Long Term Memory – Reinforcement Learning Task Name Description Cognitive Construct Validity Neural Construct Validity Sensitivity to Manipulation Probabilistic Reversal Learning Subjects select one stimulus from an array of concurrently presented stimuli (preferably 3); typically, more than one set of discriminanda are presented, in a randomized order across trials. Each choice is followed by positive or negative feedback (correct choice or incorrect choice). The relationship between the stimulus presented and the feedback given can be deterministic or probabilistic. Subjects must, based upon feedback alone, learn which cue in each discriminanda set are associated with positive feedback and then maintain optimized performance of the rule (rule maintenance). Once the subject meets pre-set performance criteria, feedback is adjusted without warning. In other words, the rules are "reversed" in one or more of the discriminanda sets. Subjects must update their behavior based upon the change in rules. Optimal reversal performance is often considered to involve cognitive control over pre-potent responding. Acquisition of a multiple choice visual discrimination is believed to reflect implicit learning processes. Optimal performance of a visual discrimination is thought to reflect rule maintenance. Reversal of a learned visual discrimination is thought to measure cognitive control over pre-potent responding (response inhibition). Interactions between declarative systems (hippocampal and prefrontal) and implicit systems (striatum) are predicted across initial learning of a discrimination, such that increasing activation in the striatum, as a function of trials performed, occurs along with learning. The ventrolateral and ventromedial frontal cortex are particularly involved in the effective inhibition of responses at reversal. Evidence in both animals and humans for change in response to pharmacological manipulations (Cools, Altamirano, & D'Esposito, 2006; Cools, Lewis, Clark, Barker, & Robbins, 2007). fMRI (Waltz & Gold, 2007) (Lee, Groman, London, & Jentsch, 2007) (Frank & Claus, 2006) (Cools et al., 2009; Robinson, Frank, Sahakian, & Cools, 2010) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Cools, R., Frank, M. J., Gibbs, S. E., Miyakawa, A., Jagust, W., & D'Esposito, M. (2009). Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific reversal learning and In humans, striatal dopamine predicts outcome specific reversal learning (Cools et al., 2009). (Dias, Robbins, & Roberts, 1996, 1997) (Fellows & Farah, 2005) (Fellows & Farah, 2003) Relationships to Behavior and Schizophrenia Individuals with schizophrenia show impairments in reversal learning (Waltz & Gold, 2007) . Psychometrics Stage of Research Practice effects are unknown, but are the subject of active investigation. Depending upon how the test is conducted, there can be ceiling effects for discrimination learning. The use of multiple discriminada sets, at least 3 stimuli in each set and probabilistic feedback can avoid this. There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia at the behavioral level. Unknown at the neural level. We need to assess psychometric characteristics such as test-retest reliability, practice effects, and ceiling/floor effects for this task. There is some evidence that performance on this task changes in response to psychological or pharmacological intervention (Cools et al., 2006; Cools et al., 2007) its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. J Neurosci, 29(5), 15381543. Robinson, O. J., Frank, M. J., Sahakian, B. J., & Cools, R. (2010). Dissociable responses to punishment in distinct striatal regions during reversal learning. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1459-1467. Probabilisiti c Selection Task fMRI The probabilistic selection (PS) task measures participants' ability to learn from positive and negative feedback, both on a trial-to-trial basis, and in integrating reinforcement probabilities over many trials. It also probes performance in a novel 'test' phase which permits evaluation of whether one implicitly learned more from the positive or negative outcomes of their decisions. This "Go" or "NoGo" learning bias is very sensitive to dopaminergic manipulation and dopamine-related genetics. (Frank, Seeberger, & O'Reilly R, 2004) (Waltz, Frank, Robinson, & Gold, 2007a) (Frank, Moustafa, Haughey, Curran, & Hutchison, 2007) MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O'Reilly R, C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science, 306(5703), 1940-1943. Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry. Performance in this task is defined by the ability to choose the probabilistically most optimal stimulus. Relative positive and negative feedback learning is similarly modulated by dopamine manipulation in this task and others that are meant to measure similar constructs using different stimuli, motor responses, and task rules. Probabilistic positive and negative feedback learning are sensitive to dopaminergic manipulation. Increases in dopaminergic stimulation, likely in the striatum, lead to better positive learning but cause impairments in negative feedback learning. Dopamine depletions, as in Parkinson's disease and older seniors (greater than 70 years of age) are associated with relatively better negative feedback learning. Genes that control dopamine function in the striatum are predictive of probabilistic positive and negative learning, whereas genes that control dopamine function in prefrontal cortex are predictive of rapid trial-to-trial learning from negative feedback. Negative feedback learning is also associated with enhanced error- There are multiple examples of how performance is sensitive to pharmacological manipulations of the dopamine system (Frank et al., 2004; Santesso et al., 2009). Cavanagh, Frank & Allen (2010) reported that performance and associated EEG measures are affected by social stress, in a way that interacts with both trait vulnerability and state variables (negative affect) (Cavanagh, Frank, & Allen, 2010). Petzold et al (2010) also reported a behavioral study in which learning from negative feedback was affected by stress (Petzold, Plessow, Goschke, & Kirschbaum, An initial behavioral study showed that patients with schizophrenia exhibit selective impairments in learning from positive but not negative prediction errors in this task (Waltz, Frank, Robinson, & Gold, 2007b). Other imaging studies (using a different task) report that SZ patients exhibit reduced neural representation of positive but not negative prediction errors, including in the striatum (Waltz et al., 2009). Practice effects have been assessed in Frank & O'Reilly (2006). On average participants are faster to learn the task after multiple sessions, but this practice does not affect relative positive versus negative feedback learning. (Frank & O'Reilly R, 2006) However we have found in other unpublished data that when the task is run twice in a row (in the same session) there is little correspondence between learning bias in the two iterations, and that striatal dopamine genetic polymorphisms are only predictive of performance in the first iteration There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. Data already exists on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. There is evidence that performance on this task can improve in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. related negativity (brain potentials originating from anterior-cingulate cortex) and activation of this same region in fmri. 2010). (Doll and Frank, in review). It is likely that repeated performance of the task allows participants to use other higher order strategies once they understand the task structure, such that single gene effects on simple aspects of reward and punishment learning are no longer visible, but that larger effects due to pharmacological manipulations nevertheless persist. (Frank et al., 2007) (Frank, Woroch, & Curran, 2005) (Klein et al., 2007) (Frank & O'Reilly R, 2006) Corlett Associative Learning Task fMRI This task is a retrospective revaluation paradigm in which expectations creating during a learning phase are violated to produce a prediction error. Subjects are asked to imagine themselves working as an allergist confronted with a new patient ‘Mr X’, who suffers allergic reactions following some meals but not others. Their task was to work out which foods caused allergic reactions by observing the consequences of eating various foods. Trials consistent of the presentation of a food picture (representing a meal eaten by Mr X), a predictive response by the subject and, following this, an outcome indicated whether or not Mr. X got sick. There are three phases to the task. In Stage 1, participants learn the initial expectancies. The key trial types in this re four pairs of foods in which subjects See task description. A reliable marker for prediction-error processing has been identified in right prefrontal cortex (rPFC) using fMRI (Corlett et al., 2004; Fletcher et al., 2001; Turner et al., 2004). Ketamine, a drug which causes a transient psychosis, perturbs this brain response in healthy individuals Corlett et al., 2006). Furthermore, the magnitude of the prediction-error response under placebo predicts an Ketamine alters casual associate prediction learning Corlett et al., 2006) . Psychotic individual show impaired causal associative learning and reduced violation of expectances (Corlett et al., 2007) . Unknown There is evidence that this specific task elicits deficits in schizophrenia. We need to collect data on psychometric characteristics of this task, such as testretest reliability, practice effects, ceiling/floor effects. There is evidence that performance on this task can change in response to psychological or pharmacological interventions. learn to expect that these food pairs will always predict an allergic response. In Stage 2, single foods, one from each of the compounds presented in stage 1, re presented with or without allergic outcomes. These trials re arranged to cause retrospective revaluation of the absent but expected cues from stage 1. For example, in the backward blocking condition one cue from a pair that had previously caused an allergy was itself paired with an allergy. This was designed to create a downward revaluation of the allergenic status of the other cue. In the unovershadowing condition, one cue from a pair that had previously been paired with an allergy was presented without an allergy outcome. In Stage 3, the outcome of some trials could violate the expectation engendered by any retrospective revaluation that had occurred during stage 2, and others fulfilled the predictions. Thus, stage 3 allows one to compare brain activity during trials in which prediction error should be larger (backward blocked items in which an allergic reaction occurred and unovershadowed items in which an allergic response did not occur;) in comparison to perfectly matched stimuli from stage 1 in which prediction error should be smaller (unovershadowed items in which an allergic reaction occurred and backward blocked items in which an allergic reaction did not occur). MANUSCRIPTS ON THE WEBSITE: Corlett, P. R., Honey, G. D., Aitken, M. R., Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. R., Absalom, A. R., et al. (2006). Frontal responses during learning predict vulnerability to the psychotogenic effects of ketamine: linking cognition, brain activity, and psychosis. Arch Gen individual’s likelihood of experiencing delusional beliefs under ketamine Corlett et al., 2007). Psychiatry, 63(6), 611-621. Corlett, P. R., Murray, G. K., Honey, G. D., Aitken, M. R., Shanks, D. R., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2007). Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain, 130(Pt 9), 2387-2400. REFERENCES: Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., & Allen, J. J. (2010). Social stress reactivity alters reward and punishment learning. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. Cools, R., Altamirano, L., & D'Esposito, M. (2006). Reversal learning in Parkinson's disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1663-1673. Cools, R., Frank, M. J., Gibbs, S. E., Miyakawa, A., Jagust, W., & D'Esposito, M. (2009). Striatal dopamine predicts outcome-specific reversal learning and its sensitivity to dopaminergic drug administration. J Neurosci, 29(5), 1538-1543. Cools, R., Lewis, S. J., Clark, L., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W. (2007). L-DOPA disrupts activity in the nucleus accumbens during reversal learning in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(1), 180-189. Corlett, P. R., Aitken, M. R., Dickinson, A., Shanks, D. R., Honey, G. D., Honey, R. A., et al. (2004). Prediction error during retrospective revaluation of causal associations in humans: fMRI evidence in favor of an associative model of learning. Neuron, 44(5), 877-888. Corlett, P. R., Murray, G. K., Honey, G. D., Aitken, M. R., Shanks, D. R., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2007). Disrupted prediction-error signal in psychosis: evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain, 130(Pt 9), 2387-2400. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1996). Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature, 380, 69-72. Dias, R., Robbins, T. W., & Roberts, A. C. (1997). Dissociable forms of inhibitory control within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: restrictions to novel situations and independence from "on-line" processing. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(23), 9285-9297. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2003). Ventromedial frontal cortex mediates affective shifting in humans: evidence from a reversal learning paradigm. Brain, 126(Pt 8), 1830-1837. Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. Cereb Cortex, 15(1), 58-63. Fletcher, P. C., Anderson, J. M., Shanks, D. R., Honey, R., Carpenter, T. A., Donovan, T., et al. (2001). Responses of human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory. Nat Neurosci, 4(10), 1043-1048. Frank, M. J., & Claus, E. D. (2006). Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychol Rev, 113(2), 300-326. Frank, M. J., Moustafa, A. A., Haughey, H. M., Curran, T., & Hutchison, K. E. (2007). Genetic triple dissociation reveals multiple roles for dopamine in reinforcement learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 104(41), 16311-16316. Frank, M. J., & O'Reilly R, C. (2006). A mechanistic account of striatal dopamine function in human cognition: psychopharmacological studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behav Neurosci, 120(3), 497-517. Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., & O'Reilly R, C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science, 306(5703), 1940-1943. Frank, M. J., Woroch, B. S., & Curran, T. (2005). Error-related negativity predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases. Neuron, 47(4), 495-501. Klein, T. A., Neumann, J., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., von Cramon, D. Y., & Ullsperger, M. (2007). Genetically determined differences in learning from errors. Science, 318(5856), 1642-1645. Lee, B., Groman, S., London, E. D., & Jentsch, J. D. (2007). Dopamine D2/D3 receptors play a specific role in the reversal of a learned visual discrimination in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(10), 2125-2134. Petzold, A., Plessow, F., Goschke, T., & Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Stress reduces use of negative feedback in a feedback-based learning task. Behav Neurosci, 124(2), 248-255. Robinson, O. J., Frank, M. J., Sahakian, B. J., & Cools, R. (2010). Dissociable responses to punishment in distinct striatal regions during reversal learning. Neuroimage, 51(4), 1459-1467. Santesso, D. L., Evins, A. E., Frank, M. J., Schetter, E. C., Bogdan, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs reinforcement learning in humans: evidence from event-related potentials and computational modeling of striatal-cortical function. Hum Brain Mapp, 30(7), 1963-1976. Turner, D. C., Aitken, M. R., Shanks, D. R., Sahakian, B. J., Robbins, T. W., Schwarzbauer, C., et al. (2004). The role of the lateral frontal cortex in causal associative learning: exploring preventative and super-learning. Cereb Cortex, 14(8), 872-880. Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007a). Selective Reinforcement Learning Deficits in Schizophrenia Support Predictions from Computational Models of StriatalCortical Dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry, 62(7), 756-764. Waltz, J. A., Frank, M. J., Robinson, B. M., & Gold, J. M. (2007b). Selective Reinforcement Learning Deficits in Schizophrenia Support Predictions from Computational Models of StriatalCortical Dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry. Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1-3), 296-303. Waltz, J. A., Schweitzer, J. B., Gold, J. M., Kurup, P. K., Ross, T. J., Salmeron, B. J., et al. (2009). Patients with schizophrenia have a reduced neural response to both unpredictable and predictable primary reinforcers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(6), 1567-1577.