotiation is employed below. Walton and McKersie identified four types of negotiation: distributive, integrative, attitudinal, and intraorganizational bargaining. Distributive negotiation is characterized by fixed sum alternatives. The distributive approach forces opposing negotiators into a competitive situation where one must win and the other must lose. Putnam and Jones (1982b) have warned that negotiators using a distributive approach can perceive a situation as win-lose when a win-win outcome is actually possible. In contrast, integrative negotiation allows for outcomes benefiting both parties. Putnam and Jones (1982b) noted that this negotiation style allows for the possibility of goal alteration, the expansion of outcomes, and sacrifice of some goals. Additionally, attitudinal structuring and intraorganizational bargaining are both described by Donohue (1981) as "more cooperative since the interaction generally may not be constrained initially by fixed potential outcomes" (273). Attitudinal structuring is negotiation that seeking to alter the relational patterns between the involved parties. Turner (1990) explains "intraorganizational bargaining" by noting "the negotiator must obtain consensus with the other party" (54). Therefore, intraorganizational bargaining seeks internal consensus among the parties involved before weighing external factors and publics. Negotiation has not always been perceived as a cooperative process. Rieke and Sillars (1984) claimed that "in negotiation, the parties are committed to reaching a decision, but will prefer no decision to one that grants them less than what they consider minimally acceptable" (41). They choose to focus on the change-seeking function of argument in negotiation without recognition of how it can include cooperation. In contrast, Crusius and Channell (1994) suggest that "negotiators must let go of the whole notion of proving one side right and the other side wrong" (155). In further defense of a cooperative perspective on negotiation, they specify that "arguing to negotiate aims to resolve-or at least reduce-conflict to the mutual satisfaction of all parties involved" (153). The instructional use of negotiation as a means for teaching cooperative argument would dictate a preference for Walton and McKersie's (1965) latter three forms of negotiation, and in the remainder of this essay we assume the instructor adopts this orientation. Seven issues will be addressed that account for the most relevant concerns in negotiation argumentation. We begin with four concepts (research, rules, trust, and deductive argument) more closely related to standard argumentation course content. Three more concepts are then offered ("know thyself", expectations, and "bids and concessions") less typical of the standard argumentation and debate course. Traditional Concepts in the Argumentation Course Research is certainly an essential element of preparation in traditional argumentation instruction. Research also plays an important role in negotiation. However, the practice and purpose of research is somewhat different in negotiation. The primary difference between negotiation and debate research is that negotiation research is more focused on the other negotiator. This practice supplements the traditional gathering of facts and issue development with information on ones' personally held beliefs, desires, and needs. Traditional argumentation instruction does not give as much credence to the arguer. This can help foster identification with the other negotiator, although it can also be used competitively. "There is a positive reason for amassing information. It makes a wealth of material in your mind so that you may take advantage of any new development in the negotiation" (Nierenberg, 1995, 48). Researchers are often advised to explore the past history, previous transactions, and business ventures of the negotiator they will face. Extensive background knowledge will facilitate understanding and identification with the other party. Preparation for negotiation also necessitates that students learn to review the rules of each individual negotiation session. Nierenberg (1995) suggests seven basic questions that must be asked: 1. Are there any penalties for bluffing or providing false information? 2. Are you aware of all interested parties in the negotiation? 3. Any time limit? 4. 5. 6. 7. Do you want to maintain or change the status quo? What is the cost of a stalemate? What means of communication will be used? Can many items be addressed in the negotiation at once? This process could be roughly correlated to students learning the procedural obligations of evidence-oriented or parliamentary debate formats. In the latter case, there is specific static knowledge the student must learn (e.g. topicality, stock issues, format time limits) in order to participate in the debate exercise. In negotiation, those rules may vary from situation to situation. Therefore, it is essential that students learn the practice of reviewing the rules for each negotiation. While traditional argumentation and debate instruction may not address trust specifically, related concepts of ethics and honesty are standard topics for the argumentation course (e.g., Hollihan & Baaske, 1998; Inch & Warnick, 1998). In negotiation, the issue of trust is of paramount importance. Trust has (not surprisingly) been correlated with positive outcomes in negotiation. Turner (1990) notes research showing "that as the negotiator feels more trusted by constituents, the negotiator feels less tension, he or she has more flexibility in the negotiations, and feels less of a need to defend his or her own actions to constituents" (61). He concluded that "low trust from constituents leads to disruptive behavior or a 'win-lose' approach that may interfere with high joint gains" (61). Instructors likely will have lectures prepared on ethical responsibility in communication, given that adherence to such standards is a prerequisite to successful, cooperative negotiation. That topic should be referenced when discussing negotiation and trust. Political adversaries in the United States Senate and House of Representatives can usually be offered as examples of the difficulty arising from a lack of trust. For example, the impeachment proceeding involving President Clinton was a difficult process because of the lack of trust exhibited by members of each political party toward each other. Facilitating trust also necessitates that instructors convey the pitfalls of coercion in negotiation. Putnam and Jones (1982b) remind us that threats, promises, offers and concessions are basic argument approaches that can greatly affect the process and outcome of negotiation. The authors' review of literature found that threats should be treated as a risky negotiation strategy. Threats can increase conflict (e.g., Guyer & Rapoport, 1970), and the threatener often has to follow with messages demonstrating a willingness and ability to follow-up on the threat. Threats also typically lead to additional coercive strategies, whereas promises induce greater compliance from negotiators. The use of coercion also clearly hinders the cooperative process and would likely foster a competitive attitude among participants. While coercion must be avoided, deductive reasoning maintains its traditional importance as a form of argument where negotiation is concerned. Mary Ann Renz (1987) offered an analysis of argumentative form and negotiation strategy in three United Nations Security Council debates. Her research provided insight into the role of causal correlation and deduction in real negotiations. She found that causal correlation was virtually ignored by the negotiators with only six of 625 arguments using that form. (Admittedly, this finding could certainly be a function of the type of negotiation and the participants.) If Renz's research can be generalized, then argumentation instructors will see similarities between in-class debates and negotiation. Deduction was the most prevalent form of argumentation used by the UN negotiators. Deduction was seen to increase as a negotiator was put on the defensive and reliance on deduction would be limited as the defensive position was perceived as less threatening. Renz's research reinforces the obvious relevance of the traditional argumentation analysis skills (deduction, fallacy, recognition, etc.) taught in argumentation and debate courses to the practice of negotiation. This further validates the view that the argumentation course provides professionally transferable skills and orientations further enhanced through the negotiation exercise. Non-Traditional Concepts in Teaching Negotiation Teaching the negotiation unit will require instructors to expand their class preparations. The concepts of self-evaluation ("knowthyself'), expectations, and bids and concessions should be addressed before students are asked to practice their negotiation skills. Nierenberg (1995) offers a suggestion that is frequently overlooked by those whose work focuses exclusively on the negotiation process. We are reminded, however, that preparation is essential and negotiators must begin by adhering to the dictum, "know thyself:" A long-range preparation for negotiation must begin with a form of self-evaluation. It involves an intimate examination of your sense of values, your philosophy of life, it means, in a sense, taking stock of your intellectual and emotional makeup. (46) The "know thyself" process is further described as self-reflective, with questions as general as "what do you want out of life?" and as specific as "can you be easily goaded into anger?" Donohue (1978) suggests that the most relevant personality traits in negotiation include: perception of situational cues, enactment of power strategies, and perception and response to role requirements. These elements should be addressed during this reflective process. Ideally, this process could take place after students have participated in some level of traditional in-class debate exercises but before the negotiation exercises. The students would be equipped with recent experiences in argumentation to judge how they would react to some of the issues mentioned above. Peer evaluations from classmates might also assist in this process. Assessing expectations is another preparatory measure not usually addressed in advance of traditional debate exercises, but it would be added in the negotiation unit. According to Donohue (1978) an evaluation of negotiation strategy might be contextually bound. He offers the maxim, "as the coordination of expectations increases, intense, dysfunctional conflict arising from situational incompatibilities, decreases" (250). Here, Schelling's (1960) early advice is sensible: Negotiators should coordinate their expectations by learning what the other will accept during the process and as an outcome. If desired, instructors could describe this process as a form of audience analysis able to facilitate cooperation between interlocutors. Whereas traditional debate will place students in a position to view the outcome in a win-loss situation, negotiation allows for the possibility of a range of mutually satisfactory outcomes. Nierenberg (1995) warns that assessing expectations must be an active process designed to avoid faulty assumptions. He claims assumptions are a vital part of the negotiation process, and one is severely limited if her or his own assumptions are not reviewed nor the others' anticipated. Hidden assumptions are of greatest concern as they can alter the direction and strategy of negotiation in unproductive ways, since such assumptions could create misunderstanding and frustration that make resorting to competition more likely. When given material for the negotiation exercise, students will need to learn to determine what assumptions they develop and how to critically evaluate those assumptions. They also need to view the issue from the opponents' viewpoint to determine what assumptions would be likely. The proper use of bids and concessions comprises the final essential component of the negotiation unit. Initial offers (bids) are an argumentation skill unique to negotiation. Negotiating parties have critical decisions to make prior to the start of the session. The bid will include the negotiators' initial suggestion for the outcome of the negotiation. The strength and tone of the bid can vary greatly and convey widely differing messages to the "opposing side." The negotiator does not simply advocate a position; he or she demonstrates the importance of that position, and the degree to which the "opponent" will or should be effected by the position, and the initial likelihood of compromise. Rubin and Brown (1975) found that extreme initial demands convey to opponents a sense of value about the proposal and a desire to negotiate without fear of confrontation. An extreme bid delivered in a cooperative tone yielded trust, while a competitive tone created distrust. In judging negotiation outcomes, Rubin and Brown's work suggests that stronger initial bids (larger requests) receive more satisfactory settlements than moderate bids. The bid differs from the traditional debate exercise because it avoids the win-loss dichotomy. The bid can be changed throughout the negotiation in order to facilitate a mutually agreeable decision. In traditional debate concession occurs primarily when a point is either lost or deemed unimportant. Concession during the negotiation process is another essential skill to be developed by the student. Putnam and Jones (1982b) noted that "concessions also signaled cooperativeness. Negotiators who made positive concessions elicited more cooperation from opponents and more reciprocal concessions than did bargainers who made negative or no concessions" (271). However, Putnam and Jones (1982b) believed that concessions will have less impact on negotiation outcome than strength of initial bid and the pressure to reach an agreement. In a study of labor-management negotiations, Putnam and Jones (1982a) found that "a negotiator's role had a profound effect on the frequency and the structure of bargaining talk in that negotiators specialized in select strategies" (190). They found that labor representatives took an offensive position and used more threats and rejections, while management used defensive tactics such as commitments and self-supporting statements. These offensive-defensive positions became symmetrical, with each side responding from its own unchanging position. By contrast, when cooperative gestures were offered, they were generally reciprocated. Based on this evidence, teaching negotiation skills may provide a unique resource for encouraging cooperative argumentation. The negotiation unit can also give the instructor an opportunity to incorporate other persuasion theory into the argumentation course. Donohue (1981) suggests that assertiveness instruction and questioning strategies would be beneficial additions to the class lectures on negotiation. Crusius and Channell (1994) indicate that interpersonal concepts can be easily added to the argumentation course: 'Just like other aims of argument, arguing to negotiate requires sound logic and clear presentation of positions and reasons. However, negotiation challenges our interpersonal skills more than do the other aims" (7). While cross-examination lectures and reference material will have already addressed the questioning strategy, assertiveness instruction could be added. Essentially, the instructor is encouraged to be creative in determining what to add to this unit. While this list of strategies available to negotiators is not exhaustive, it does give argumentation instructors a starting point for the negotiation unit. The actual implementation of a negotiation unit in a debate course inevitably raises certain practical issues which we will now explicitly address. CONDUCTING THE NEGOTIATION UNIT The negotiation unit should be scheduled near the end of the semester after in-class debates, if such debates are a part of the curriculum. As noted previously, much of the instruction for traditional debate is transferable to negotiation. Therefore, students will be able to utilize some debate skills further while learning about a new argumentation arena. Instructors should initially consider devoting two or three class meetings to lecture and discussion about negotiation. The concepts introduced here might provide the beginning structure and resources for those lectures. Many of the lecture items (e.g., research) will complement, but not necessarily duplicate, what students have already learned. Following a general lecture/discussion of negotiation preparation and argument strategy, the instructor should provide a means for practicing negotiation. The most direct means for doing this is to develop a few hypothetical scenarios dealing with labor-management negotiations or negotiations in alternative dispute resolution sessions where students are cast in the role of worker or owner. In the case of labor-management negotiations, the instructor can provide workers with a description of their situation including a pay rate, work load description, work place description, benefits package, etc. Likewise, managers can be provided with the same information plus a list of organizational facts including, projected growth, company earnings, physical plant status and plans for future development. We suggest that each side be provided at least eight items that describe their situation. In dyads or groups, the instructor can arrange for mock negotiations to take place. A couple of abbreviated examples of hypothetical scenarios are provided below: Campus Bookstore Dispute The campus bookstore has an owner and full-time management. Most other employees are local college students. The workers are growing increasingly distressed over their working conditions and would like to see some concessions made by the owner and management to improve working conditions. The owner and management are generally happy with the way the bookstore has been running and the worker distress surprises them. CAMPUS BOOKSTORE OWNER/MANAGERS 1. There is no competition for book sales. 2. The owner and managers have no growth plans. 3. After all bills and salaries are paid, the owner clears $4,000 per month. 4. There has been no change in the store for 15 years. 5. Earnings are stable, but there is constant fear that new competition will emerge. 6. The owner and managers are comfortable with the status quo aside from fear of future competition. 7. Most employees are short term. 8. The owner does not plan to own the business for more than 10 more years. WORKERS 1. Workers are paid $5.75 per hour to start. 2. No remodeling of building done in 15 years. 3. Workers think the business is understaffed. 4. Worker duties include: stock, place orders, sales, customer questions/relations, clean up, and inventory. 5. No insurance plan is offered but workers do receive a reduced rate on purchases at the store. 6. There is no opportunity to provide input to management/owner. 7. Customers blame workers for things they can not control. 8. Managers frequently are gone during peak hours. Halloween Party Dispute On Halloween weekend, the university becomes a gathering place for university and nonuniversity student party-goers. The event is becoming a "tradition" and the administration is not in favor of this development. The party begins in local taverns and then spills into the streets. There are frequent arrests and minor disturbances. The students generally favor this party and do not see the harm that the administration sees. CAMPUS ADMINISTRATION 1. The party is five years old. 2. It creates a negative reputation for the school in the surrounding community. 3. Sometimes arrests are made. 4. Each year there are injuries. 5. The town has a population of about 30,000 people. 6. The city government does not like the party. 7. Each year thousands of students from other schools come to the party. 8. The university administration fears the party will grow in size each year. 9. Alumni do not like the party. STUDENT GROUP 1. Students generally like the party. 2. They view it as a tradition. 3. Only a minority (10%) of students disapprove of the party. 4. The student population is 10,000. 5. They are willing to negotiate in order to keep the party. 6. They see the party as a disturbance, nothing worse. 7. Student Government and student media want to keep the parties 8. Most students are not concerned about alumni views; current students think it should not matter to alumni as the party was not around when most of the alumni were in school. Hypothetical negotiation scenarios like these are designed to allow students to practice the argumentation strategies they have just learned. It should be emphasized that they need to focus on learning how to use argumentation as a cooperative skill. Because of the hypothetical nature of this exercise, much of the preparation element could not be practiced. Following the negotiations, students should go through a debriefing where they discuss their operating assumptions and the degree of trust and cooperation they witnessed in their opponents. It would be beneficial to videotape these sessions to allow for student observation of their performance. This could be a graded exercise, but we suggest that it not be weighted as heavily as the next exercise. In the next phase of the negotiation unit, the instructor identifies a campus or local (or larger) event that could reasonably be addressed by negotiation. The ideal event might be difficult to discover, should be one that includes at least several viable issues on which informed people might disagree, adequate and accessible background information, and at least one individual from each side who has received some public attention or is wellknown. It is important that students be able to ascertain quickly the primary issues involved in the dispute, the developing arguments on each side and some insight (if possible) into the personalities of the primary actors. This data gathering process will be easier for students if the event and the primary players have garnered some media attention. The instructor should write a negotiation scenario based on the event and assign students to negotiate for either side. The instructor should provide students with some initial information, such as a few newspaper articles, as a starting point for their research efforts. The students should then be assigned the task of researching and preparing for negotiations. Students can prepare self-report evaluations of themselves and catalogue information they have gathered. The negotiations can be performed in class or in written form as a test. If the negotiations are performed, negotiators can come to agreement on the rules for the session. They should then proceed to initiate bids and the negotiation process using the research on oneself, the other negotiator(s), and the facts of the case. Again, it should be emphasized to students that they will be evaluated on their apparent preparation and utilization of strategies leading to an acceptable negotiated settlement. These sessions should also be videotaped for student review. In the written version of the exercise, students will need to reveal what they discovered about the people and issues involved, the likely approach the opponent would take in negotiation, and their likely preferences for how the negotiation should take place. The students would then offer their initial bids and describe the strategies they would plan to use in the negotiations with an accompanying rationale for their use. (A sample negotiation scenario is provided in Appendix 1.) One of the current authors has used this option for a final exam, with positive results. The students knew the form that the test would take but expressed some initial uncertainty about expressing their bids and arguments in written form. Most students needed about ten to fifteen minutes to process the scenario and formulate their thoughts. Most then were able to proceed with the exercise and produce their written responses. Evaluative comments noted that they enjoyed the opportunity to be reflective about their argumentation and consider the personalities of the people involved as well as the issues at stake. Trial and error with this activity have also allowed the author to identify a few of the most likely difficulties that might arise. In the performed version of the exercise, anxiety and newness of the exercise can leave some students with mental blocks during their performance. It can be helpful for students to develop a modified debate brief for the negotiation exercise. This can serve the purpose of a flow or notes but more importantly it can serve as a reference for the student about to offer a bid or argue a concession. In the written version of the exercise, there will be a tendency among some students to write in detail about the argument to be made in the negotiation to the exclusion of why those arguments are being made. In the written exercise, the instructor will need to know why the students are offering specific arguments. It will be quite difficult to evaluate a students use of assumptions, for example, if the answer does not explain the assumption. This is easily avoided with clear directions to the student before administering the scenario. Students might have difficulty in researching individuals for the scenario. Instructors will likely need to provide boundaries for this research. For example, key individuals will likely become annoyed by 25 student requests for interviews on a class project. The instructor should provide guidance on other means of discerning information about local individuals that can be useful, but not intrusive. Finally, grading of this activity might need some elaboration. The prepared brief (if used) on the relevant issues can be incorporated into the grading scheme although the majority of the grade should be based on the oral presentation. A distinction can be made between the creation of briefs and the actual blending of that information into the negotiation session in an effective manner. Creating the brief might move the student toward a "C" grade but no higher if they are not used or simply read verbatim. The more extemporaneous use of the briefs such that they are only used as reference can move the grade higher. Students will learn more from the oral performance of the activity if they view the videotape as a class, with instructor commentary and debriefing, and then receive a detailed written evaluation from the instructor. Admittedly, it will be difficult to grade some elements such as trust and cooperation. These issues should be discussed during the debriefing allowing students to compare insights without the consequence of grade reduction. Trust and cooperation should only affect the grade if obvious deceit or trickery is evidenced by the instructor. This essay has sought to offer instructors of argumentation and debate courses a means for teaching professionally relevant and applicable skills beyond those taught in formal debates. Negotiation is offered as a practice that is based in argumentation, but seeks a cooperative practice and outcome. The practice of negotiation both serves as a good compliment to traditional debate instruction and helps demonstrate the transferability of argumentation skills to more informal situations in which consensus or compromise is the primary objective for the interlocutors. The negotiation unit also allows for instructor creativity with regard to persuasion, interpersonal, and psychological concepts that could be added to the instruction. The use of negotiation in the argumentation and debate classroom is one means of introducing students to the concept of cooperative argumentation. This theoretical and practical addition to the course will help students as they enter business and civic situations that require a negotiated settlement. The skills developed in a traditional course format would be complemented with the cooperative perspective's focus on mutually agreed conclusions and benefiting from the other's knowledge and expertise. As our culture is criticized as become overly adversarial, the cooperative perspective can teach students how to reach compromise with less conflict. This unit is not meant to replace inclass debates. However, negotiation does provide a useful extension and application of the skills that are learned during in-class debates. (n1) Not everyone agrees that reasonable, rational argumentation would exclude reliance on emotion. See Garrett (1993), Makau (1990b, 1992), and McGee (1998). (n2) The history of "discussion" is beyond the scope of this essay. Several scholars defended discussion at one time as a necessary prerequisite to debate, while others described discussion as a laboratory exercise or competitive event of equal or greater pedagogical value when compared to debate. For whatever the reason, interest in the comparison of discussion and debate appears to have declined in the 1960s and 1970s, with discussion eventually mentioned more often by small group communication scholars than by students of argumentation. On discussion and debate, see Baird (1928), Ehninger and Brockriede (1963), McKean (1934), Nichols (1942), Shepard (1955), Thompson (1979), and Thonssen (1939). REFERENCES Arnold, M. (1932). Culture and anarchy (J. D. Wilson, Ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Baird, A. C. (1928). Public discussion and debate. Boston: Ginn & Co. Bartanen, M. D., & Frank, D. A. (1991). Debating values. Scottsdale, AZ: Gorsuch Scarisbrick. Baskerville, B. (1963). The cross and the flag: Evangelists of the far right. Western Speech, 27, 197-206. Booth, W. C. (1974). Modern dogma and the rhetoric of assent. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Brockriede, W. (1975). Where is argument? Journal of the American Forensic Association, 11, 179-182. Colbert, K. (1987). The effects of CEDA and NDT debate on critical thinking. Journal of the American Forensic Association, 23, 194-201. Colbert, K., & Biggers, T. (1985). Why should we support debate? Journal of the American Forensic Association, 21, 237-240. Colburn, C. W. (1972). Strategies for educational debate. Boston: Holbrook. Crusius, T. W., & Channell, C. E. (1995). The aims of argument: A brief rhetoric. Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing. Darsey, J. (1997). The prophetic tradition and radical rhetoric in America. New York: New York University Press. Donohue, W. A. (1981). Analyzing negotiation tactics: Development of a negotiation interact system. Human Communication Research, 7, 273-287. Donohue, W. A. (1978). An empirical framework for examining negotiation processes and outcomes. Communication Monographs, 45, 249-257. Ehninger, D., & Brockriede, W. (1963). Decision by debate. New York: Dodd, Mead. Freeley, A.J. (1990). Argumentation and debate: Critical thinking for reasoned decision making (7th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Garrett, M. M. (1993). Pathos reconsidered from the perspective of classical Chinese rhetorical theories. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 79, 19-39. Gehrke, P.J. (1998). Teaching argumentation existentially: Argumentation pedagogy and theories of rhetoric as epistemic. Argumentation and Advocacy, 30, 76-86. Gulley, H. (1942). Debate versus discussion. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 28, 305-307. Guyer, M., & Rapoport, A. A. (1970). Threat in a two-person game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, 11-25. Haiman, F. S. (1967). The rhetoric of the streets: Some legal and ethical considerations. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 53, 99-114. Harnack, R. V. (1951). Competition and cooperation. Central States Speech Journal, 3(1), 15-20. Hoffer, R. (1963). The true believer: Thoughts on the nature of mass movements. New York: Time. (Original work published 1942) Hollihan, T. A., & Baaske, K. T. (1998). Arguments and arguing.' The products and process of human decision making. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press, Inc. Inch, E. S., & Warnick, B. (1997). Critical thinking in communication: The use of reason in argument (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Klopf, D. W., & Cambra, R. E. (1979). Academic debate: Practicing argumentative theory (2nd ed.). Denver, CO: Morton. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Makau,J. M. (1996). Notes on communication education and social justice. Communication Studies, 47, 135-141. Makau, J. M. (1992a). Raising consciousness in the classroom. Paper presented at the Second National Communication Ethics Conference, Kalamazoo, MI. Makau, J. M. (1992b). Revisioning the argumentation course. Women's Studies in Communication, 15, 79-91. Makau, J. M. (1990). Reasoning and communication: Thinking critically about arguments. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Matlon, R. J., & Keele, L. M. (1984). A survey of participants in the National Debate Tournament, 1947-1980. Journal of the American Forensic Association, 20, 194-205. McGee, B. R. (1998). Rehabilitating emotion: The troublesome case of the Ku Klux Klan. Argumentation and Advocacy, 34, 173-188. McKean, D. D. (1934). Debate or conference? Quarterly Journal of Speech, 20, 222-236. Mitchell, G. R. (1998). Pedagogical possibilities for argumentative agency in academ.ic debate. Argumentation and Advocacy, 35, 41-60. Nichols, A. (1942). The discussion-debate duality. Southern Speech Bulletin, 7, 100-102. Nierenberg, G. I. (1995). The art of negotiating. New York: Barnes & Noble. Patterson, J. W., & Zarefsky, D. (1982). Contemporary debate. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Pfau, M., Thomas, D. A., & Ulrich, W. (1987). Debate and argument: A systems approach to advocacy. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman. Putnam, L. L., & Jones, T. S. (1982a). Reciprocity in negotiations: An analysis of bargaining interaction. Communication Monographs, 49, 171-191. Putnam, L. L., & Jones, T. S. (1982b). The role of communication in bargaining. Human Communication Research, 8, 262-280. Renz, M. A. (1987). Argumentative form and negotiating strategy in three United Nations Security Council debates. Central States Speech Journal, 38, 166-180. Rieke, R. D., & Sillaxs, M. O. (1984). Argumentation and the decisionmaking process (2nd ed.). Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman & Co. Rothwell, j. D. (1971). Verbal obscenity: Time for second thoughts. Western Speech, 35, 231-242. Rubin, J., & Brown, B. (1975). The social psychology of bargaining and negotiation. New York: Academic Press. Schelling, T. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press Scott, R. L., & Smith, D. K. (1969). The rhetoric of confrontation. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 55, 1-8. Shepard, D. W. (1955). Some observations on high school discussion. Speech Teacher, 4, 191-195. Simerly, G., & McGee, B. R. (1991). A conceptual schema for assessing the educational function of a forensics program. Speech and Theatre Association of Missouri Journal, 21, 5-14. Tannen, D. (1998). The argument culture: Moving from debate to dialogue. New York: Random House. Thompson, W. N. (1979). The early history of the National Contest in Public Discussion. Communication Education, 28, 104-109. Thompson, W. N. (1971). Modern argumentation and debate: Principles and practices. New York: Harper & Row. Thompson, W. N. (1944). Discussion and debate: A re-examination. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 30, 288299. Thonssen, L. (1939). The social values of discussion and debate. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 25, 113-117. Turner, D. B. (1990). Intraorganizational bargaining: The effect of goal congruence and trust on negotiator strategy use. Communication Studies, 41, 54-75. Walton, R. E., & McKersie, R. B. (1965). A behavioral theory of labor negotiations: An analysis of a social interaction system. New York: McGraw-Hill. Windt, T. O., Jr. (1972). The diatribe: Last resort for protest. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 58, 1-14. Wood, J. T. (1993). Diversity & commonality: Sustaining their tension in communication courses. Western Journal of Communication, 57, 367-380. Wood, R. V., & Goodnight, L. (1990). Strategic debate (4th ed.). Lincolnwood, IL: National Textbook. Ziegelmueller, G., Kay, J., & Danse, C. (1990). Argumentation: Inquiry and advocacy (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. APPENDIX 1 NEGOTIATION SCENARIO O'HAIR SHUTTERS You will be negotiating for either "All Neighbors United" or O'Hair Shutters. You choose. O'Hair is a shutter manufacturing company located in Lubbock, Texas. Negotiations (hypothetically) will take place between the company and a group known as "All Neighbors United, Inc." All Neighbors United developed when O'Hair announced that it would be using an incinerator to burn its wood debris. O'Hair had previously used other means of disposal for the wood debris including recycling and allowing a particle board company to haul away the material. However, O'Hair determined that using a particle trench burner would be the most efficient means of disposing of the waster. In March 1996 O'Hair put a legal notice in the Avalanche Journal newspaper indicating its plan to use the burner. The Texas Natural Resource Commission (TNRC) granted O'Hair a temporary burning permit to operate the incinerator on a limited basis. All Neighbors United quickly developed in protest to the planned use of the incinerator. The group claimed that the burner would put dangerous chemicals in the air that could cause breathing problems for area citizens and children who attend a nearby elementary school. The group also claimed that O'Hair could return to recycling the debris and that would be a better option that burning. All Neighbors United, in April, filed a lawsuit against the TNRC in response to the temporary permit that was granted to O'Hair. In May the group tried, unsuccessfully, to get the Lubbock Zoning Board of Adjustment to overturn a decision that granted O'Hair the opportunity to use the incinerator. You are to assume that the negotiation is to take place in late May. You should tell me which side you will be negotiating for and then explain all that you have done to prepare for the negotiation and which strategies you would employ in the negotiation and why. Good Luck. [This scenario was accompanied by eight newspaper articles about the O'Hair situation.]