Clothing Running Head: CLOTHING AND APPEARANCE Express yourself: Manifestations of personality in clothing and appearance Laura P. Naumann The University of Texas at Austin 1 Clothing 2 Express yourself: Manifestations of personality in clothing and appearance People form initial impressions of others on the basis of their physical attributes and observable behaviors. Physical appearance is one domain that people frequently use to formulate impressions of others. Physically attractive people are helped, trusted, and preferred as employees (Bardack & McAndrew, 1985) or mates (Buss, 1995) to a greater degree than less attractive people. Additionally, individuals may consciously or unconsciously manifest aspects of their personality through their physical appearance. For instance, one argument against forcing children to wear school uniforms is that uniforms suppress their freedom of expression and individuality. Transgendered peoples’ use of clothing to express their gender-identity is another example. Despite these initial research findings, the larger question remains: how do observers form impressions of a target individual purely on the basis of his or her everyday physical appearance and how does the individual’s appearance reflect what he or she is actually like? Judging personality across different physical contexts Researchers have examined impression formation in conditions where there is zero-acquaintance between the observer and the target individual. Albright, Kenny, and Malloy (1988) describe zero-acquaintance as a situation in which one person observes another, but has had no or minimal direct exposure to that target. Past zeroacquaintance research includes judging target individuals on the basis of brief film clips (Borkenau & Liebler, 1992), photographs (Berry & Finch-Wero, 1993; Robins, Gosling, & Donahue, 1997), offices and bedrooms (Gosling, Ko, Manarelli, and Morris, 2002), and everyday language use (Mehl, Pennebaker, Crow, Dabbs, & Price, 2001). Results Clothing 3 of these zero-acquaintance studies have shown that observers exhibit consensus and can make accurate judgments of targets on some personality traits (Paulhaus & Bruce, 1992). In a meta-analysis of nine zero-acquaintance studies performed by Kenny (1994), studies that used photos, short video clips, and brief interactions found that observers exhibited strong consensus for the traits of Extraversion and Conscientiousness while observers exhibited the least consensus for Agreeableness. Gosling et al. (2002) asked observers to rate target individuals’ personalities on the basis of the individuals’ offices or bedrooms. Observers perceived certain aspects (i.e., Conscientiousness; Openness to experience) of personality based on the targets’ physical environments. The wealth and diversity of information found in offices and bedrooms may have increased the observers’ consensus and accuracy for Openness and Conscientiousness (Gosling et al., 2002) where the information provided in Kenny’s (1994) meta-analysis studies did not. Examining impression formation across various physical contexts and environments helps clarify which cues observers use and which cues are valid in making judgments across the different dimensions of personality. Observers gather a variety of information across different environmental contexts (e.g., clothing vs. physical spaces), which may account for the variance in their judgments of a target’s personality. Borkenau & Liebler (1992) found a strong correlation between observed physical attractiveness and the observer’s judgment of the target’s level of Extraversion; their participants reported using cues such as the pitch of target’s voice, showy dress, stylish hair, and friendly expression to judge the target individual as extraverted. In Borkenau & Liebler’s (1992) study, observers used both Clothing visual and acoustic cues to help them to form more accurate impressions of Extraversion; however, observers were unable to accurately judge Conscientiousness using acoustic cues alone. This suggests that judges might focus only on the target’s personality traits that can be accurately inferred from the limited number of physical attributes presented (Borkenau & Liebler, 1992). The Gosling et al. (2002) study of physical spaces (i.e., offices and bedrooms) may have provided more cues to an individual’s level of organization, tidiness, values, or recreational pursuits, which would have allowed the observers to more accurately assess other dimensions of personality such as an individual’s level of Openness and Conscientiousness. Clothing and appearance as an extension of personality Observers can form judgments based on a target’s conscious clothing decisions or behavioral residue that reflects one’s appearance. Gosling et al. (2002) proposed a model to explain how individuals can influence the environment around them and how that individual expression in physical environments provides valuable information that observers use in forming their impressions. According to this model, there are four mechanisms that link individuals to their environments. Figure 1. Model of Processes by which individuals impact their personal environments Mechanism Residue or Evidence of Behavior Underlying Disposition Manifest Behavior 1. Self-directed identity claims Sentimental Collect memorabilia Shirt with favorite city/place 2. Other-directed identity claims Socially responsible Politically Active Candidate’s Campaign Shirt 3. Interior behavioral residue Conscientiousness Well groomed Neat dress and appearance 4. Exterior behavioral residue Careless Messy Eater Food Stains Step 2: Observers infer disposition from behavior Step 1: Observers infer behavior from residue 4 Clothing 5 Figure 1 illustrates Gosling et al.’s (2002) model by which individuals influence their environments. The first two mechanisms describe the more conscious efforts of clothing selection and are categorized as identity claims: self-directed and otherdirected. Individuals may choose clothing (e.g., a t-shirt with cultural symbols or icons) that (1) makes statements intended to reinforce their self-views or (2) communicates their attitudes and values to others. The third and fourth mechanisms describe how aspects of personality unconsciously permeate our clothing selection. These mechanisms are identified as interior and exterior behavioral residue. Behavioral residue refers to the physical traces or activities that an individual conducts in his or her environment (Gosling, 2002). Observers can use this residue of repeated behaviors to form impressions of personality. An individual’s clothing may contain (3) residue of behavior conducted while wearing it (e.g., grass stains in the knee area of a woman’s jeans). This residue not only reflects past behaviors, but one can imply future behaviors as well (e.g., gardening). Behavioral residue may (4) leave the space that in which it originally occurred. Observers can make inferences about an individual’s behavior that occurred entirely outside the surroundings in which the clothing is seen. From the behavioral residue, observers infer dispositions and personality traits. Gosling et al.’s (2002) model explains how an individual’s clothing choices may consciously and unconsciously reflect elements of his or her personality traits. However, one might argue that daily selection of clothing is not an expression of personality but is instead determined by its function (e.g., cold weather or important meeting). The process of selecting personal attire occurs in stages and includes the Clothing 6 decision to purchase a particular clothing item, the anticipation of potential environmental conditions, and the freedoms or restrictions of various social conditions. Personality can influence choice of attire at any stage in this process. An individual has a wide variety of clothing styles to choose from: professional, casual, stylish, or comfortable attire. In selecting clothing, an individual might choose an outfit that fits his or her social or environmental needs—a well-pressed suit and power tie for an important board meeting; a t-shirt and jeans to run errands; a flashy blouse and mini skirt for a night on the town; or sweatshirt and jogging pants for a cold day’s morning run. An individual also might adjust other aspects of his or her appearance such as hairstyle, jewelry/accessories, or whether to reveal tattoos and body piercings. Consider the hypothetical clothing outfits presented above. The individual attending the board meeting may have chosen a tie that had a cartoon character on it as a way to express his fun-loving and agreeable persona. An outside observer might perceive the individual in torn jeans and a Bob Marley t-shirt to be laid-back and open to new experiences. The individual who dressed up for a party may want to be seen as attractive and outgoing. The individual out for a morning run may have decided to bundle up for the cold morning in a sweatshirt with her favorite sports team. Observers who had never met this individual might identify her as athletic and a loyal sports fan. In addition to selection clothing for its function, personality can play a role in determining clothing selection and appearance. Furthermore, others may use an individual’s appearance to form impressions about him or her. Accuracy and Consensus Clothing 7 Using Brunswik’s (1956) Lens Model, as shown in figure 2, we can see the links between a target’s clothing and appearance and how observers form perceptions of the targets from cues their clothing or appearance provide. Brunswik proposes that elements in the environment can serve as a kind of lens through which observers indirectly perceive underlying constructs. Brunswik’s model helps to explain the specific mechanisms that individuals use to influence their physical environment and how the that environment helps observers draw inferences about him or her. Observers can make accurate judgments by not only using cues from the environment (i.e., cue utilization), but also by using the correct observable cues (i.e., cue validity) in formulating judgments of personality traits. Figure 2: Brunswik’s Lens Model “Lens Cue 1 (e.g., neat & well groomed) Cue 2 (e.g., clean & pressed clothing) Underlying Construct (e.g., Target’s real Conscientiousness) Observer Judgment (e.g., rating of target’s Conscientiousness) Cue 3 (e.g., conservative dress) Cue 4 (e.g., jewelry) Cue Validity Cue Utilization Observer Accuracy Borkenau & Liebler (1992) describe three instances where observers encounter difficulties finding consensus in their ratings with other observers. First, an observer may make (incorrect) judgments for a trait where valid cues do not exist. Second, an observer may overlook valid and existing cues that he or she could have used to infer Clothing 8 specific traits. Finally, both valid cues and trait inferences are present, but observers may infer the wrong personality trait from those cues. We know that some traits may be “easier” detect on the basis of certain cues. How might individuals influence the cues that clothing and physical appearance provide for observers? Present Study This study will build on previous research of physical environments (i.e., offices and bedrooms) at both methodological and theoretical levels (Gosling et al., 2002). Individuals surround themselves with many items that reflect their personality to one degree or another in many different contexts (e.g., progressive bumper stickers on a car; favorite quotes in an email signature). The various environmental contexts that can exhibit personality fall on a continuum from extremely private to extremely public. Offices are a public space and bedrooms are almost entirely a private space. This study will examine a very public context, clothing and physical appearance, to determine how observers form impressions on the basis of target individuals’ everyday physical appearance and how the target individuals’ appearances reflect what they are actually like. I will analyze the agreement of observers (i.e., consensus), the correlation between observers’ impressions and the targets’ self and peer-reports (i.e., accuracy), the features observers use to form impressions (i.e., cue utilization), and the features that are actually related to what the targets are like (i.e., cue validity). Secondly, this study will attempt to differentiate between impressions based on clothing versus other elements of appearance (e.g., physical attractiveness) by showing three separate groups of observers photos that contain either a combination of clothing and appearance, just clothing, and just physical appearance. Clothing 9 Based on the previous research of physical environments (Gosling et al, 2002), I predict significant consensus and accuracy for the personality traits and preferences: political orientation, Extraversion, Conscientiousness and Openness. Also, I predict that there will be a difference in judgments based on information presented—clothing and appearance, clothing only, and appearance only. Method Participants Targets were 160 undergraduate students from the fall introductory psychology subject pool. Observers will be solicited from the spring subject pool. [Will include descriptive statistics] All participants received psychology credit for their participation. Photos of Target Individuals Target individuals were photographed twice (at the beginning and at midsemester) in a standardized pose (i.e., standing with legs shoulder-width apart, hands at side, with a neutral facial expression) wearing the clothing of their choice. Two photo sessions occurred in order to capture the variation of clothing selection across seasons. These individuals filled out personality assessments and provided names of two friends who also provided personality assessments of them. Self- and peer-ratings will be compared to observers’ ratings to determine accuracy. Slide Presentation Participants will view a slide show containing 40 slides with a standardized photo of each target per slide. Group A will view standardized photos in their entirety (i.e., face, body, and clothing). Group B will view standardized photos with the body and clothing blacked out using a photo editor (i.e., face only). Group C will view Clothing 10 standardized photos with the face blacked out using a photo editor (i.e., body and clothing only). Groups will further be divided into participants who view clothing choices from session 1 and those who view clothing choices from session 2. Observers’ ratings of session 1 and session 2 will be compared to determine if the perception of a target’s personality remains constant over time. Instruments Participants will assess the targets’ personalities using a 10-item rating form. Five of the items assess the dimensions of the Five Factor Model (McCrae & Costa, 1999). The remaining five items asses other aspects of personality: athleticism, wealth, intelligence, attractiveness, and political views. Each item is rated on a seven-point bipolar scale (e.g., 1=Reserved, shy; 7=Extraverted, talkative). Procedure I will run participants who will serve as observers in groups of 10-15 in several conditions. Each participant will receive a packet containing 40 rating forms described above. I will show participants a slide show containing 40 target individuals’ photos (altered depending on the condition). The participants will view photos of each target for 45 seconds and then make ratings on the 10-item personality inventory using a seven-point bipolar scale. Clothing 11 References Bardack, N. R. & McAndrew, P. T. (1985). The influence of physical attractiveness and manner of dress on success in a simulated personnel decision. The Journal of Social Psychology, 125, 777-778. Berry, D. S., & Finch-Wero, J. L.. (1993). Accuracy in face perception: A view from ecological psychology. Journal of Personality, 61, 497-521. Borkenau, P. & Liebler, A. (1992). Trait Inferences: Sources of validity at zeroacquaintance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(4), 645-657. Brunswick, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley: University of California Press. Burroughs, J. W., Drews, D. R., & Hallman, W. K. (1991). Predicting personality from personal possessions: A self-presentational analysis. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 147-163. Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50, 164-168. Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A Room with a Cue: Judgments of Personality Based on Offices and Bedrooms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379-398. Kenny, D. A. (1994). Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis. New York: Guilford Press. Mehl, M.R., Pennebaker, J. W., Crow, D. M., Dabbs, J. & Price, J. H. (2001). The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR): A device for sampling naturalistic daily Clothing 12 activities and conversations. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 33(4), 517-523. Paulhaus, D. L., & Bruce, M. N. (1992). The effect of acquaintanceship on the validity of personality impressions: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 816-824. Robins, R. W., Gosling, S. D., & Donahue, E. M. (1997). Are personality judgments based on physical appearance consensual and accurate? In J. Bermúdez, B. de Raad, J. de Vries, A. M. Pérez-García, A. Sánchez-Elvira, & G. L. van Heck (Eds.) Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 6, pp. 70-75). Tilburg, the Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.