Supplementary Materials: Personality, Persuasion and Argumentation Bayesian argumentation focuses on the effect of content on persuasion (Hahn & Oaksford, 2007). In the social psychology of persuasion, a range of non-content factors have been shown to be influential, affective and personality variables in particular (for reviews, see, Eagly & Chaiken, 1993; Johnson, Maio, & Smith-McLallen, 2005). We therefore also investigated whether the evaluator’s personality influences these different ad hominem arguments. The variety of contexts and argument forms are suggestive in this respect. For example, work on message-person congruence (Hirsh, Kang, & Bodenhausen, 2012), would seem to predict that conscientious individuals, who value order and efficiency (Roberts, Chernyshenko, Stark & Goldberg, 2005), may reject the use of ad hominem arguments in a scientific context. On completing the argument evaluation task, all participants completed the ten item personality inventory (TIPI) which is readily available on-line and which has, “adequate levels…[of] convergent and discriminant validity, test–retest reliability, patterns of external correlates” (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003, p. 523). This instrument provides direct estimates of the big five personality traits: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness to New Experiences. TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE To investigate whether the personality of the evaluator affects how persuasive participants found the ad hominem argument we used the difference between the posterior and prior ratings as our index of argument force.1 Together with twelve argument 1 We also repeated these analyses on the posterior ratings as an index of argument strength and very similar results were obtained. type/context combinations this led to 136 possible correlations. To guard against Type 1 errors, we therefore carried out an exploratory principal components analysis using all seventeen variables (for the correlation matrix, see Correlations in the Supplemental Materials available on-line). Six components with eigenvalues greater than 1 were extracted using the maximum likelihood method and rotated using the Varimax procedure. Summary statistics for the rotated component solution are presented in Table 1. If the character of the evaluator influenced how forceful an argument was perceived to be we would expect to find both argument/context and personality variables loading strongly on one or more of the six extracted components. Table 1 shows that only the 5th and the 6th components had loadings from both groups of variables. However, the 5th component loads only -.32 on conscientiousness and so this is really a single variable abusive ad hominem/scientific context component. Moreover, for the 6th component the loadings are not high enough for them to be regarded as reliable (Stevens, 1992, p. 384) and the two argumentation variables loaded more highly on the 1st component. The 5th and 6th components make some intuitive sense. The 6th component suggests that agreeable and emotionally stable individuals do not regard the abusive and tu quoque arguments as persuasive in a domestic context. Moreover, the similarly bipolar 5th component seems to show that conscientiousness is inversely related to acceptance of the abusive ad hominem in a scientific context. While eminently reasonable, further research is required to establish if such effects are reliable. Provisionally, therefore, we must conclude that these data show little evidence of any influence of the character of the evaluator on judgements of the force of the ad hominem argument in these contexts. It is perhaps unsurprising that we found no effects of personality. Children as young as five are very discriminating about weighing the testimony they receive from different sources (Harris, 2012). Moreover, the kinds of testimony provided in these brief ad hominem arguments was quite stereotypical political, domestic, and scientific debates of which participants are likely to have had some experience. The fallacies are also likely to be culturally well embedded. Although not explicitly taught at School (at least not in the UK), critical thinking is actively encouraged in the School curriculum. “Creativity and Critical Thinking” is one of seven key cross-curriculum dimensions identified by the UK’s Qualification and Curriculum Authority. This will involve implicit training in avoiding fallacies whether explicitly named or not. While people are differentially susceptible to persuasive messages, argument forms that are culturally and socially well embedded and important in the development of belief formation mechanisms may be much less susceptible to the effects of personality variables. References (not included in main article) Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Orlando, FL, US: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers. Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. R. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504-528. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1 Hirsh, J. B., Kang, S. K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2012). Personalized persuasion: Tailoring persuasive appeals to recipient’s personality traits. Psychological Science, 23, 578581. Johnson, B. T., Maio, G. R., & Smith-McLallen, A. (2005). Communication and attitude change: Causes, processes, and effects. In D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 617-669). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Roberts, B. W., Chernyshenko, O. S., Stark, S., & Goldberg, L. R. (2005). The structure of conscientiousness: An empirical investigation based on seven major personality questionnaires. Personnel Psychology, 58, 103–139. Stevens, J. (1992). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Table 1 Rotated Component Matrix - Loadings Component Variables 1st Abuse/Dom TuQ/Sci TuQ/Dom Circ//Sci Circ/Pol 2nd Cont/Sci Cont/Dom TuQ/Pol Circ/Dom 3rd Openness 4th Cont/Pol Abuse/Pol 5th Abuse/Sci Conscientious 6th Agreeable Emote Not inc. Extra Final Statistics Component Eigenvalue 1st 1.73 2nd 1.24 3rd 1.21 4th 1.12 th 5 0.97 6th 0.78 1st 2nd 3rd 4th 5th 6th .62 .52 .46 .62 .34 * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * .49 .38 .69 .36 * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * .98 * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * .58 .68 * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * .73 -.32 * * * .36 * .39 * * * * * * * * * * * -.38 -.32 * % Var 10.18 7.31 7.09 6.60 5.73 4.58 Cum. % Var 10.18 17.50 24.59 31.19 36.91 41.49 * Indicates a component loading less than .30. Abuse = abusive ad hominem, Circ = circumstantial ad hominem, Cont = control, TuQ = tu quoque ad hominem, Sci = scientific context, Pol = political context, Dom = domestic context, Openness = Openness to experience, Conscientious = Conscientiousness, Agreeable = Agreeableness, Emote = Emotional stability, Extra = Extraversion, Not inc. = not included in any component, % Var = percentage variance accounted for, Cum. % Var = cumulative percentage variance accounted for. Table 1. Summary statistics for the principal components analysis of the combined argumentation and personality data.