ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts 382 Psychophysics and vision testing: visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, color vision and visual field Tuesday, May 05, 2015 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Exhibit Hall Poster Session Program #/Board # Range: 3887–3909/D0029–D0051 Organizing Section: Visual Psychophysics / Physiological Optics Contributing Section(s): Anatomy/Pathology, Clinical/ Epidemiologic Research Program Number: 3887 Poster Board Number: D0029 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Slope of the psychometric function for low contrast logMAR charts. Andrew Carkeet1, 2, Ian L. Bailey3. 1Optometry and Vision Science, QUT, Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia; 2QUT, IHBI, Kelvin Grove, QLD, Australia; 3School of Optometry, UC Berkeley, Berkeley, CA. Purpose: Data from acuity charts can be analysed by fitting psychometric functions. While psychometric functions are traditionally used to yield acuity thresholds, the slopes of such psychometric functions can be used to predict the variability of such visual acuity thresholds. This repeated-measures research examined whether high contrast and low contrast acuity charts yield different slopes for their psychometric functions. Methods: Ten participants, 6 female & 4 male, mean age 43 years (SD 18 years), took part in this research. Participants were tested with their preferred eye and wearing their best spectacle correction. Stimuli were Sloan letters presented on an LCD computer monitor, with 9 rows of letters arranged in randomized letter sequences with a standard logMAR chart format. The background had a luminance of 235 cd m-2 and the high and low contrasts were 99.2% and 18.7% Weber contrast. Each participant read 32 charts, 16 at low contrast and 16 at high contrast. For each chart, responses were analysed by probit analysis to generate thresholds and slopes for the psychometric functions. Results: For our participants, the mean high and low contrast visual acuity thresholds (logMAR) were -0.189 0.076 and -0.027 0.079 respectively. Probit sizes were used as a measure of the slopes of the psychometric functions, with smaller probit sizes indicating steeper slopes. Low contrast acuity charts yielded flatter psychometric functions than high contrast acuity charts, indicating a more gradual transition between seeing and non-seeing for the low contrast charts. The difference was statistically significant (F1,9=12.8, p=0.006). The estimates of slope differed slightly, according to whether a lower asymptote of 0.1 (1 in 10 guess rate) or 0.0385 (1 in 26 guess rate) was selected for probit fits. (F1,9=74.1, p<0.001). Probit sizes and inter-subject standard deviations are shown in Table 1. Conclusions: These results indicate that visual acuity measurements are intrinsically more variable with low contrast charts. Previous research has shown that low levels of optical blur also flatten the psychometric function for visual acuity. Monte Carlo modelling based on the probit values shows that stopping patients reading down a low contrast acuity chart, after they’ve made three or more mistakes on a 5-letter row, gives close to optimal precision of acuity measurements. Table 1. Mean probit sizes (logMAR) with inter subject standard deviations. Commercial Relationships: Andrew Carkeet, None; Ian L. Bailey, None Program Number: 3888 Poster Board Number: D0030 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Validation of the Dyop™ Visual Acuity Test Paul A. Harris, Erin Keim. Southern College of Optometry, Memphis, TN. Purpose: Sloan and Snellen optotypes are the global eye chart standard for visual acuity testing, but routinely patients struggle with the required endpoint of response confusion. A new Dynamic Optotype, or “Dyop”, using a dynamically sized, rotating, visual arcarea figure to measure acuity was prospectively, clinically compared to Sloan measures of visual acuity under various test conditions. The Dyop has a completely unique endpoint: the rotation animation appears to suddenly stop when threshold is reached. Methods: Acuity was assessed with 162 subjects each randomly with the Dyop test (Konan Medical Chart2020) and Harris StairStep test (M&S Technologies) comparing each with the following strategies: BCVA, UCVA, + Lens (+2, +3, +4) over spectacles. The relationship between Sloan VA/20 and Dyop size in arc-minutes (both log-transformed) was investigated using correlations and repeatedmeasures log-log regression models. Results: There was a strong linear relationship between Sloan and Dyop acuity measures (Pearson r=.94; p<001). In a single predictor model, the Dyop measure explained 89% of the variance in Sloan acuity. An interaction model relaxing the assumption of common slopes by testing condition indicated a significant measure X condition interaction (p=.004), and explained over 91% of the variance in Sloan acuity. Optimal conversion algorithms between Dyop and Sloan measures were developed via regression models. Conclusions: The Dyop is a novel method of measuring visual acuity that is strongly associated with, and may offer a viable alternative to traditional visual acuity methods. Beyond high correlation with standard methods, the Dyop was observed to be advantaged by speed to threshold endpoint, finer acuity granularity compared to the typically used acuity “line” steps, and ease of endpoint interpretation by subjects. Figure 1: Plot of the log of the Dyop size in arc minutes against the log of the Sloan VA/20. Rx Corrected = Dark Gray (x) +2.00 blur = Orange (o) +3.00 blur = Red (+) +4.00 blur = Green (•) ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts to be presented avoiding the glare due to the glossy screen of the iPad tablet. Commercial Relationships: Jae-hyung Kim, None; Sang Yoon Hyun, None; Ju Byung Chae, None; Soolienah Rhiu, None; Hye Jin Lee, None Figure 2: Scatter plots for each of the separate conditions. Correlations were significant to the p <.001 level in all conditions. Pearson correlations for each condition were: Rx Corrected r=.54, +2 blur r=.72, +3 blur r=.72, +4 blur r=.63, overall pooled r=.94. Commercial Relationships: Paul A. Harris, None; Erin Keim, None Program Number: 3889 Poster Board Number: D0031 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM A pilot trial for visual acuity testing using a random method visual acuity application Jae-hyung Kim1, Sang Yoon Hyun1, Ju Byung Chae1, Soolienah Rhiu2, Hye Jin Lee3. 1Ophthalmology, Chungbuk National University Hospital, Cheongju-si; 2Ophthalmology, Hallym University Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, Hwaseong, Korea (the Republic of); 3 Ophthalmology, Jeju National University, Jeju, Korea (the Republic of). Purpose: A visual acuity (VA) testing app for the iPad tablet computer using mirroring technique was developed which randomly presented letters categorized by cognoscibility. The aim of this study was to assess whether measurements of distance VA using this application were in agreement with standard clinical tests of VA in adults with normal vision. Methods: Forty-three normally sighted subjects were tested using Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) chart. The logMAR VA results were compared with those from the iPad based application which contains a Snellen chart, a Tumbling E chart, a Landolt C chart and a VA chart consisted with Arabic figures. After a 10-min break, subjects were retested with each test in the same order. Repeatability was assessed by testing the subjects 1-day later with each visual chart. Repeatability and agreement were assessed by determining the 95% limits of agreement (LoA) ± 1.96 SD of the differences between tests. Results: The logMAR VA showed no significant difference between the ETDRS chart and the iPad Snellen chart (P=0.66) and iPad Arabic figure chart (P=0.29). The logMAR VA of the ETDRS chart was significantly better than iPad Tumbling E chart (P<0.01) and iPad Landolt C chart (P<0.01). The subjects showed no chart letter memory of the ETDRS chart (P=0.05), iPad Snellen chart (P=0.62), and iPad Arabic figure chart (P=0.12). The logMAR VA of Tumbling E chart (P=0.03) and Landolt C chart (P=0.001) was significantly better at 10 min. Conclusions: The iPad-based application of VA charts showed similar repeatability and may be a rapid and convenient alternative to some existing measures. Mirroring technique allows the visual chart Program Number: 3890 Poster Board Number: D0032 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM A Comparative Clinical Evaluation of Two Visual Acuity Testing Systems: ETDRS vs. FrACT Linda Tsai, Eugenia Thomas, Janice Tarrant, Stan Bentow, Sanjeev Kasthurirangan. Clinical R&D, Abbott Medical Optics, Santa Ana, CA. Purpose: Visual acuity (VA) results with physical Early Treatment of Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) letter charts are heavily dependent upon the test administrator ability (i.e., better threshold VA with persistent encouragement, accurate recording of correctly read letters). In addition, the risk of subject memorization with physical charts requires the administrator to change charts frequently. The impact of such factors may be reduced with the use of a VA software program that automates testing by allowing the test subject to view randomized optotypes in a staircase size presentation and respond with direct input to the software program, which then determines threshold acuity. This clinical study evaluated results with both test systems. Methods: A total of 25 subjects were evaluated for distance-corrected visual acuity at far, intermediate and near and distance defocus testing from +2.00 diopters (D) to -4.00 D, in 0.5 D increments. Each subject was tested monocularly with a retroilluminated ETDRS chart and a computer-based vision testing software, the Freiburg Visual Acuity and Contrast Test (FrACT). All results were obtained in logMAR format, and analyses were conducted in paired comparisons between the two testing systems. Results: Across all test distances, visual acuity results with FrACT were found to be within 0.05 logMAR of ETDRS chart results. Generally, a difference less than 0.1 logMAR (1 line of Snellen acuity) was found in 80% (20/25) of subjects. ETDRS visual acuities were frequently better than that with FrACT; there was a 0.1 logMAR difference at far and 0.06 logMAR differences at both intermediate and near. With defocus testing, there was a mean difference of 0.05 logMAR across the defocus range, with the greatest differences between -2.00 and -4.00 D (mean of 0.08 logMAR). The mean bestcorrected distance VA (BCDVA) was slightly better than the mean VA with the manifest refraction (0.00 D defocus) in the defocus sequence with both ETDRS (0.02 logMAR) and FrACT (0.03 logMAR). Conclusions: VA test results with the FrACT system were found to be slightly worse than that with the ETDRS letter charts. However, the benefits of automated administration of VA testing with the FrACT system merit further evaluation of test factors that may affect results. Commercial Relationships: Linda Tsai, Abbott Medical Optics (E); Eugenia Thomas, Abbott Medical Optics (E); Janice Tarrant, Abbott Medical Optics (E); Stan Bentow, Abbott Medical Optics (E); Sanjeev Kasthurirangan, Abbott Medical Optics (E) Program Number: 3891 Poster Board Number: D0033 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Evaluation of defocus curve performance with two visual acuity testing systems: ETDRS and FrACT Janice Tarrant, Eugenia Thomas, Linda Tsai, Sanjeev Kasthurirangan. Abbott Medical Optics, Milpitas, CA. Purpose: An important clinical method to evaluate multifocal and accommodating intraocular lens performance is the defocus curve ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts test. To have a clear interpretation of the defocus curve data, the test methods used and the metrics obtained from this test should be evaluated relative to other clinical measures. The aim of this study was to compare near visual acuity (VA) and defocus curve measurements with two different methods, standard letter charts (ETDRS) and a computer based technique (FrACT), in nonpresbyopic and presbyopic subjects. Methods: A total of 25 subjects were recruited (22 to 80 years; mean: 46 ± 15 years). Outcome measures included monocular manifest refraction of the right eye, distance corrected intermediate VA (DCIVA) at 66 cm and distance corrected near VA (DCNVA) at 40 cm with both the ETDRS and FrACT systems. Defocus curves (far visual acuity measured through trial lenses from +2.0 D to -4.0 D in 0.5 D steps) were measured with both systems. Near add power at 40 cm and push-down accommodative amplitude (AA) were obtained. Two depth of focus (DOF) metrics from defocus curves were evaluated: a 20/32 VA threshold (DOF-M1) and a 0.2 logMAR loss in VA from the 0.0 D defocus VA (DOF-M2). Age related trends for DCIVA, DCNVA and depth of focus measurements were evaluated through regression analyses and slopes compared. Results: Intermediate VA (DCIVA) declined at about 0.1 logMAR per decade with both ETDRS (-0.367 + 0.009 * age; R2 = 0.51) and FrACT (-0.430 + 0.011 * age; R2 = 0.61). Near VA (DCNVA) declined at 0.14 logMAR per decade with both ETDRS (-0.455 + 0.014 * age; R2 = 0.58) and FrACT (-0.408 + 0.014 * age; R2 = 0.58). Depth of focus metrics (DOF-M1) and (DOF-M2), measured with ETDRS and FrACT, showed a significant relationship with age (p < 0.05 for all regression slopes) and had slopes between -0.6 D and -0.7 D per decade. Similarly, minimum near add declined at 0.6 D per decade (R2 = 0.83). All non-presbyopes accommodated well to the ETDRS chart, but some showed reduced accommodation with the FrACT system, especially through -3.5 D and -4.0 D defocus levels. Conclusions: The ETDRS charts and FrACT computer system measured comparable reductions in intermediate visual acuity and near visual acuity and depth of focus with increasing age. Generally, with each decade increase in age, near vision declined at 0.1 logMAR and depth of focus declined at about 0.6 D. Commercial Relationships: Janice Tarrant, None; Eugenia Thomas, None; Linda Tsai, None; Sanjeev Kasthurirangan, None and an iPad, respectively. The tests were controlled by a 2-down, 1-up staircase procedure with 4 reversals. SDH was estimated using a maximum likelihood fitting procedure. Results obtained from 162 eyes of these 86 subjects with BCVA 20/100 or better were included for linear regression and Bland-Altman analysis to assess the agreement of the self-testing results obtained with these two paradigms. Results: The linear regression of the SDH obtained with the 4AFC mVTTM versus those obtained with 3AFC mVTTM showed that the results of these two paradigms are highly correlated (r = 0.87, p<0.0001). The slope of linear regression is 0.94 (95% confidence interval, 0.85 – 1.02), including slope one, suggesting no significant difference in SDH measurements by these two testing paradigms. The Bland-Altman plot of the difference of 4AFC and 3AFC measurements versus their means showed the mean difference is 0.057 logMAR, indicating that SDH measured by 4AFC paradigm is slightly worse than that by 3AFC paradigm. This bias is significantly different from zero because the 95% confidence interval (0.033 to 0.081 logMAR) of the mean difference doesn’t include zero. Conclusions: The performance of mVTTM employing a 4AFC testing paradigm is comparable to that of mVTTM using a 3AFC testing paradigm. The slight bias of the 4AFC measurements compared to 3AFC supports the hypothesis that the 4AFC paradigm reduces chance level (lucky guesses), so that it reduces the likelihood of overestimating patients’ ability to detect distortion in a shape discrimination task. Program Number: 3892 Poster Board Number: D0034 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Comparison of myVisionTrack® Vision Monitor Performance with 3-Alternative Forced-Choice (3AFC) and 4AFC Testing Paradigms for Assessing Shape Discrimination Hyperacuity Michael B. Bartlett1, Gina Mitzel2, Song Zhang3, Yi-Zhong Wang2, 4 1 . Vital Art and Science, LLC, Richardson, TX; 2Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, TX; 3Clinical Sciences, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX; 4Ophthalmology, UT Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX. Purpose: The myVisionTrack® (mVTTM), a mobile shape discrimination hyperacuity (SDH) test, was originally developed as a spatial 3-alternative forced-choice (3AFC) test (Wang, et al. IOVS 54:5501, 2013). Newer phones and tablets have bigger screens which allow for additional stimulus patterns to be displayed simultaneously. Increasing the number of choices will reduce the chance level and so decrease the likelihood of overestimating the performance in a psychophysical test. To test this hypothesis we compared the 4AFC and 3AFC test paradigms among normal subjects and patients with maculopathy in this study. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted with 86 subjects (40 with normal vision and 46 with various types of maculopathy). The 3AFC and 4AFC SDH tests were implemented on an iPod touch ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Commercial Relationships: Michael B. Bartlett, Vital Art and Science, LLC (E), Vital Art and Science, LLC (I), Vital Art and Science, LLC (P), Vital Art and Science, LLC (S); Gina Mitzel, None; Song Zhang, None; Yi-Zhong Wang, Vital Art and Science, LLC (C), Vital Art and Science, LLC (F), Vital Art and Science, LLC (I), Vital Art and Science, LLC (P), Vital Art and Science, LLC (S) Support: NIH Grant 5R44EY020016-03 Program Number: 3893 Poster Board Number: D0035 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Objective Alternanting Cover Test technique implemented in a new vision analyzer. Juan Carlos Ondategui Parra, Irene Claramunt, Rosa Borras, Selena Gomez, Jaume Pujol. DAVALOR Research Center (DRC) Universistat Politècnica de Catalunya, Terrassa, Spain. Purpose: To compare the values of near phoria measured with the clinical Alternating Cover Test (CACT) commonly used in clinics, objective method for the patient and subjective method for the examiner, with the objective method using the Alternating Cover Test Technique (OACT) implemented in a prototype of a new fully autonomous and automated vision analyzer (Eye and Vision Analyzer, EVA, DAVALOR, Spain). In this latter one eye movements are recorded while the patient watches a true-3D short video game, with a precise stimulation of accommodation and vergence. Methods: 55 healthy subjects with no previous history of strabismus or amblyopia, no ocular pathology, and no history of eye surgery were enrolled in the study. All eyes achieved a visual acuity equal o higher to 0.00 logMAR. The CACT method was the commonly used in clinics consisting of alternating occlusion of each eye every 2 seconds and measuring the deviation angle with a prism bar in prismatic diopters (PD). The phoria value was defined as the mean value obtained with the maximum and minimal limit technique of the compensation ocular movement. The OACT method was implemented in the prototype of EVA device showing the video game only in one eye during 2 seconds in an alternant manner. The procedure was repeated 5 times. Phoria values were obtained as the deviation angle before the occlusion of the other eye recorded by the eye tracker. All measures were done at near vision (40 cm) and the visual acuity for the test stimuli was 0.2 logMAR. Three measurements were performed for each method. Results: The mean age (mean ± standard deviation) of the sample was 21.5±1.5 years (range: 19 to 24). The mean phoria values obtained were -1.0±3.8 PD for CACT and -2.3±3.4 PD for OACT. The mean value of difference between methods was 1.3±2.2 PD and was statistically significant (p<0.01). The Bland and Altman plot shows a confidence interval at 95% between -5.57 and 3.03 PD. The Pearson Correlation Coefficient between both methods was 0.82 and the Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) was 0.90, therefore, the strength of agreement is very good. Conclusions: The EVA prototype is a useful device to use the Alternating Cover Test procedure to measure phoria. The results obtained with EVA were similar to the results with the method used commonly in clinics. Despite it is statistically significant; it is not clinically significant, because the difference is lower than 2PD. Commercial Relationships: Juan Carlos Ondategui Parra, DAVALOR (F); Irene Claramunt, DAVALOR (F); Rosa Borras, DAVALOR (F); Selena Gomez, DAVALOR (F); Jaume Pujol, DAVALOR (F) Support: DAVALOR, DPI 2011-30090-C02-01 Program Number: 3894 Poster Board Number: D0036 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Objective horizontal heterophoria measurements using a new vision analyzer Jaume Pujol, Rosa Borras, Irene Claramunt, Mireia Sanchez, Alfonso Sanchez-Magan, Juan Carlos Ondategui Parra. Davalor Research Center (DRC) - Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Terrassa, Spain. Purpose: To compare the results of two subjective methods commonly used in clinics to measure horizontal heterophoria with an objective method implemented in a prototype of a new fully autonomous and automated vision analyzer (Eye and Vision ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Analyzer, EVA, DAVALOR, Spain), that records eye movements while the patient watches a true-3D short video game Methods: Measurements were performed in a group of 54 young healthy subjects. Monocular visual acuity at far and near distances equal or better than 0.0 logMAR was required. Subjective methods consisted of Von Graefe with a line of letters (VGL) and the Modified Thorington (MT) test. VGL was performed at 40 cm with an increment speed of prismatic diopters (PD) of 2PD/sec and under controlled conditions of ilumination (L≈450 lux). MT was performed at 40 cm with a RAF ruler and spotlight under controlled conditions of ilumination (L≈50 lux). Moreover, accurate instructions about stimulus alignment were given to the patients. Three measurements were performed for each method with an interval of 5 seconds between them. Objective measurements were made using an Alternant Cover Test procedure (OACT) showing the video game only in one eye during 2 seconds and recording the ocular movements. This procedure was also repeated three times. Runtime, including time for instructions, was also measured Results: The mean age of the sample was 21.5±1.5 years (range:19 to 24). The mean horizontal heterophoria values were -6.7±6.0 PD for VGL, -1.0±3.8 PD for MT and -2.0±3.0 PD for OACT. The mean value of differences was -5,6±5,3 PD for VGL vs MT, -4,6±4,6 PD for VGL vs OACT and 0,9±2,8 PD for MT vs OACT. The 95% confidence interval (Bland & Altman plot) was 10,48 for VGL vs MT; 9.83 for VGL vs OACT and 2.52 for TM vs OACT. The Intraclass Coefficient Correlation (ICC) was 61,2% for MT vs OACT; 61.9% for VGL vs OACT and 80.4% for TM vs OACT. The runtime was 137±20 sec for VGL;, 83±13 sec. for MT and 26±5 sec. for OACT Conclusions: The EVA prototype is a useful device to objectively measure horizontal heterophoria using an Alternating Cover Test procedure. Results show a good ICC (>80%) when OACT is compared with MT. Differences between both methods (1PD) are not clinically significant and are within a good confidence interval. VGL shows higher differences and lower ICC when it is compared with OACT and MT. In addition, OACT is more than 3 times faster than MT and more than 5 times faster than VGL Commercial Relationships: Jaume Pujol, DAVALOR (F); Rosa Borras, DAVALOR (F); Irene Claramunt, DAVALOR (F); Mireia Sanchez, DAVALOR (F); Alfonso Sanchez-Magan, DAVALOR (F); Juan Carlos Ondategui Parra, DAVALOR (F) Support: DPI2011-30090-C02-01 Program Number: 3895 Poster Board Number: D0037 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM A pilot trial for a self-testing application for reading speed Ju Byung Chae1, Sang Yoon Hyun1, Soolienah Rhiu2, Hye Jin Lee3, Jae-hyung Kim1. 1Ophthalmology-Coll of Med, Chungbuk National University Hospital, Cheong-ju, Chung-buk, Korea (the Republic of); 2Dongtan Sacred Heart Hospital, Hallym University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea (the Republic of); 3Department of Ophthalmology, Jeju National University School of Medicine, Jeju, Korea (the Republic of). Purpose: To develop a reading chart app for the iPad tablet computer in the Korean language and to investigate reading speed in a normalsighted population according to age groups. Methods: Sixty-three Korean sentences were selected from textbooks for second grade students in elementary school. Commonly used typeface in everyday printed material, “Batangche” was used. Letter size was presented logMAR 0.0 to 1.0 at 0.1 logMAR steps at a reading distance 40 cm. A 3rd generation retina display iPad was used to present the chart and the sentences were presented randomly for each vision and reading speed was checked twice. Pilot testing followed in 55 normal vision adults under 60 years old of age. The subjects read aloud to prevent them from skipping reading words Results: The average word count for the sentences was 6.5 0.7. The mean reading speed for logMAR 0.5 optotype (point 10) was 133.9 ± 26.1 words per minute (wpm) in 20s (n = 19), 121.1 ± 31.6 in 30s (n = 25), 96.8 ± 17.2 in 40s (n = 6), 67.7 ± 41.9 (n = 5) in 50s. The mean reading speed for logMAR 0.0 optotype (point 3.5) was 121.2 ± 27.2 wpm in 20s, 106.6 ± 34.9 in 30s, 61.4 ± 20.1 in 40s, 43.6 ± 77.2 in 50s. Conclusions: This Korean reading chart app can present a new standard when checking reading speed according to age groups. The app also provides portability and accessibility for this new reading acuity chart. Commercial Relationships: Ju Byung Chae, None; Sang Yoon Hyun, None; Soolienah Rhiu, None; Hye Jin Lee, None; Jaehyung Kim, None Program Number: 3896 Poster Board Number: D0038 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Average Precision as a test-retest reliability measure: a quick CSF study on myopia Michael Dorr1, 2, Luis A. Lesmes2, Tobias Elze3, Hui Wang4, 3, ZhongLin Lu5, Peter J. Bex6. 1Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany; 2Adaptive Sensory Technology, Boston, MA; 3Mass Eye and Ear, Boston, MA; 4Jilin University of Finance and Economics, ChangChun City, China; 5Ohio State University, Columbus, OH; 6 Northeastern University, Boston, MA. Purpose: The Contrast Sensitivity Function (CSF) provides a comprehensive assessment of visual sensitivity, but its routine evaluation in clinical care is hampered by practical challenges. We evaluated test-retest reliability (TRR) of an iPad-based quick CSF implementation (Dorr et al., IOVS 2013) in a cohort of myopes and age-matched controls. Methods: We collected repeated measurements of the full CSF on a handheld device at a viewing distance of 60cm from 101 subjects (63 myopes with 33 uncorrected/30 corrected eyes, 38 controls); in each of 50 trials per measurement, a bandpass-filtered Sloan letter was presented for 500ms. Spatial frequency (SF, 24 levels from .64 to 41cpd) and contrast (48 levels from .2 to 100%) were chosen by the quick CSF algorithm to maximize information gain about the CSF. Subjects then indicated their response (10-AFC) on the touch screen. We computed cross-correlation coefficients (CC) and Bland-Altman Coefficients of Repeatability (CoR) for contrast sensitivities at 6 individual SF, CSF Acuity (the SF where sensitivity=0), and the Area under the Log CSF (AULCSF). However, both CC and CoR are vulnerable to artefacts due to test score range and quantization. Therefore, we also computed Average Precision, the area under the Precision-Recall curve that more accurately describes test-retest variance in terms of between-subject variability: how easily can a repeat measurement be identified from the whole population-wide set of measurements, given only the initial measurement? Results: CC ranged from .873 for contrast sensitivity at 1.5cpd to .98 for the AULCSF. CoR were likewise small for AULCSF (.225) and CSF Acuity (.193), and increased for individual contrast sensitivities at higher SF (1.5cpd, CoR=.2; 18cpd, CoR=.308). Precision-Recall scores were worst for low SF (1.5cpd, AveP=.79) and best for AULCSF (AveP=.87). Notably, repeatability as assessed by AveP was better for uncorrected eyes than for corrected eyes, despite much higher Bland-Altman CoR (e.g. uncorrected AULCSF, AveP=.879, CoR=.255; corrected AULCSF, AveP=.829, CoR=.168). Conclusions: Despite very short testing times (2-3 minutes) and without specialized laboratory equipment, the iPad-based quick CSF test reliably assessed the full CSF in myopes and controls. While ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Bland-Altman CoR is routinely used to quantify TRR, its absolute scores cannot be compared across different test measures; Average Precision should be used instead. Commercial Relationships: Michael Dorr, Adaptive Sensory Technology (I), US 14/399,136 (P); Luis A. Lesmes, Adaptive Sensory Technology (E), Adaptive Sensory Technology (I), US 14/399,136 (P); Tobias Elze, None; Hui Wang, None; Zhong-Lin Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology (I), US 14/399,136 (P); Peter J. Bex, Adaptive Sensory Technology (I), US 14/399,136 (P) Program Number: 3897 Poster Board Number: D0039 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Hierarchical Bayesian adaptive estimation of the contrast sensitivity function: Part I – effect of sample size Hairong Gu, Woojae Kim, Fang Hou, Zhong-Lin Lu, Mark Pitt, Jay Myung. Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH. Purpose: Lesmes et al (2010) developed a Bayesian adaptive method for accurately and efficiently measuring the contrast sensitivity function (CSF). Kim et al (2013) recently proposed a hierarchical Bayesian extension, dubbed hierarchical adaptive design optimization (HADO), that provides a judicious way to exploit prior information gained from past experiments to achieve even greater efficiency. The purpose of the present study is to evaluate the benefits and validity of HADO in both human and simulated experiments. Methods: We first conducted a 10AFC letter identification experiment with 100 subjects using the quick CSF method of Lesmes et al (2010) and used the data to construct informative priors. We varied the amount of information in the priors by using four different numbers of subjects (5, 12, 30,100) included in the prior construction. We then repeated the experiment with 10 new subjects using the four priors. Performance of the CSF estimation was compared between these different prior conditions, and also against the quick CSF method with a diffuse prior. The same HADO procedure was carried out in Monte Carlo simulations as well to take the effect of sampling error into account. Results: Figure 1 shows root-mean-squared-error (RMSE) plots of the area-under-the-log-CSF (AULCSF) averaged across all 10 subjects as a function of trial number. The results showed that the informative priors increased the efficiency by lowering the RMSE at a certain number of trials. From the diffuse prior, for comparison, the reduction of RMSE for sample size 5 and 12 averaging over the first 10 trials is about 6.47 dB and the reduction for sample size 30 and 100 is about 10.72 dB. The errors decreased for all priors as the trials accumulated, with the differences among them being indistinguishable at trial 50. Essentially the same (but less noisy) pattern of results was obtained in simulated experiments. Conclusions: Using well-informed priors in HADO shows higher efficiency in estimating CSF than using the non-informative, diffuse prior in the standard adaptive method. The advantage is considerable even when a small number of subjects are available for constructing the prior. Increasing the sample size brings further but small advantage, which can still be beneficial in clinical settings. Figure 1: Effect of sample sizes on the estimates of AULCSF. Commercial Relationships: Hairong Gu, None; Woojae Kim, None; Fang Hou, None; Zhong-Lin Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (I), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (P); Mark Pitt, None; Jay Myung, None Support: National Institute of Mental Health MH093838 ; National Eye Institute EY021553 Program Number: 3898 Poster Board Number: D0040 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Hierarchical Bayesian adaptive estimation of the contrast sensitivity function: Part II — effect of type of prior Mark Pitt, Hairong Gu, Fang Hou, Woojae Kim, Zhong-Lin Lu, Jay Myung. Psychology, Ohio State Univeristy, Columbus, OH. Purpose: The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) characterizes spatial vision in both normal and clinical populations. The quick CSF method (Lesmes, et al, 2010) measures CSF precisely in only a few trials. Kim et al. (2014) introduced a hierarchical Bayesian framework in which adaptive estimation of the CSF can be further accelerated and improved by using prior knowledge (e.g., parameter estimates) from previously tested participants. The current experiment explored how the specification of priors influences adaptive estimation by comparing conditions in which the priors varied from correctly specified to incorrectly specified. Methods: Priors were created from parameter estimates from a study in which the CSF of 100 observers with normal vision were measured using the quick CSF procedure in a 10AFC letter identification task under three viewing conditions: no filter (Normal), weak neutral density filter (ND1, 78.8% attenuation) and strong neutral density filter (ND2, 97.2% attenuation). In the current study, 10 participants were tested with quick CS in the Normal viewing condition three times, each using a different prior: Normal (correctly specified), ND2 (misspecified) and a mixture prior consisting of equal parts Normal, ND1, and ND2. Diffuse priors (no prior knowledge) were used in a control condition. Results: The root mean squared error (RMSE) of the Area Under the log CSF (AULCSF) was calculated across trials for each participant in all conditions using the estimated true AULCSF, obtained after 100 trials in an additional diffuse condition. Aggregate data over the first ten trials showed that, when compared to results in the diffuse condition, estimation error in the ND2 (misspecified) condition decreased by 0.41dB. The Normal (correctly specified) prior condition yielded the greatest improvement (7.80 dB) over the diffuse condition. Surprisingly, the mixture prior showed a significant benefit (4.24 dB drop). Estimates improved for all conditions as trials accumulated, with differences among them being almost indistinguishable by trial 50. Conclusions: Prior knowledge can influence the accuracy and efficiency of adaptive CSF measurement. A correctly specified prior can greatly improve estimation, but a misspecified prior is ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts comparable to a diffuse prior. When the prior is unknown, a mixture prior is a smart choice, providing significant benefit without cost. Commercial Relationships: Mark Pitt, None; Hairong Gu, None; Fang Hou, None; Woojae Kim, None; Zhong-Lin Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology (S); Jay Myung, None Support: National Institute of Mental Health (MH093838), National Eye Institute (EY021553) Program Number: 3899 Poster Board Number: D0041 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM A large-sample study for evaluating the precision of the quick CSF method Zhong-Lin Lu1, Fang Hou1, Luis A. Lesmes2, Woojae Kim1, Hairong Gu1, Mark Pitt1, Jay Myung1. 1Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH; 2Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC., Boston, MA. Purpose: The quick CSF method (Lesmes, et al, 2010) applies a Bayesian adaptive algorithm to estimate the contrast sensitivity function (CSF) with high precision and reduced testing time (~5 min). We collected a large dataset of CSF to 1) determine reliability as a function of test duration, 2) evaluate the concordance between estimates against intra- and inter-run variability (via Bayesian confidence and repeated testing, respectively), and 3) conduct a power analysis for detecting CSF change. Methods: CSFs of 112 college students with normal vision were repeatedly assessed using quick CSF with a 10-letter identification task. For each observer, running CSF estimates were calculated for each trial, via bootstrap statistics for the area under the log CSF (AULCSF), computed by resampling from the Bayesian posterior distribution of the CSF. Results: 1) After 6 trials, the AULCSFs from the two repeated measurements were significantly correlated. Pearson’s r increased from 0.22 (p=0.02) to 0.84 (p<0.001) as trial number increased from 6 to 50. The 95% confidence interval of the ratio between the two AULCSFs was [0.94, 1.02] at trial 6 and [0.99, 1] at trial 50. 2) The comparable metrics of intra- and inter-run variability provided by standard deviations of AULCSF estimates were 0.20 and 0.25 log units after 10 trials, 0.13 and 0.16 log units after 20 trials, and 0.07 and 0.09 log units after 50 trials, respectively. 3) From the posterior distributions of the CSFs, we computed the minimum AULCSF difference (MAD) that can be detected by quick CSF with 95% posterior probability as a function of both trial and observer numbers (Figure 1). To detect MADs of 0.2, 0.1 and 0.05 l log units with 25 quick CSF trials, we needed to run 2, 6 and 27 observers, respectively. To detect the same MADs in 50 trials, only 1, 3 and 11 observers were needed. With 20 observers, we needed 5, 11, and 30 trials to detect MADs of 0.2, 0.1 and 0.05 log units, respectively. With 112 observers, we needed only 3, 6 and 12 trials to detect the same MADs, respectively. Conclusions: The quick CSF method is very precise and highly reliable. The high precision and reliability make it possible to use the method to efficiently measure CSF and detect CSF changes with greatly reduced sample size and costs in clinical trials. Figure 1. The minimum AULCSF difference (MAD, in log units) that can be detected by quick CSF as a function of both trial and observer numbers. Commercial Relationships: Zhong-Lin Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (I), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (P); Fang Hou, None; Luis A. Lesmes, Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (E), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (I), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (P); Woojae Kim, None; Hairong Gu, None; Mark Pitt, None; Jay Myung, None Support: Supported by the National Eye Institute (EY021553 to ZLL) and by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH093838 to JM and MP) Program Number: 3900 Poster Board Number: D0042 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Evaluating the sensitivity for detecting contrast sensitivity changes using the quick CSF method Fang Hou1, Luis A. Lesmes2, Woojae Kim1, Hairong Gu1, Mark Pitt1, Jay Myung1, Zhong-Lin Lu1. 1Department of Psychology, the Ohio State University, Columbus, OH; 2Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC., Boston, MA. Purpose: The contrast sensitivity function (CSF) has shown promise for monitoring the progression of vision loss in eye disease or its remediation with treatment (Barnes, et al., 2004). The quick CSF is a novel Bayesian adaptive method developed to enable the wide, easy application of precise CSF testing. Here, we 1) induced predictable visual changes in a large sample of observers, 2) determined the quick CSF’s sensitivity for detecting CSF changes, and 3) used the large dataset to empirically determine the minimum sample sizes and testing times needed to detect CSF changes. Methods: CSFs of 112 college students with normal vision were assessed by the quick CSF with a 10-letter identification task in low, medium, and high mean luminance conditions: 2.65 (L), 20.2 (M) and 95.4 (H) cd/m2. Visual acuity (VA) was measured using the Snellen eye chart. Data were analyzed using Bayesian and conventional statistics. For each observer, CSF metrics across different luminance conditions were calculated in each trial, via bootstrap statistics for the area under the log CSF (AULCSF), computed by resampling from the Bayesian posterior distribution of the CSF. Results: 1) Average AULCSF values obtained in the L and M conditions were only 38.7% and 72.1% of those obtained in the ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts H condition, while average relative VA values were 95% and 99%. Although both AULCSF (F(2,335)=293, p<0.001) and VA (F(2,332)=5.89, p<0.001) reductions were significant, the magnitudes of AULCSF changes were much greater than those of VA reduction (F(1,443)=461, p<0.001). 2) After 10 trials, we needed only 1, 3, and 17 subjects to reliably (with 95% posterior probability) detect AULCSF changes in comparisons of L-H, L-M, and M-H conditions, respectively; after 25 trials, we needed only 1 subject to detect all the changes. In contrast, we needed 16, 20, and 82 subjects to reliably detect VA changes in those comparisons 3) With 10 subjects, we needed 3, 4, and 11 quick CSF trials to reliably detect AULCSF changes between the L-H, L-M and M-H luminance conditions, respectively. With 25 subjects, we needed only 2, 3, and 7 trials. Conclusions: This assay calibration study demonstrates that the quick CSF method is very sensitive in detecting CSF changes. Whereas the current study uses the quick CSF to detect visual changes modeled by different luminance conditions, future studies will examine the performance of the method in clinical settings. Commercial Relationships: Fang Hou, None; Luis A. Lesmes, Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (E), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (I), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (P); Woojae Kim, None; Hairong Gu, None; Mark Pitt, None; Jay Myung, None; Zhong-Lin Lu, Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (I), Adaptive Sensory Technology, LLC. (P) Support: Supported by the National Eye Institute (EY021553 to ZLL) and by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH093838 to JM and MP) in first and second measurement and the limits of agreement as assessed by the Altman and Bland plots. The AULCSF had upper limit of agreement (ULA) -0.18 and lower limit of agreement (LLA) +0.22. The CSF acuity had ULA -7.0 and LLA +10.9 cpd. The t-tests revealed that all parameters, including AULCSF and CS at various cpd, were not significantly different between the first and second sets of measurement (paired t-test p>0.05). However, CSF acuity was significantly higher in the second measurement compared to the first measurement (mean difference 1.95 cpd; paired t-test t-statistic -2.09, p= 0.04). Conclusions: The quick CSF provides contrast sensitivity parameters that are repeatable in groups with normal and mild impaired vision. Program Number: 3901 Poster Board Number: D0043 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Repeatability of measurements obtained using the quick CSF method Lilia Babakhan1, Anna Parfenova1, Katherine Ha1, Raymond Maeda1, Steven Thurman2, Aaron Seitz2, Pinakin G. Davey1. 1College of Optometry, Western University of Health Sciences, Burbank, CA; 2 UC Riverside, Riverside, CA. Purpose: Contrast sensitivity is a fundamental measure of visual function, and obtaining quick and reliable estimates of contrast sensitivity is vital in clinical settings. The quick CSF is an adaptive method that uses Bayesian inference and a trial-to-trial information gain strategy to obtain rapid measurements of contrast sensitivity and to produce an estimate of the entire contrast sensitivity function (CSF) (Lesmes et al Journal of Vision 2010, ARVO 2012 and 2013).The purpose of the present study was to evaluate short term repeatability of CSF parameter estimates using the quick CSF method. Methods: Twenty four individuals with normal or mild impaired vision (glaucoma, cataract or age related macular degeneration) participated in the study. Measurements were performed twice with the quick CSF technique in binocular viewing conditions. The CSF was measured with 50 trials and estimates of area under the log CSF (AULCSF), high spatial frequency cutoff (CSF acuity), and contrast sensitivity at 1, 1.5, 3, 6, 12 and 18 cycles per degree (cpd) were obtained (see figure 1). Altman and Bland plots were performed to evaluate the limits of agreement and paired t-tests were performed to assess difference between the first and second measurements. Results: The median age of the study participants was 32 years (range 22-76). The figure 2 provides the contrast sensitivity measures obtained Commercial Relationships: Lilia Babakhan, None; Anna Parfenova, None; Katherine Ha, None; Raymond Maeda, None; ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Steven Thurman, None; Aaron Seitz, None; Pinakin G. Davey, None Support: NIH 1 R01EY023582 Program Number: 3902 Poster Board Number: D0044 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Clinical assessment of the Landolt C-CSF test-M&S Smart System Contrast Sensitivity Testing Device Kaydee McCray1, Violeta Paronian1, Aaron Seitz2, Pinakin G. Davey3. 1 Gradutate College of Biomedical Sciences, Western University of Health Sciences, Pomona, CA; 2Psychology, University of CaliforniaRiverside, Riverside, CA; 3College of Optometry, Western University of Health Sciences, Pomona, CA. Purpose: Contrast sensitivity function is decreased in many disease entities notably: cataract, age related macular degeneration and glaucoma. It is also lowered in individuals that undergo keratorefractive surgery procedures. Traditional contrast sensitivity tests printed on charts are limited in the fact that a certain working distance is needed and are prone to degradation due to use. The electronic M&S Smart System contrast sensitivity testing unit provides the flexibility to use at variable distance and provides better resolution capabilities especially at lower contrast due to the visual display system. The purpose of the study was to evaluate the short-term repeatability and use of Landolt C contrast sensitivity function test in ocular healthy and in individuals with decreased visual function. Methods: Twenty six individuals were tested twice using the Landolt C-CSF test of the M&S Smart System contrast sensitivity testing system. The mean age of the study participants was 37.3 years (SD 15). The Landolt C-CSF was performed for 20/20, 20/40 and 20/80 and all measurements were obtained viewing the chart binocularly at a distance of nine feet. Repeatability was assessed using Altman and Bland plots and 95% limits of agreement were calculated. The difference in measurements of first and repeat measurements was assessed using paired samples t-test. Results: Table 1 below provides the mean of first and second measurements of obtained data using the Landolt C contrast sensitivity function test and the limits of agreement at different acuities. Overall the contrast sensitivity peak measured as percent contrast was very repeatable at different visual acuity levels with average difference in percent contrast being not significantly different in all three acuity levels (paired samples t-test p value 0.8, 0.9 and 0.9 respectively). Conclusions: The Landolt C-CSF test of the M&S Smart System contrast sensitivity testing system is repeatable and provides data that can be useful in measuring contrast levels. Further tweaks may be done to provide area under log CSF from the various peak contrast sensitivity tested. Commercial Relationships: Kaydee McCray, None; Violeta Paronian, None; Aaron Seitz, None; Pinakin G. Davey, None Support: NIH Grant 1R01EY023582 Program Number: 3903 Poster Board Number: D0045 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Assessment of Evans low contrast sensitivity in measuring log contrast sensitivity Pinakin G. Davey1, Raymond Maeda1, Aaron Seitz2. 1College of Optometry, Western University of Health Sciences, Pomona, CA; 2 Psychology, UC Riverside, Riverside, CA. Purpose: Clinically a quick and reliable estimate of contrast sensitivity levels is desirable to evaluate patients with decreased vision or difficulty of vision in dim illumination. The Evans Low Contrast Test (ELCT) chart presents Sloan Letters optotypes line size of 20/630 (a testing distance of 1m) varying contrast in sets of 3 letters of equal contrast level. All triplets are balanced for letter difficulty and differ in contrast by 0.15 log contrast step. The purpose of this study was to evaluate the short term repeatability of the ELCT in measuring lowest log contrast appreciated in healthy and eyes with ocular pathology. Methods: Study participants were tested at 13 feet in a dark environment and the retro illuminated box provided the background illumination for the ELCT chart. Twenty five study participants with either normal visual function or decreased visual function performed assessment with ELCT twice viewing the chart binocularly. Repeatability was assessed using Altman and Bland plots and 95% limits of agreement were calculated. Difference in measurements was assessed using paired samples t-test. Results: The mean log contrast of attempt 1 and attempt 2 was 2.198 and 2.208 (standard deviation 0.17 and 0.08 respectively). The mean difference between the groups was not statistically significant (paired samples t-test: t statistic =0.36; p value =0.72). The test was easy to perform and the results were obtained in a couple of minutes maximum. Conclusions: The Evans Low Contrast Test provides a quick and easy method of estimating lowest log contrast and may be useful test in identifying decreased contrast sensitivity in individuals with ocular pathology. Commercial Relationships: Pinakin G. Davey, None; Raymond Maeda, None; Aaron Seitz, None Support: NIH1R01EY023582 Program Number: 3904 Poster Board Number: D0046 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Assessment of the Impact of congenital dichromacy on the lives of color blind adults Amanda Bastos, Lívia Rego, Daniela Bonci, Dora F. Ventura, Mirella Gualtieri. Experimental Psychology, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. Purpose: Congenital color blindness affects 6 to 8% of the male caucasian population. Recent developments point to gene therapy as a possible treatment for this type of impairment. However, little is known regarding the impact of dyschromatopsia on people with congenital color blindness. Questionnaires assessing quality of life in vision research have been effective in revealing the impact of the visual losses. However, issues related to color vision are still largely absent from these instruments.The objective of this work is to evaluate the impact of congenital dyschromatopsia on the life of adults Methods: We investigated different dimensions of the impact of color blindness in everyday life, based on the analysis of the content of interviews - using the Sphinx software (Le Sphinx, UK). Twelve male participants were interviewed (age = 33.6 years old ± 9.9; educational level between undergraduate and graduate school). All participants underwent psychophysical color vision assessment – using the Cambridge Colour Test (Cambridge Research Systems, ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts UK) - as well as genotyping of the opsin genes (Gentra Systems - QUIAGEM, Dusseldorf, Germany). The results were compared to those from a control group for psychophysical assessment and genetics of color vision. (age = 27,1 years old ± 4,7) Results: Dyschromatopsias had direct negative impact on professional daily life activities in 27% of the individuals; 12% of the participants found to be color blind during childhood as a result of self-assessment; 22% considered that their vision was good; 60% revealed that life would be no different if they had no visual defect; 29% depended on someone else in their daily life and 30% would like to undergo treatment for color blindness cure. The psychophysical colour vision results revealed: Protan: 320 ± 129,61; Deutan: 684,45 ± 392,2 Tritan: 76,64 ± 24,3 and control group Protan: 50 ± 26,5 Deutan: 50,2 ± 16,9 e Tritan: 77,5 ± 26,9. Genetic analysis confirmed the psychophysical results Conclusions: Color blindness has an impact on individuals, as revealed by semi-structured interviews. However, the extent of the impact is not homogeneous among subjects most participants had different difficulties in daily life, mostly related to professional activity developed. A questionnaire being designed based on these results might constitute a tool for identifying individuals who are more strongly affected by congenital dyschromatopsia Commercial Relationships: Amanda Bastos, None; Lívia Rego, None; Daniela Bonci, None; Dora F. Ventura, None; Mirella Gualtieri, None Program Number: 3905 Poster Board Number: D0047 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Genetic analysis, the Color Assessment and Diagnosis (CAD) test, and the Cambridge Color Test (CCT) yield the same color vision classifications in humans Victoria Honnell2, 1, Daniela Bonci2, Mirella T. Barboni2, Mirella Gualtiere2, Amanda Bastos2, Lívia Rego2, Givago S. Souza2, Malinda E. Fitzgerald3, Luiz Carlos L. Silveira2, Dora F. Ventura2. 1 Neuroscience, Rhodes College, Memphis, TN; 2University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil; 3Christian Brothers University, Memphis, TN. Purpose: To determine whether the three color vision classification procedures produce identical results in trichromats and dichromats. Methods: Blood samples were analyzed from trichromat (26.5±5.2 years old, 5 males and 14 females) and dichromat (31.1±8.1 years old, 8 males) volunteers living in Sao Paulo, Brazil. The opsin genes, specifically Exon 5, of all subjects were sequenced after DNA extraction and PCR amplification procedures. The amino acids in place 277 and 285 on Exon 5 were identified and the visual pigment alleles were assessed. Each participant was also tested using the Cambridge Colour Test (CCT) and the Colour Assessement Diagnosis test (CAD) using the full assessment protocol (N=26). Results: 26 subjects were evaluated using psychophysical tests and genetic analysis. The subjects that self identified as daltonic (N=8) showed only one M or L allele through the genetic analysis. Seven of the eight dichromat patients were classified as deutan and only one subject was classified as protan by the psychophysical tests, CCT and CAD. Of the eight dichromats that had their genes sequenced, seven were classified as deutan and one was protan. The psychophysical test results match the genetic test results for each daltonic patient. The subjects that self identified as trichromats (N=18) had normal results for the genetic analysis, CCT, and CAD. Conclusions: Equal classifications were obtained with the three methods of color vision classification. This suggests that each testing method in itself is a reliable measure. Commercial Relationships: Victoria Honnell, None; Daniela Bonci, None; Mirella T. Barboni, None; Mirella Gualtiere, None; Amanda Bastos, None; Lívia Rego, None; Givago S. Souza, None; Malinda E. Fitzgerald, None; Luiz Carlos L. Silveira, None; Dora F. Ventura, None Support: MHIRT Grant NIH 2T37 MD001378-13 Program Number: 3906 Poster Board Number: D0048 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Neural Compensation for Color Deficiency: Binocular Enhancement of Cone-Specific Color VEPs Jeff C. Rabin, Dan Lam, Andrew Kryder. Optometry, UIW Rosenberg School of Optometry, San Antonio, TX. Purpose: Central nervous system (CNS) degeneration often precedes glaucomatous retinal changes suggesting that glaucoma is a CNS disease. Moreover, preservation of the binocular field in glaucoma as well as cognitive function in Alzheimer’s disease are mediated by CNS control.1,2 Our purpose was to determine if comparable CNS control mechanisms operate developmentally to optimize function in hereditary color vision deficiency (CVD) by comparing binocular to monocular cone specific color VEPs. Methods: Red (L), green (M) and blue (S) cone specific VEPs were recorded in pattern-onset mode with colored checkerboards on a grey background (L&M cone: 1 deg. checks, S cone: 2 deg., 2 onsets/ sec., Diagnosys LLC). Display luminance and CIE chromaticity were transformed to cone contrasts to selectively stimulate L, M and S cones. Subjects included 17 color vision normal (CVN) and 11 hereditary red or green CVDs confirmed to be CVD on a battery of tests. The ratio of binocular VEP amplitude (N1-P1) to mean (RE & LE) monocular amplitude was used to quantify enhancement. Results: CVDs showed binocular facilitation of VEP amplitude (enhancement >2X; mean = 3.1X) for the color corresponding to their CVD. Values exceeded enhancement for other cone types within CVDs (3.1X vs. 1.2X, p<0.003) and compared to CVNs (3.1X vs. 1.2X, p<0.007). Binocular facilitation of CVD VEPs remained high (2.4X) even when quantified as binocular amplitude/higher amplitude from right or left eyes (2.4X vs. 1.1X, p <0.008). Dichromatic CVDs did show an enhancement effect. Conclusions: Hereditary CVDs with anomalous trichromacy show binocular facilitation of VEPs for the color corresponding to their CVD. This suggests neural compensation for CVD similar to preservation of function in glaucoma and Alzheimer’s disease. Other congenital anomalies and rod-cone, macular and/or corneal dystrophies may be subject to neural compensation. Elucidation of underlying mechanisms could lead to new treatments for visually debilitating disease. 1 Crish et. al, PNAS 2010;107:5196–5201. 2Sponsel et. al TVST 2014;1-13. Commercial Relationships: Jeff C. Rabin, None; Dan Lam, None; Andrew Kryder, None Program Number: 3907 Poster Board Number: D0049 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Comparison of interpolation algorithms for static visual field data Travis Smith1, Ning Smith2, Richard G. Weleber1. 1Ophthalmology, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, OR; 2Center for Health Research, Kaiser Permanente, Portland, OR. Purpose: Static perimetry generates 3-D data (x-y test location and z sensitivity value) representing the visual field (VF), sometimes called the hill of vision (HOV), which is often sparsely sampled. Data interpolation produces a finer HOV representation to aid interpretation, visual display, and quantitative analysis. The goal of this study is to compare the accuracy of several scattered data interpolation algorithms and identify the optimal one for VF data. ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Methods: Full-field VF data was analyzed from 129 exams of 10 normal subjects and 10 retinitis pigmentosa (RP) patients that passed quality assessment. Data was acquired with the Octopus 900 with 164 radially oriented, centrally condensed test points using GATEi, target size V, and a 10 cd/m2 background. Repeated exams for each subject were included if obtained within 90 days of the first. Interpolation accuracy was assessed by the root mean square error (RMSE) and mean absolute error (MAE) from leave-one-out cross-validation (LOOCV) after blind spot removal. In LOOCV, each location’s z-value is interpolated from the other 163 points and compared with a target value to produce an error residual; this is repeated for all locations in each exam. Two types of target values were considered: the median z-value at each location across all exams for that eye (Target 1), and the measured z-value itself (Target 2). LOOCV was performed with the 8 nonparametric interpolation methods in the top row of Table 1. Significance was assessed by one-sided paired t-tests with Bonferroni correction. Results: Table 1 summarizes the interpolator performances. Linear radial basis function (RBF) interpolation had the smallest mean RMSE and MAE compared to all other methods for both target types, significant (p<0.006) in each case except those identified by * in Table 1. Linear RBF performance was significantly better in RP patients than in normals in all scenarios. Conclusions: Interpolation of static VF data was most accurate with a linear RBF kernel. Accuracy improved in subjects with visual field loss, likely due to higher spatial correlation in the data. Future work will assess parametric and regularized methods to mitigate overfitting, incorporate a larger number of exams, and analyze the influence of perimetric test grid density and target size on interpolation accuracy. Weleber, AGTC SAB (S), Foundation Fighting Blindness ESAB (F), U.S. patent no. 8657446, Method and apparatus for visual field monitoring, also known as Visual Field Modeling and Analysis (P) Support: Supported by an Unrestricted Grant from Research to Prevent Blindness, Foundation Fighting Blindness, and Hear See Hope Program Number: 3908 Poster Board Number: D0050 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Automated static threshold perimetry using a remote eye tracker Pete R. Jones1, 2, Sarah Kalwarowsky1, Gary S. Rubin1, 2, Marko Nardini3, 1. 1Institute of Ophthalmology, UCL, London, United Kingdom; 2NIHR Moorfields Biomedical Research Centre, London, United Kingdom; 3Department of Psychology, Durham University, Durham University, United Kingdom. Purpose: Current methods of Static Threshold Perimetry require (i) an explicit, button-press response (precluding testing of infants) and (ii) expensive, specialised equipment. Here we present a novel measure that addresses these problems by combining a cheap, commercially available, eye tracker (Tobii EyeX: $135), with an ordinary desktop computer. Methods: Luminance detection thresholds were measured monocularly in 7 healthy adults (additional data collection ongoing), using both a Humphrey Field Analyzer [HFA] and an automated remote eyetracking [ARE] procedure (Fig 1A). The eye tracker was used to present stimuli relative to the current point of fixation, and to assess whether the participant made an eye-movement towards the stimulus. In both tests, Goldman III stimuli were arranged on a 24-2 grid, and were presented individually against a 10 cd/m2 white background. Participants completed each test twice (same eye) in order to assess test-retest reliability. Results: The pointwise Coefficient of Repeatability was similar for the two tests (ARE: 8.1 dB. HFA: 6.3 dB). Differences in mean sensitivity to stimuli in the central 10° and those located more peripherally (10—24°) were observed in both the ARE (CI95% = 0.9—2.7 dB) and the HFA (CI95% = 2.3—4.6 dB). Furthermore, as shown in Fig 1B, the ARE was able to differentiate between the blind spot and surrounding retinal locations (t6 = -3.1, p = 0.021). Conclusions: An eye tracker can be used to perform Static Threshold Perimetry based on eye movement responses alone. The ARE was sensitive to normal variations in sensitivity across the healthy eye, and could isolate the blind spot. It may therefore be capable of detecting visual field deficits, including acute scotomas. Its low price and ease of use could make such a test particularly effective as a means of screening infants. Mean RMSE and mean MAE values (both in dB) across all exams for each interpolator analyzed, as assessed by LOOCV. Commercial Relationships: Travis Smith, Foundation Fighting Blindness (F), Hear See Hope (F); Ning Smith, None; Richard G. ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org. ARVO 2015 Annual Meeting Abstracts Fig 1. (A) Example trial for the ARE. Here a test point is presented at [-3° -6°], relative to the current point of fixation. Fixation at trial onset was unconstrained. The monitor was a Samsung 305T LCD, gain-corrected in software for uniformity. (B) Mean threshold data from the ARE, computed from 4 participants tested with their left eye. Numbers show the final threshold estimate at each location, in dB (higher = more sensitive). Note that the blind spot was measured in an identical manner to all other points, with no prior assumptions or constraints, in order to simulate an unknown scotoma. Commercial Relationships: Pete R. Jones, None; Sarah Kalwarowsky, None; Gary S. Rubin, None; Marko Nardini, None Support: This work was supported by Fight for Sight, the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre at Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, the Special Trustees of Moorfields Eye Hospital, and the Leverhulme Trust. Results: Univariate correlation analysis revealed test- retest SD was significantly correlated (p < 0.002) with the eccentricity, visual field threshold, Spatial SD, and SI. Test- retest SD showed strongest correlation with visual field threshold (p < 0.002; r = -0.342) and Spatial SD (p < 0.002; r = 0.452). Multivariate regression analysis showed test- retest SD was affected by visual field threshold (p < 0.01; t-value 5.05) and Spatial SD (p < 0.01; t-value 12.5). Conclusions: Visual field threshold fluctuation was mostly affected by visual field threshold and Spatial SD. Commercial Relationships: Takuya Numata, None; Chota Matsumoto, None; Sachiko Okuyama, None; Fumi Tanabe, None; Shigeki Hashimoto, None; Mariko Eura, None; Tomoyasu Kayazawa, None; Sayaka Yamao, None; Yoshikazu Shimomura, None; Ted Maddess, None Program Number: 3909 Poster Board Number: D0051 Presentation Time: 3:45 PM–5:30 PM Influence of visual field threshold fluctuation on high resolution perimetry with 0.5-degree interval Takuya Numata1, Chota Matsumoto1, Sachiko Okuyama1, Fumi Tanabe1, Shigeki Hashimoto1, Mariko Eura1, Tomoyasu Kayazawa2, Sayaka Yamao1, Yoshikazu Shimomura1, Ted Maddess3. 1 Ophthalmology, Kinki Univ Faculty of Medicine, Osaka-Sayama City, Japan; 2Ophthalmology, Kinki University Faculty of Medicine, Nara Hospital, Ikoma, Japan; 3Biology & Environment, Australian National University College of Medicine, Canberra, ACT, Australia. Purpose: It is well known that visual field threshold can fluctuate. The fluctuation is small where the visual field threshold is small, and the fluctuation is large where it is large. The visual field threshold also fluctuates at localized areas such as scotoma. We performed an observational clinical study using high resolution perimetry with 0.5-degree interval to investigate the factors for the visual field threshold fluctuation. Methods: Sixteen eyes of 16 patients with glaucoma (mean age: 63.1 ± 5.9) were studied. Octopus 900 Custom test program was used with target size III and background luminance of 31.4 asb to measure the sensitivity on the upper temporal meridian of 45 degrees from the fixation point to the eccentricity of 30 degrees with the interval of 0.5 degrees. The sensitivity was evaluated 3 times at each point. Visual field threshold was determined by computing the mean sensitivity of the 3 repeats at each test point. The standard deviation was defined as test- retest SD. In order to evaluate local visual field threshold fluctuation at scotoma or else, we also computed Spatial SD along each sample line using a moving window of between 3 and 20 points wide. In addition, we computed the independent variable Spatial Interaction (SI) multiplying visual field threshold by Spatial SD at each point. Using univariate correlation analysis, associations were examined between test- retest SD and visual field threshold, Spatial SD, SI, and the eccentricity. We also did a multivariate regression analysis to look for the independent factors that best determined test- retest SD. ©2015, Copyright by the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc., all rights reserved. Go to iovs.org to access the version of record. For permission to reproduce any abstract, contact the ARVO Office at pubs@arvo.org.