RELATIONAL AGGRESSON: A PRECURSOR TO WORKPLACE BULLYING? Phillip L. Davidson University of Phoenix, School of Advanced Studies 1 10/13/2011 Introduction • Original research in 1939 by Lewin, Lippitt, and White at the University of Iowa’s Child Welfare Research Station. • Attempt to understand group behavior – Rebellion against authority. – Persecution of a scapegoat. – Submissiveness to authoritarian domination. – Aggression. 2 (Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939) The social psychology issues • Aggression in young children – A precursor to adult behavior? – Association with leadership styles. • Precursor to studies in adults regarding – Rebellion against authority. – Submissiveness to authority. – Effectiveness of democratic leadership. 3 (Chang, 2003) Research by Rose and Swenson. – 439 seventh and ninth grades. – Findings: • Popular students did not internalize feelings in relational aggression. • Relational aggression expected in popular students. Relational aggression includes the spreading rumors, creation of an inner circle of friends while excluding others. 4 (Rose & Swenson, 2009) Metropolitan Area Child Study Research Group (MACS) • Seven-year longitudinal study. • Focused on interventions to reduce aggression. – Classroom interventions – Peer group interventions – Family interventions • the results demonstrated only moderate success, and that was with only one intervention and with only students from moderate-income families. The interventions had no statistical affect on students from low-income families. 5 (MACS, 2007) Support for original study by the follow-up studies • Aggression may be • perceived in a positive way by some students (Rose & Swenson, 2009). • Relational aggression may have value to some groups, especially students in low-income families (MACS, 2007) 6 Increase in study variables offers greater richness to the study, but makes projection of result validity more difficult. Lewin et al. carefully controlled and limited their variables (Lewin, Lippitt, & White, 1939) How researchers and practitioners interpret personality and personalityrelated behaviors • Primary question is whether aggression is always “bad.” – Further studies are needed covering different age groups. – Better understanding of how both men and women express aggression. – Increased understanding of the interplay of aggressive acts between men and women and same-gender aggression. 7 Implications for interpreting behavior in employment • The study by Lewin et al. • Can leaders change? (1939) focused on a small – Charismatic leaders controlled group of young reduce aggression boys. (Hepworth & Towler, • Application to employment 2004). requires a better and deeper – Laissez-faire leaders understanding of how provide a destructive different leadership styles leadership style relate to aggression. (Skogstad, Einarsen, • Leaders do not typically Torsheim, Aasland, & change leadership styles to Hetland, 2007). deal with aggression. 8 Implications for interpreting behavior in the social environment • Potentially important to understand perceptions of aggression by children. – How do these perceptions perpetuate into adulthood? – Is bullying and victimization a normal evolution of aggression? – Does aggression fulfill a positive social role from an evolutionary perspective? 9 (LeShan, 1958) Implications for interpreting personal behaviors. • Aggression is always personal. • Authoritarian aggression plays in important part in the evolution of social discrimination (Kessler, 2008). • Aggression is biologically hardwired into humanity. A deeper understanding of its etiology as well as pathological aspects will provide the psychologist with better tools (Cillessen & Mayeux, 2004). 10 Impact on the treatment and understanding of personality disorders. • Researchers commonly equate aggression with violence. • Relational aggression may be of value to an individual (MACS, 2007). • Hostility “is a valuable lever to help the individual break free of both physical and ideational restriction” (LeShan, 1958, p. 119). 11 The Pilot Study The pilot study examined ethnicity, family income, and gender in adults as the independent variables and responses to a modified EXPAGG (Campbell, Muncer, & Coyle, 1992) scale as the dependent variables. The intent was to discover whether adults see relational or indirect aggression as advantageous. 12 Results of Study Figure #1 Males averaged 3.2 compared to females at 2.0 on the instrumental scale. The small number of participants does not provide significance for the data. However, the data does suggest agreement with Driscoll et al. (2005). 13 Results of Study (continued) Figure #2 While the data is too small a sample for accurate prediction, there is a suggestion that more physical aggression or dominance might have a place with the poor and the wealthy. 14 Results of Study (continued) Figure #3 Instrumental and expressive aggression by ethnic background. Reported ethnic backgrounds included three African-Americans, one individual of Asian ancestry, and five white, non-Hispanics. 15 Discussion of Pilot Study • Men possibly tend to be more instrumental in the expression of aggression whereas women tend to be more expressive. • Women view aggression as more personal. • Asking participants to recall an early age is an interesting consideration. 16 Formal Study • 200 nonmanagement employees (100 men and 100 women), in a stratified random population sample. • Use EXPAGG survey. • Correlational analysis between the independent and dependent variables will provide insight into relationships. • A multivariate analysis (MANOVA) will provide comparative data between the three primary groups. 17 References Boulton, M. J. (1999). Concurrent and longitudinal relations between children's playground behavior and social preference, victimization, and bullying. Child Development, 70(4), 944-954. Campbell, A., Muncer, S., & Coyle, E. (1992). Social representation of aggression as an explanation of gender differences: A preliminary study. Aggressive Behavior, 18(2), 95-108. Chang, L. (2003). Variable effects of children's aggression, social withdrawal, and prosocial leadership as functions of teacher beliefs and behaviors. Child Development, 74(2), 535-548. Cillessen, A. H. N., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From censure to reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Child Development, 75(1), 147-163. Cote, S. M. (2007). Sex differences in physical and indirect aggression: A developmental perspective. European Journal on Criminal Policy & Research, 13(3|4), 183-200. Courtois, C. A. (2001). Commentary on ‘guided imagery and memory’ Additional considerations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(2), 133-135. 18 References (Continued) Dodge, K. A., Coie, J. D., Pettit, G. S., & Price, J. M. (1990). Peer status and aggression in boys' groups: Developmental and contextual analysis. Child Development, 61(5), 1289-1309. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1977). Evolution of destructive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 3(2), 127-144. Hepworth, W., & Towler, A. (2004). The effects of individual differences and charismatic leadership on workplace aggression. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9(2), 176-185. Kessler, T., & Cohrs, J. C. (2008). The evolution of authoritarian processes: Fostering cooperation in large-scale groups. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12(1), 73-84. LeShan, L. (1958). Some aspects of the positive value of hostility. American Psychologist, 13(3), 118-119. Lewin, K., Lippitt, R., & White, R. K. (1939). Patterns of aggressive behavior in experimentally created "social environments." The Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 271-299. Lippitt, R. (1939). Field theory and experiment in social psychology: Autocratic and democratic group atmospheres. American Journal of Sociology, 45(1), 26-49. 19 References (Continued) 20 Metropolitan Area Child Study Research Group (MACS). (2007). Changing the way children 'think' about aggression: Socialcognitive effects of a preventive intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(1), 160-167. Passini, S. (2008). Exploring the multidimensional facets of authoritarianism: Authoritarian aggression and social dominance orientation. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67(1), 51-60. Rose, A. J., & Swenson, L. P. (2009). Do perceived popular adolescents who aggress against others experience emotional adjustment problems themselves? Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 868-872. Skogstad, A., Einarsen, S., Torsheim, T., Aasland, M. S., & Hetland, H. (2007). The destructiveness of laissez-faire leadership behavior. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12(1), 80-92. Webster, G. D. (2008). The kinship, acceptance, and rejection model of altruism and aggression (KARMAA): Implications for interpersonal and intergroup aggression. Group Dynamics Theory, Research, and Practice, 12(1), 27-38. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison in social psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 90(2), 245-271.