doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01629.x Evaluation of offspring size–number invariants in 12 species of lizard T. ULLER,*, G. M. WHILE,à E. WAPSTRA,à D. A. WARNER,§,– B. A. GOODMAN,** L. SCHWARZKOPF,** T. LANGKILDE,§, P. DOUGHTY,àà R. S. RADDER,§ D. H. ROHR,§§ C. M. BULL,–– R. SHINE§ & M. OLSSON *Department of Zoology, Edward Grey Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK School of Biological Sciences, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW, Australia àSchool of Zoology, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Tas., Australia §School of Biological Sciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia –Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Organismal Biology, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, USA **School of Marine and Tropical Biology, James Cook University, Townsville, Qld, Australia Biology Department, Penn State University, University Park, PA, USA ààDepartment of Terrestrial Zoology, Museum of Western Australia, Perth, WA, Australia §§Applied Ecological Research P ⁄ L, Melbourne, Vic., Australia ––School of Biological Sciences, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia Keywords: Abstract life history; lizards; oviparous; resource allocation; size–number invariants; viviparous. The optimal division of resources into offspring size vs. number is one of the classic problems in life-history evolution. Importantly, models that take into account the discrete nature of resource division at low clutch sizes suggest that the variance in offspring size should decline with increasing clutch size according to an invariant relationship. We tested this prediction in 12 species of lizard with small clutch sizes. Contrary to expectations, not all species showed a negative relationship between variance in offspring size and clutch size, and the pattern significantly deviated from quantitative predictions in five of the 12 species. We suggest that the main limitation of current size–number models for small clutch sizes is that they rely on assumptions of hierarchical allocation strategies with independence between allocation decisions. Indeed, selection may favour alternative mechanisms of reproductive allocation that avoid suboptimal allocation imposed by the indivisible fraction at low clutch sizes. Introduction Life-history theory is one of the cornerstones of evolutionary biology. Its goal is to explain why organisms differ in the timing of life-history events such as the onset of breeding, reproductive investment and patterns of senescence (reviewed in Roff, 1992, 2002; Stearns, 1992). For mathematical convenience, most theoretical optimality models rely upon a continuous function to describe the relationships between variables. However, many interesting biological phenomena do not easily lend themselves to the assumptions of continuity and need to be modelled using a discrete approach (DownCorrespondence: T. Uller, Department of Zoology, Edward Grey Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK. Tel.: +44(0) 1865 281194; fax: +44(0) 1865 271168; e-mail: tobias.uller@zoo.ox.ac.uk hower & Charnov, 1998). For example, in species with small clutch sizes, the size–number trade-off only approximates continuous functions for relatively large clutch sizes. Thus, a female with a resource availability that is intermediate to that required to produce two or three eggs can either produce two eggs of larger than optimum size or three eggs of smaller than optimum size (Ebert, 1994; Charnov et al., 1995). This has a number of potential consequences. For example, it implies that the ‘optimal’ division of resources is not available for most values of total reproductive effort, and this could have consequences for the relationship between offspring size and number (Ebert, 1994; Charnov et al., 1995; Downhower & Charnov, 1998). Small clutch sizes are very common throughout the animal kingdom. For example, a substantial proportion of lizards has a clutch size below 8 (Fitch, 1970). Thus, most size–number models are of doubtful applicability for ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 143 T. ULLER ET AL. these species. However, models for small clutch sizes have been developed. Early discussions of the problem by Ricklefs (1968) and Nussbaum (1981) were formalized by Ebert (1994) and Charnov and co-workers (Charnov & Downhower, 1995; Charnov et al., 1995; Downhower & Charnov, 1998). Under the assumption that animals differ in the amount of resources that are available for reproduction, these models suggest that the variance in offspring size in a population should decrease with increasing clutch size. This pattern arises because, as clutch size increases, the total reproductive effort can be divided into offspring sizes that are closer to the ‘optimal’ value. Furthermore, Charnov et al.’s (1995) model specifically predicts that variance in offspring size will follow a simple, invariant, relationship with clutch size (i.e. independent of model parameters, such as functions relating offspring size to survival; reviewed in Charnov, 1993). We here reiterate the most important aspects of their model (Charnov & Downhower, 1995; further discussions can also be found in Guinnee et al., 2004; Kasparian et al., 2005; Uller & Olsson, in press). Provided that a number of assumptions hold (Table 1), the models predict that the mean offspring size should be independent of clutch size. However, the range in offspring size should decline with clutch size. More specifically, the ratio of the ranges of investment per offspring at clutch size n + 1 and n should be inversely proportional to the ratio of clutch sizes, i.e. Imax nþ1 Imin nþ1 Cn ¼ : Imax n Imin n Cnþ1 ð1Þ Thus, the maximum and minimum per-offspring investment at each clutch size should converge upon the optimal level of investment per offspring (Fig. 1). Empirical tests of this prediction are restricted to natural or laboratory populations of some species of parasitic wasps (West et al., 2001; Guinnee et al., 2005), one mammal (Kasparian et al., 2005), one fish (Charnov et al., 1995), one lizard (Uller & Olsson, in press), and an experimental approach on one clone of Daphnia (Guinnee et al., 2004). Whereas all studies found a decrease in Table 1 Key assumptions for the Charnov et al. (1995) small clutch size model. Maternal control over resource allocation Resources available for reproduction are fixed for a given female Resources should represent an individual’s total reproductive investment at each given time of reproduction Conversion of resources into per-offspring investment is linear Selection favors a single optimal offspring size (e.g. independently of clutch size) Females do (or can) not vary the per-offspring investment within clutches No lower or upper limit on per-offspring investment References: Charnov & Downhower, 1995; Charnov et al., 1995; Downhower & Charnov, 1998; Guinnee et al., 2004; Kasparian et al., 2005; Uller & Olsson, in press. Offspring size 144 1 2 3 4 5 Clutch size Fig. 1 Predicted relationship between clutch size and the range in average per offspring investment (e.g. offspring mass). Figure modified from Charnov et al. (1995). variance in offspring size with increased clutch size, the fit to eqn 1 was relatively poor in most cases. However, many of these species are likely, or have been shown, to violate one or more assumptions (e.g. mammals; Kasparian et al., 2005). Furthermore, there is only one published study that is set in a comparative framework (Guinnee et al., 2005), despite that this could substantially further our understanding of the models’ accuracy in predicting size–number patterns. For example, if species differ in at least one trait that is likely to violate assumptions to a lesser or greater degree, a comparative approach can constitute a direct test of the role of specific assumptions for poor fit to theory or, at the least, suggest some reasons for any observed among-species variation. Here, we test the fit to the invariant relationship described in eqn 1 using data from 12 species of Australian lizards with clutch sizes in the range of 1–9. Materials and methods Brief overview of lizard reproduction Most lizards are oviparous. Eggs are soft-shelled and are either buried in soil or sand or attached to the substrate. However, approximately 20% of the world’s >4000 lizard species are viviparous with viviparity having evolved independently around 100 times among squamates (reviewed in Blackburn, 1982; Shine, 1985; Pough et al., 2001). The degree of placental structure differs substantially among species (Weekes, 1935). Those with no or very limited post-ovulatory exchange between mother and offspring are frequently referred to as ovoviviparous. However, most species are not sufficiently studied to allow any detailed classification regarding the degree of transfer across the placenta and we therefore limit ourselves to the classification oviparous vs. viviparous. These two categories could differ in a number of ways that could influence the degree to which they fit the assumptions of the models, in particular the extent to which offspring size is affected by maternal effects post-ovulation (see Discussion). Thus, we may expect ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 145 Tests of size–number invariants in lizards Table 2 Data summary and references to primary literature on the Materials and methods used to catch, house and measure reproductive traits for each of the 12 lizard species. Species Reproductive mode No. clutches Clutch size range Eulamprus tympanum Viviparous 356 1–5 Eulamprus heatwolei Eulamprus quoyii Tiliqua rugosa Egernia whitii Niveoscincus microlepidotus Niveoscincus ocellatus Lampropholis mirabilis Bassiana duperreyi Amphibolurus muricatus Ctenophorus pictus Ctenophorus fordi Viviparous Viviparous Viviparous Viviparous Viviparous Viviparous Oviparous Oviparous Oviparous Oviparous Oviparous 53 35 41 106 148 558 34 62 122 143 270 1–4 4–8 1–3 1–3 1–5 2–6 2–4 5–9 3–8 3–6 2–5 oviparous lizards to more closely follow the predictions of the Charnov et al. (1995) models. Data collection We obtained data on 12 Australian lizards (Table 2), five oviparous and seven viviparous. In most studies, animals from natural populations were brought into the laboratory to oviposit or give birth just before oviposition or parturition. The holding facilities differ between species and studies and we direct the reader to key papers in the primary literature for further information for each species (Table 2). We exclusively used offspring mass at hatching or parturition as our dependent variable and consistently pooled data within species whenever possible to increase sample sizes per species. The likelihood that this should cause error is small considering that the prediction from eqn 1 is independent of variation in assumptions regarding trade-off functions and offspring survival (that could fluctuate across time and space) and because measures of mass should show limited variation across settings (under the assumptions that balances are accurately calibrated). The prediction that mean offspring mass should not be correlated with clutch size was tested by carrying out a Spearman’s rank correlation on mean offspring mass (per clutch) and clutch size. We tested for a convergence of maximum and minimum mean offspring mass by applying the ordered heterogeneity (OH) test (Rice & Gaines, 1994) following a Bartlett’s test of homogeneity of variance (following Guinnee et al., 2004). This allowed us to evaluate hypotheses regarding the increase (or decrease) in the variation in mean offspring mass with increasing clutch size in the context of a nondirectional test of the difference in the variation in mean offspring mass between clutch sizes (Rice & Gaines, 1994). Finally, the main prediction of an invariant relationship between the range in offspring mass and clutch size was tested by Mean offspring mass (g) 0.81 ± 0.01 0.98 0.94 101.53 1.36 0.36 0.59 0.16 0.34 1.16 0.96 0.59 ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ± ± 0.01 0.02 2.75 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 References Schwarzkopf (1992), Rohr (1997), Doughty & Shine (1997) Langkilde et al. (2005) Schwarzkopf (2005) Bull et al. (1993) While et al. (2007) Olsson & Shine (1998, 1999), Shine & Olsson (2003) Wapstra et al. (1999), Wapstra & Swain (2001) B. Goodman, unpublished Radder & Shine (2007a,b) Warner & Shine (2006, 2007) Uller et al. (2006) Uller & Olsson (2006, in press) calculating the left-hand side of eqn 1, comparing it with the theoretical predictions for clutch sizes of {n + 1,n} for each of the species, and testing if predictions were within the 95% confidence interval. Confidence intervals of the observed values were estimated using bootstrap with 10 000 random sample events with replacement (Poptools add-ins for Microsoft Excel, Hood, 2002). We used the smallest of the sample sizes of the two clutch sizes to avoid bias due to differences in sample size (see e.g. West et al., 2001). Results Mean offspring mass differed significantly among clutch sizes for most species (Table 3). Data for two of the five oviparous and for four of the seven viviparous species Table 3 Results from a Spearman’s rank order correlation, testing the prediction that mean offspring mass is independent of clutch size, and a fixed factor analysis of variance, testing for significant differences in offspring mass (dependent variable) across clutch sizes (independent variable), for each of the 12 lizard species. Correlation ANOVA Species rs P-value F Eulamprus tympanum Eulamprus heatwolei Eulamprus quoyii Tiliqua rugosa Egernia whitii Niveoscincus microlepidotus Niveoscincus ocellatus Lampropholis mirabilis Bassiana duperreyi Amphibolurus muricatus Ctenophorus pictus Ctenophorus fordi )0.16 )0.32 )0.28 )0.47 )0.40 0.27 )0.19 )0.35 )0.09 )0.28 )0.10 )0.06 < 0.01 0.02 0.08 < 0.01 < 0.01 < 0.01 < 0.01 0.04 0.48 < 0.01 0.22 0.30 F4,351 = 2.18 F3,49 = 3.84 F5,33 = 1.52 F2,38 = 6.04 F2,103 = 10.61 F4,143 = 9.03 F4,553 = 7.47 F2,31 = 2.59 F4,57 = 3.89 F5,116 = 2.47 F3,139 = 0.98 F3,265 = 0.95 ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY P-value < < < < < 0.07 0.01 0.21 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.01 0.09 0.01 0.04 0.40 0.41 146 T. ULLER ET AL. Table 4 Results of the ordered heterogeneity test of a decrease in variation in average offspring mass with clutch size for each of the 12 lizard species. Species N Pc rs rsPc P-value Eulamprus tympanum Eulamprus heatwolei Eulamprus quoyii Tiliqua rugosa Egernia whitii Niveoscincus microlepidotus Niveoscincus ocellatus Lampropholis mirabilis Bassiana duperreyi Amphibolurus muricatus Ctenophorus pictus Ctenophorus fordi 5 4 6 3 3 5 5 3 5 6 4 4 0.8375 0.7023 0.9237 0.0926 0.9967 0.9999 0.9870 0.8312 0.1297 0.9291 0.8958 0.9997 )0.70 )0.40 )0.94 )0.50 )1.00 0.60 )1.00 0.50 )1.00 )0.95 )0.80 )1.00 )0.5863 )0.2809 )0.8683 )0.0463 )0.9967 0.5999 )0.9870 0.4156 )0.1297 )0.8826 )0.7166 )0.9997 NS NS ** NS ** NS *** NS NS ** * *** NS, P > 0.05, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. N is the number of clutch sizes for each species. Pc is the complement of the P-value from the Bartlett’s test of homogeneity of variance (i.e. Pc = 1 ) PBartlett). rs is the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient. failed to show the convergence of the maximum and minimum mean offspring mass (Table 4). The confidence intervals were very large for some species for which we had limited data, in particular at low and high clutch sizes. However, it is clear that the fit to quantitative predictions was poor, with the predicted value falling outside of the confidence interval for at least one test of eqn 1 for five of the 12 species (Appendix 1). This is clearly seen from plots of the means and 10th and 90th percentiles for each clutch size for each species (Fig. 2), showing that several species failed to show convergence and, contrary to the most fundamental prediction, some species actually showed an increase in the variation in offspring mass with increasing clutch size. Discussion Our evaluation of invariant offspring size–number relationships in 12 species of lizards showed an overall weak fit to theoretical expectations. In particular, almost half of the species had at least one test where the confidence interval did not include the ratio predicted by eqn 1. Thus, our results provide an important extension of previous single-species tests and strongly suggest that the Charnov et al. (1995) model does not capture the complexities of size–number strategies in lizards with small clutch sizes. We provide an overview of the most likely reasons for the lack of fit and their implications for the evolution of reproductive investment. When models fail to capture reality When optimality models fail to predict empirical data, one or several assumptions are likely to be incorrect or too simplistic. The critical assumptions of the small clutch size models are listed in Table 1 in the Introduction. We will discuss what we consider to be the most problematic when applying these to lizards, but our discussion has applicability also to other taxa. Virtually all size–number models, including the ones for small clutch sizes, assume that reproductive investment is a step-wise optimization problem with independence of decisions at each step. In other words, females first decide on how much to invest into reproduction, and secondly, how to divide these resources into offspring size vs. number (additional steps may also occur, including whether to produce sons or daughters, Hardy, 2002; Wapstra et al., 2007). Thus, because the available resource pool is fixed when size–number allocation occurs, employing a certain clutch size strategy necessarily generates a mean offspring size and vice versa (Smith & Fretwell, 1974; reviewed in Roff, 1992). There are several problems with the logic of this approach. First, total allocation to reproduction may not be independent of the division of resources to offspring size or number. For example, if allocation occurs continuously during embryonic development, as in mammals, allocation to individual offspring (and hence total allocation) can continue once clutch size has been fixed. More generally, resource allocation to offspring in direct response to clutch size, or vice versa, could reduce the variation in offspring size at low clutch sizes. Indeed, we argue that this particular allocation strategy should be favoured when clutch sizes are small, as it would allow better precision and avoid suboptimal allocation (i.e. avoid producing unnecessarily small or large offspring) by allowing flexible adjustment of total allocation directly in response to clutch size. Thus, this allocation mechanism would to some extent resolve the dilemma imposed by the integer change-over problem (sensu Downhower & Charnov, 1998) that a fixed, hierarchical, allocation strategy would incur. Indeed, a model based on continuous functions that linked total reproductive effort and per-offspring investment found that changes in parameter values had consequences for both the total reproductive effort and the per-offspring investment (Winkler & Wallin, 1987; Caley et al., 2001). Thus, it would be useful to explore similar models that explore the importance of flexibility in allocation decisions for discrete cases. The above reasoning suggests that species with continuous allocation throughout offspring development, such as mammals, should show a poorer fit than would species that more closely follow the standard allocation assumption (fixed total reproductive effort and subsequent size–number decision). Many reptiles, including lizards, rely on stored resources for allocation (i.e. they are capital rather than income breeders; Schwarzkopf, 1994; Bonnet et al., 1998), suggesting that they do conform to the assumption of a fixed pool of resources. Thus, it would be interesting to compare data from mammals with a similar range of litter sizes to our data ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 147 Tests of size–number invariants in lizards 1.1 1.2 Eulamprus tympanum 1.0 Eulamprus heatwolei 1.1 0.9 1.0 0.8 0.9 0.7 0.6 0.8 1 1.4 2 3 4 5 Eulamprus quoyii 1.2 110 1.0 90 0.8 70 0.6 50 4 5 6 1.8 7 2 3 4 Tiliqua rugosa 1 8 Egernia whitii 1.6 Average offspring mass (g) 1 130 0.8 2 3 Niveoscincus microlepidotus 0.6 1.4 0.4 1.2 1.0 0.2 1 2 1 3 0.7 0.3 0.6 0.2 0.5 2 3 4 5 Lampropholis mirabilis 0.1 Niveoscincus ocellatus 0.4 0.0 2 0.5 3 4 5 6 2 1.6 Bassiana duperreyi 3 4 Amphibolurus muricatus 1.4 0.4 1.2 0.3 0.2 1.0 5 6 7 1.2 Fig. 2 Means and 10th and 90th percentiles of average offspring mass for each clutch size for each of the 12 species of lizard. See Appendix 1 for sample sizes. Scales on y-axes differ due to variation in offspring mass between species. 8 0.8 9 Ctenophorus pictus 1.1 0.7 1.0 0.6 0.9 0.5 0.8 3 4 5 Clutch size on reptiles. Unfortunately, only one mammal has been investigated in this respect (Cavia aperea; data deviated significantly from predictions; Kasparian et al., 2005). Alternatively, as oviparous and viviparous reptiles differ in their potential for continuous maternal allocation throughout offspring development, comparisons between the two reproductive modes may also give us valuable insights into the importance of a fixed pool of resources. Our data showed that many viviparous 6 3 4 0.8 0.4 5 6 7 8 Ctenophorus fordi 2 3 4 5 Clutch size (43%) and oviparous (40%) species had confidence intervals that did not include the predicted value from eqn 1. The majority of viviparous lizards also failed to show a general convergence of maximum and minimum offspring mass with increasing clutch size, whereas the majority of oviparous species did show significant convergence. However, this difference could be driven also by alternative factors (see below) and data for more species would be required to accurately ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 148 T. ULLER ET AL. test for a quantitative difference between reproductive modes, preferably controlling for phylogeny. Furthermore, the majority of viviparous lizards only have limited resource allocation to offspring post-ovulation (Thompson et al., 2001; Thompson & Speake, 2006), which make them less suitable for evaluating the role of fixed vs. nonfixed per-offspring allocation at the timing of clutch size decisions. However, post-ovulatory offspring provisioning is not necessary to cause nonindependence between allocation steps. For example, studies of the lizard Uta stansburiana suggest that egg size and number are physiologically, and potentially genetically, linked (Sinervo & Licht, 1991), which would cause a similar link between total reproductive effort and the size–number strategies (see also Caley et al., 2001; Fischer et al., 2006). Deviations from model predictions could also arise if either egg size or clutch size is constrained within females (Christians, 2002) or if clutch size directly affects offspring size, for example, via differential uptake of water during incubation (reviewed in Brown & Shine, in press). Nevertheless, we argue that post-ovulatory maternal effects on offspring size could explain deviations from model predictions for at least some of the species in this study. Importantly, maternal effects on offspring size driven by female basking during pregnancy are common in viviparous lizards and documented in several of the species (or closely related species) included in this data set (e.g. Shine & Harlow, 1993; Swain & Jones, 2000; Wapstra, 2000; Caley & Schwarzkopf, 2004). Unfortunately, at present too few lizards with low clutch sizes seem to have been studied in sufficient detail to generate the large data sets required to test invariant predictions, which precludes a detailed comparative analysis. Experimental manipulations could potentially also directly address the importance of specific assumptions in generating deviations from model predictions, although this approach will only be feasible for a limited number of species. The small clutch size model tested here also assumes that selection on offspring size does not differ within clutches and across clutch sizes. The first assumption is doubtful in species with selection for differential sex allocation (Wapstra et al., 2007), but evidence for sexspecific offspring size has not been clearly documented in the present species (but see Langkilde & Shine, 2005). Finally, physical or physiological constraints may limit the lower and upper size of offspring. For example, the size of the pelvic girdle may constrain maximum offspring size, as has been described in turtles (Congdon & Gibbons, 1987) and been suggested for lizards (Sinervo & Licht, 1991; Ji et al., 2006). Alternatively, there may be a minimum size below which offspring are unviable (Charnov et al., 1995). These patterns would be evident as asymmetries in the convergence of the 10th and 90th percentiles in Fig. 1. Although results are qualitative rather than quantitative, there is little evidence for wide- spread asymmetric convergence. In fact, convergence was poor overall as supported by both the OH test and the test of eqn 1 (summarized in Table 4 and Appendix 1). Thus, we suggest that the assumption of hierarchical, and independent, allocation steps is a more serious problem than the existence of upper and lower limits on offspring mass for the deviations from invariant predictions in lizards. Conclusions Our data represent the most comprehensive evaluation of invariant predictions from size–number models for species with small clutch sizes. There is, however, a general agreement among this and previous tests that the discrete models fail to capture the complexities of offspring size–number strategies. We have argued that the most likely reason is that there exists greater flexibility and links between different steps of resource allocation than is allowed by this, and most other, optimality models. This would violate assumptions of a hierarchical allocation sequence and the independence among allocation decisions. We agree with several other workers (e.g. Christians, 2002; Glazier, 2002; Williams, 2005) that a better integration of mechanisms of allocation with life-history theory could provide insights into the evolution of reproductive allocation. Despite that it is over three decades since the publication of one of the most influential papers in this field (Smith & Fretwell, 1974), our understanding of the evolution of the division of resources into offspring size and number remains surprisingly incomplete. We argue that the evidence is strong that current offspring size– number models for small clutch sizes fail to capture empirical patterns and therefore must be revised. Invariants may be less common in nature than is frequently believed (Nee et al., 2005; Savage et al., 2006). Acknowledgments This paper is testimony to the positive attitude towards data sharing and collaborations that is the hallmark of herpetological research in Australia and elsewhere. We dedicate this paper to one of its authors, Raju Radder, who died while the work was in progress. Raju was an inspiration to us all, and his generosity of spirit, enthusiasm and insights will be sorely missed by his colleagues. We emphasize that important research is conducted on many more Australian lizard species than the ones that met the requirements for inclusion in this paper and apologize for any unintended omissions. Two anonymous reviewers provided valuable comments on the manuscript. Financial support was provided by the Wenner-Gren Foundations (TU), a James King of Irrawang Traveling Scholarship (TL) and the Australian Research Council (TU, EW, CMB, RS, MO). ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY Tests of size–number invariants in lizards References Blackburn, G.G. 1982. Evolutionary origins of viviparity in the reptilia I. Sauria. Amphib-Reptil. 3: 185–205. Bonnet, X., Bradshaw, S.D. & Shine, R. 1998. Income versus capital breeding: an ectothermic perspective. Oikos 83: 333–341. Brown, G.P. & Shine, R. Beyond size-number trade-offs: clutch size as a maternal effect. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B in press. Bull, C.M., Pamula, Y. & Schulze, L. 1993. Parturition in the sleepy lizard, Tiliqua rugosa. J. Herpetol. 27: 489–492. Caley, M.J. & Schwarzkopf, L. 2004. Complex growth rate evolution in a latitudinally widespread species. Evolution 58: 862–869. Caley, M.J., Schwarzkopf, L. & Shine, R. 2001. Does total reproductive effort evolve independently of offspring size? Evolution 55: 1245–1248. Charnov, E.L. 1993. Life History Invariants. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Charnov, E.L. & Downhower, J.F. 1995. A trade-off invariant life-history rule for optimal offspring size. Nature 376: 418– 419. Charnov, E.L., Downhower, J.F. & Brown, L.P. 1995. A trade-off invariant life-history rule for optimal offspring size in small litters. Evol. Ecol. 9: 57–63. Christians, J.K. 2002. Avian egg size: variation within species and inflexibility within individuals. Biol. Rev. 77: 1–26. Congdon, J.D. & Gibbons, J.W. 1987. Morphological constraint on egg size: a challenge to optimal egg size theory? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 84: 4145–4147. Doughty, P. & Shine, R. 1997. Detecting life history trade-offs: measuring energy stores in capital breeders reveals costs of reproduction. Oecologia 110: 508–513. Downhower, J.F. & Charnov, E.L. 1998. A resource range invariance rule for optimal offspring size predicts patterns of variability in parental phenotype. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 95: 6208–6211. Ebert, D. 1994. Fractional resource allocation into few eggs: Daphnia as an example. Ecology 75: 568–571. Fischer, K., Bot, A.N.M., Brakefield, P.M. & Zwaan, B.J. 2006. Do mothers producing large offspring have to sacrifice fecundity? J. Evol. Biol. 19: 380–391. Fitch, H.S. 1970. Reproductive cycles of lizards and snakes, Vol. 52. University of Kansas, Museum of Natural History, Miscellaneous Publication, Kansas, USA. Glazier, D. 2002. Resource-allocation rules and the heritability of traits. Evolution 56: 1696–1700. Guinnee, M.A., West, S.A. & Little, T.J. 2004. Testing small clutch size models with Daphnia. Am. Nat. 163: 880–887. Guinnee, M.A., Bernal, J.S., Bezemer, T.M., Fidgen, J.G., Hardy, I.C.W., Mayhew, P.J., Mills, N.J. & West, S.A. 2005. Testing predictions of small brood size models using parasitoid wasps. Evol. Ecol. Res. 7: 779–794. Hardy, I.C. 2002. Sex Ratios: Concepts and Research Methods. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Hood, G. 2002. Poptools, v. 2.5. Pest Animal Control Cooperative Research Centre, Wildlife and Ecology. CSIRO, Canberra, Australia. Available at: http://www.cse.csiro.au/poptools. Ji, X., Du, W.G., Li, H. & Lin, L.H. 2006. Experimentally reducing clutch size reveals a fixed upper limit to egg size in snakes, evidence from the king ratsnake, Elaphe carinata. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 144: 474–478. 149 Kasparian, K., Geißler, E. & Trillmich, F. 2005. Optimal offspring size in a small mammal: an exception to the tradeoff invariant life-history rule. Oikos 111: 271–278. Langkilde, T. & Shine, R. 2005. Different optimal offspring sizes for sons versus daughters may favor the evolution of temperature-dependent sex determination in viviparous lizards. Evolution 59: 2275–2280. Langkilde, T., Lance, V.A. & Shine, R. 2005. Ecological consequences of agonistic interactions in lizards. Ecology 86: 1650– 1659. Nee, S., Colegrave, N., West, S.A. & Grafen, A. 2005. The illusion of invariant quantities in life histories. Science 309: 1236–1239. Nussbaum, R.A. 1981. Seasonal shifts in clutch size and egg size in the side-blotched lizard, Uta stansburiana Baird and Girard. Oecologia 49: 8–13. Olsson, M. & Shine, R. 1998. Timing of parturition as a maternal care tactic in an alpine lizard species. Evolution 52: 1861–1864. Olsson, M. & Shine, R. 1999. Plasticity in the frequency of reproduction in an alpine lizard, Niveoscincus microlepidotus. Copeia 3: 794–796. Pough, F.H., Andrews, R.M., Cadle, J.E., Crump, M.L., Savitzky, A.H. & Wells, K.D. 2001. Herpetology, 2nd edn. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River. Radder, R.S. & Shine, R. 2007a. Sex Biased hatching asynchrony in an oviparous lizard (Bassiana duperreyi, Scincidae). Aust. Ecol. 32: 502–508. Radder, R.S. & Shine, R. 2007b. Why do female lizards lay their eggs in communal nests? J. Anim. Ecol. 76: 881–887. Rice, W.R. & Gaines, S.D. 1994. Extending nondirectional heterogeneity tests to evaluate simply ordered alternatives. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 91: 225–226. Ricklefs, R.E. 1968. On the limitation of brood size in passerine birds by the ability of adults to nourish their young. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 61: 847–851. Roff, D.A. 1992. The Evolution of Life Histories: Theory and Analysis. Chapman and Hall, New York. Roff, D.A. 2002. Life History Evolution. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland. Rohr, D.H. 1997. Demographic and life history variation in two proximate populations of a viviparous skink separated by a steep altitudinal gradient. J. Anim. Ecol. 66: 567–578. Savage, V.M., White, E.P., Moses, M.E., Ernest, S.K.M., Enquist, B.J. & Charnov, E.L. 2006. Comment on ‘‘The illusion of invariant quantities in life histories’’. Science 312: 198. Schwarzkopf, L. 1992. Annual variation in litter size and offspring size in a viviparous skink. Herpetologica 48: 390–395. Schwarzkopf, L. 1994. Measuring trade-offs: a review of costs of reproduction in lizards. In: Lizard Ecology Historical and Experimental Perspectives (L.J. Vitt & E.R. Pianka, eds), pp. 7–30. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Schwarzkopf, L. 2005. Sexual dimorphism in body shape without sexual dimorphism in body size in water skinks (Eulamprus qyouii). Herpetologica 61: 116–123. Shine, R. 1985. The evolution of viviparity in reptiles: an ecological analysis. In: The Biology of Reptilia, Volume 15 (C. Gans & F. Billit, eds), pp. 605–694. Jon Wiley and Sons, New York. Shine, R. & Harlow, P. 1993. Maternal thermoregulation influences offspring viability in a viviparous lizard. Oecologia 96: 122–127. Shine, R. & Olsson, M. 2003. When to be born? Prolonged pregnancy or incubation enhances locomotor performance in neonatal lizards (Scincidae). J. Evol. Biol. 16: 823–832. ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY 150 T. ULLER ET AL. Sinervo, B. & Licht, P. 1991. Proximate constraints on the evolution of egg size, number, and total clutch mass in lizards. Science 252: 1300–1302. Smith, C.C. & Fretwell, S.D. 1974. The optimal balance between size and number of offspring. Am. Nat. 108: 499–506. Stearns, S.C. 1992. Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Swain, R. & Jones, S.M. 2000. Maternal conditions associated with gestation conditions in a viviparous lizards, Niveoscincus metallicus. Herpetol. Monogr. 14: 432–440. Thompson, M.B. & Speake, B.K. 2006. A review of the evolution of viviparity in lizards: structure, function, and physiology of the placenta. J. Comp. Physiol. 176: 179–189. Thompson, M.B., Speake, B.K., Stewart, J.B., Russell, K.J. & McCartney, R.J. 2001. Placental nutrition in the Tasmanian skink, Niveoscincus ocellatus. J. Comp. Physiol. 171: 155–160. Uller, T. & Olsson, M. 2006. No seasonal sex-ratio shift despite sex-specific fitness returns of hatching date in a lizard with genotypic sex determination. Evolution 60: 2131–2136. Uller, T. & Olsson, M. Offspring size-number trade-off in a lizard with small clutch sizes: tests of invariants and potential implications. Evol. Ecol. in press. Uller, T., Mott, B., Odierna, G. & Olsson, M. 2006. Consistent sex ratio bias of individual female dragon lizards. Biol. Lett. 2: 569– 572. Wapstra, E. 2000. Maternal basking opportunity affects juvenile phenotype in a viviparous lizard. Funct. Ecol. 14: 345–352. Wapstra, E. & Swain, R. 2001. Geographic and annual variation in life history traits in a temperate zone Australian skink. J. Herpetol. 35: 194–203. Wapstra, E., Swain, R., Jones, S.M. & O’Reilly, J. 1999. Geographical and annual variation in reproductive cycles in the Tasmanian spotted snow skink, Niveoscincus ocellatus (Squamata: Scincidae). Aust. J. Zool. 47: 539–550. Wapstra, E., Uller, T., Pen, I., Komdeur, J., Olsson, M. & Shine, R. 2007. Disentangling the complexities of vertebrate sex allocation: a role for squamate reptiles? Oikos 116: 1051– 1057. Warner, D.A. & Shine, R. 2006. Morphological variation does not influence locomotor performance within a cohort of hatchling lizards (Amphibolurus muricatus, Agamidae). Oikos 114: 126–134. Warner, D.A. & Shine, R. 2007. Fitness of juvenile lizards depends on seasonal timing of hatching, not offspring body size. Oecologia 154: 65–73. Weekes, H.C. 1935. A review of placentation among reptiles. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 3: 625–645. West, S.A., Flanagan, K.E. & Godfray, H.C.J. 2001. Variable host quality, life-history invariants, and the reproductive strategy of a parasitoid wasp that produces single sex clutches. Behav. Ecol. 12: 577–583. While, G.M., Jones, S.M. & Wapstra, E. 2007. Birthing asynchrony is not a consequence of asynchronous offspring development in a non-avian vertebrate, the Australian skink Egernia whitii. Funct. Ecol. 21: 513–519. Williams, T.D. 2005. Mechanisms underlying the costs of egg production. Biosciences 55: 39–48. Winkler, D.W. & Wallin, K. 1987. Offspring size and number: a life history model linking effort per offspring and total effort. Am. Nat. 129: 708–720. Received 17 June 2008; revised 3 September 2008; accepted 4 September 2008 Appendix 1 Summary of the tests of eqn 1 for 12 species of Australian lizards. Species Reproductive mode Eulamprus tympanum Viviparous Eulamprus heatwolei Viviparous Eulamprus quoyii Viviparous Tiliqua rugosa Viviparous Egernia whitii Viviparous Niveoscincus microlepidotus Viviparous Niveoscincus ocellatus Viviparous Clutch sizes (2,1) (3,2) (4,3) (5,4) (2,1) (3,2) (4,3) (5,4) (6,5) (7,6) (8,7) (2,1) (3,2) (2,1) (3,2) (2,1) (3,2) (4,3) (5,4) (3,2) (4,3) (5,4) (6,5) Sample sizes Expected Cn ⁄ Cn + 1 Observed Imax nþ1 Imax nþ1 =Imax n Imin n Lower CI (2.5%) Upper CI (97.5%) (106,33) (135,106) (65,135) (17,65) (13,5) (25,13) (10,25) (10,5) (6,10) (11,6) (3,11) (20,8) (13,20) (52,26) (28,52) (71,14) (42,71) (16,42) (5,16) (188,85) (177,188) (86,177) (22,86) 0.50 0.67 0.75 0.80 0.50 0.67 0.75 0.80 0.83 0.86 0.87 0.50 0.67 0.50 0.67 0.50 0.67 0.75 0.80 0.67 0.75 0.80 0.83 0.91* 0.93* 1.16* 0.76 0.83 1.00 1.53 0.71 1.29 1.10 0.86 1.34 0.82 0.71 0.86 1.03 1.14* 1.60* 0.57 0.90* 1.11* 0.95 0.45 0.54 0.79 0.85 0.46 0.20 0.48 0.51 0.15 0.42 0.39 0.00 0.46 0.39 0.38 0.48 0.34 0.69 1.05 0.03 0.74 0.87 0.55 0.39 1.38 1.17 1.61 1.36 2.04 1.76 3.30 2.26 3.15 2.60 4.00 3.68 1.52 1.11 1.51 2.65 1.90 2.40 1.77 1.11 1.29 1.37 1.07 ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY Tests of size–number invariants in lizards 151 Appendix 1 (Continued) Species Reproductive mode Lampropholis mirabilis Oviparous Bassiana duperreyi Oviparous Amphibolurus muricatus Oviparous Ctenophorus pictus Oviparous Ctenophorus fordi Oviparous Clutch sizes Sample sizes Expected Cn ⁄ Cn + 1 Observed Imax nþ1 Imax nþ1 =Imax n Imin n Lower CI (2.5%) Upper CI (97.5%) (3,2) (4,3) (6,5) (7,6) (8,7) (9,8) (4,3) (5,4) (6,5) (7,6) (8,7) (4,3) (5,4) (6,5) (3,2) (4,3) (5,4) (23,7) (4,23) (11,9) (19,11) (17,19) (6,17) (14,6) (34,14) (37,34) (16,37) (15,16) (57,14) (54,57) (18,54) (134,69) (58,134) (9,58) 0.67 0.75 0.83 0.86 0.87 0.88 0.75 0.80 0.83 0.86 0.87 0.75 0.80 0.83 0.67 0.75 0.80 2.63* 0.84 0.95 1.16 1.14 0.81 0.92 1.37 0.70 0.90 0.78 1.25 0.78 1.03 0.96* 0.73 0.62 1.16 0.06 0.45 0.38 0.35 0.36 0.31 0.64 0.43 0.53 0.51 0.56 0.34 0.54 0.76 0.48 0.20 4.84 2.50 2.27 2.27 3.16 2.04 3.55 2.83 1.21 1.37 1.20 3.63 1.45 1.66 1.22 1.00 1.24 *Significant deviation from the predicted value. ª 2008 THE AUTHORS. J. EVOL. BIOL. 22 (2009) 143–151 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2008 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY