We’re all going to die! Trevor Butterworth Statistical Assessment Service (STATS) George Mason University, Virginia www.stats.org “A journalist is a lookout on the bridge of the ship of state” — Joseph Pulitzer Sounding the alarm at Starbucks An everyday story of risk… And the serious health problems are: “Pesticides in food have been linked to ADHD and other childhood health problems. And it's not just on the food, either. Scientists have found that systemic pesticides are taken up inside of the plants that we eat. Gross!” It’s a bug’s life… “ Many bug killers work because they screw with a bug's nerves. But those same chemical controls could be messing with children's health, too.” Killer plastic… “Sure, we've known for a few years now that Nos. 3 and 6 plastic and BPA are particularly bad for you. But a recent study published in the journal of Environmental Health Perspectives found that all plastic is bad for you.” And so on… Toxic toys Unsafe soap “Harsh” disinfectants Scented candles Air fresheners But… 374 2 1,282,635 Top substances in pediatric exposures (age 0-5) 1 Cosmetics / personal care products 2 Analgesics 3 Cleaning substances (household) 4 Foreign bodies / toys/ misc. 5 Topical preparations 174,073 130,213 125,394 93,574 91,127 Innumerate “Generally I will try not to let two paragraphs with numbers bump against each other — ever. Because I think numbers are absolutely deadly.” — Best Newspaper Writing, 1982 “Most journalists seem unable to judge whether numbers are really meaningful or accurate. Consequently, they either trust all figures or they trust none; and they tend to focus exclusively on a report writer's conclusions, while ignoring specific numbers and data collection techniques.” — Tankard and Ryan A history of inaccuracy 8.8% science stories judged error free by scientists 40-59% for other subjects 42 “types” of error Lack of methodological detail worst error Lack of context – earlier research on issue ignored — Tankard and Ryan, 1974 “…a lot of scientists distrust the press. They have good cause for this. Some of the things the press has done to science are horrible to contemplate.” — Ralph Coghlan, St. Louis Post Dispatch, writing in The Scientific Monthly on “The need for science writing in the press.” 1946 Have things improved or worsened? How SOT Members Rate the Accuracy of the Media's Reporting on Certain Scientific Issues and Principles Percentage Rating Poorly/Well 100% 90% 97% 96% 96% 95% 97% 96% 90% 80% 70% 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 8% 3% 3% 3% 2% 2% 2% 0% In providing diverse view s & balance Explaining risk/benefit tradeoffs Explaining that Distinguishing Distinguishing "dose makes correlation from betw een the poison" causation absolute & relative risk Distinguishing good & bad studies Explaining odds ratios STATS at George Mason University POORLY WELL How SOT Members Rate the Weight the Media Gives to Various Studies 100% 18% 90% 27% 80% 70% 60% 21% 68% 73% 74% 36% 50% 40% 30% 48% 20% 6% 9% 13% 10% 14% 12% 10% 28% 0% Individual studies relative to overall evidence Individual scientists relative to scientific community Studies by environmental group scientists Studies by government scientists Studies by private sector scientists STATS at George Mason University TOO LITTLE RIGHT TOO MUCH Consequences? So what happens when you take an innumerate watchdog that’s always on the lookout for new threats, and subject it to a relentless assault of novel, alarming studies? Always the deadliest context? Two years later… “For farmed salmon, the cardiovascular benefits are greater than the cancer risks by a factor of at least 300:1. With the exception of some locally caught sport fish from contaminated inland waters, the levels of PCBs and dioxins in fish should not influence decisions about fish intake.” — Dariush Mozaffarian, Dept of Epidemiology, Harvard School of Public Health “ Unfortunately, the media and others may have contributed to this confusion by greatly exaggerating the unsubstantiated claim of a health risk from fish.” — Erich Rimm, Dept of Epidemiology, Nutrition, Harvard School of Public Health Barking mad “ Journalists are, in the very nature of their calling, alarmists; and this is their way if giving interest to what they write. Herein they are like little dogs, if anything stirs, they immediately set up a shrill bark. ” — Arthur Schopenhauer Is the public credulous? 29% believe news organizations get their facts straight 63% say stories are inaccurate (up from 34% in 1985) 70% believe news orgs try to cover up mistakes - Pew Research Center for the People and the , 2009 Is schmexpertise driving expertise out of circulation? Who do you trust? Gisele? “Are you going to give chemical food to your child, when they are so little?” “I cannot put this poison on my skin. I do not use anything synthetic.” Bruce? “We have estimated that 99.9% of the chemicals humans are exposed to are natural, and we find that they are as frequently positive in rodent cancer tests as synthetic chemicals… Many ordinary foods would not pass the health criteria that have been used to regulate human exposures to synthetic chemicals based on results of animal cancer tests.” Not a fair fight… Bruce Ames may be one of the most cited scientists in the world, but his views on chemophobia have rarely made the media – perhaps 10-15 citations in past decade. Gisele Bundchen has approximately 300 news citations on chemical issues in past two years (not all favorable, mind you). 5,790,000 web citations according to Google Visible risks, invisible risk assessment 49:1 Error rates What is the likelihood that one new study with a novel or alarming conclusion can overturn the weight of evidence? For scientists, the likelihood is directly in relation to study method and replication, which is why they are, generally, suspicious of novelty Journalism’s precautionary principle For journalists, the risk of being wrong about something going wrong is vastly greater than the risk of being wrong about something that is right. Which is a truer picture of reality? Conclusions Journalism is intellectually and emotionally invested in reporting risk. Journalism about risk gives the audience an emotional experience, generally using selective data for intellectual support. Risk assessment gives people an intellectual experience but little emotional connection to the weight of evidence. The emotional experience is much more likely to move people towards action unless that action is so engrained in normal life the risk becomes acceptable. It all comes down to storytelling “If you can succeed in telling a story that makes your audience feel as if this heart is beating for them… they will buy your product in order to own the story.” — Lynda Resnick (Fiji water etc) to Peter Guber, in Tell to Win. A call to action? “The life of academic quietism is over. The man of learning, however well equipped he may be, must learn to become a man of action, a politician, a man of the people, speaking for people, leading people…” — Joseph Brandt, Harpers, March 1946