Personality and well-being 1 Running Head: PERSONALITY AND WELL-BEING Happiness is a Personal(ity) Thing: The Genetics of Personality and Well-being in a Representative Sample Alexander Weiss and Timothy C. Bates University of Edinburgh Michelle Luciano Queensland Institute of Medical Research Personality and well-being 2 Abstract While happiness has a genetic component it has been unclear what might explain these heritable differences. Here we use a population sample of 973 twin-pairs to test the hypothesis that heritable differences in subjective wellbeing are entirely accounted for by genetic architecture of the five-factor model personality domains. Results supported a model in which wellbeing was accounted for by unique genetic influences on extraversion, neuroticism, and conscientiousness, and by a genetic “covitality” factor influencing all five major personality domains in the direction of low N, and high extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness, as well as elevated wellbeing. Personality and well-being 3 Happiness is a personal(ity) thing: The Genetics of Personality and Well-being in a Representative Sample The fact that personality dimensions, especially Neuroticism and Extraversion, explain a substantial amount of subjective well-being is well-known. To date there are several explanations offered, though none so far has considered the possibility that common genes explain variance in personality and subjective well-being. In the present study we used a large representative twin sample to test whether the latent genetic effects explaining the individual differences in the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality also account for individual differences in subjective well-being. A considerable body of work has focussed on the construct of subjective well-being, operationalized in reliable measures of positive and negative moods and global satisfaction (Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Researchers have found that subjective well-being was largely independent of environmental and socio-economic differences, and showed high levels of within-person stability across measurement times (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996) and high heritability (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Nes, Røysamb, Tambs, Harris, & ReichbornKjennerud, 2006). Other studies indicated that subjective well-being was strongly linked to the FFM, especially Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). In addition, Agreeableness and Openness to Experience also appear to play roles in some elements of happiness and unhappiness (McCrae & Costa, 1991). Given that subjective well-being variance is mostly stable and not the result of environmental or demographic factors prompted some to suggest that individuals have biologically-based, stable well-being “set points” (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996). This hypothesis is supported by behavior genetic studies which have found that genetic effects explain approximately 50% of variance in subjective well-being, with evidence for both additive (Nes et al., 2006) and non-additive (gene × gene interaction) genetic effects (Lykken Personality and well-being 4 & Tellegen, 1996). These behavior genetic studies also did not find evidence for significant common environmental effects, i.e. all of the familial similarity between siblings within families appears to be genetic in origin. In fact, these studies found that environmental impacts on subjective well-being consist of non-familial environmental effects. These findings are supported by non-behavior genetic research showing that finding or losing a job (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2004) and change in marital status (Lucas, Clark, Georgellis, & Diener, 2003) can affect subjective well-being. Like subjective well-being, the domains of the FFM are also mostly stable in adulthood (see, e.g., Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006) and heritable (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001). Moreover, numerous studies have shown that personality traits, especially Extraversion and Neuroticism, account for approximately half of the variance in subjective well-being (DeNeve & Cooper, 1998). Previously offered explanations for these correlations include direct effects, such as the relationship of Extraversion and Neuroticism to reward and punishment, respectively (Cantor & Sanderson, 1999; Carver & Scheier, 1990; Carver & White, 1994) as well as indirect or instrumental effects, where trait differences act to alter the experiences an individual encounters (McCrae & Costa, 1991). One explanation for the correlation between personality and subjective well-being is that, as with Neuroticism and depression (Kendler, Gardner, Gatz, & Pedersen, 2007; Kendler, Gatz, Gardner, & Pedersen, 2006) personality and well-being share genes in common. We tested a common genetic cause hypothesis in a large representative sample of adult United States twins. Our hypothesis posits that the heritable component of subjective well-being is entirely explained by the latent genetic architecture of the FFM. If supported, this hypothesis provides important insights for theories of subjective well-being, suggesting Personality and well-being 5 that the genetic and environmental models of subjective well-being may be framed in terms of personality. Methods Participants The sample consisted of 973 twin-pairs from the MacArthur Foundation Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS). Sample recruitment was initially by telephone. Approximately 50,000 households, representative of the population of the United States were screened by telephone. Just under 15% of respondents reported having a twin in the family, of whom 60% gave permission for the twins to be contacted as part of the MIDUS recruitment process (Kendler, Thornton, Gilman, & Kessler, 2000; Kessler, Gilman, Thornton, & Kendler, 2004). Zygosity was determined using self-report questions (eye and hair color similarity, similarity in childhood indicated by misidentification) with > 90% accuracy (Lykken, Bouchard, McGue, & Tellegen, 1990). Inclusion criteria included being first degree relatives of the original contact or their partner, currently aged between 25 and 74 years, living in the continental United States, having home contact telephone, speaking English. The resultant twin sample consisted of 973 twin-pairs (365 MZ and 608 DZ) where at least one member of the pair had some personality or subjective well-being data. Mean age was 44.9 (SD = 12.1). The number of each twin pair type and their ages are shown in Table 1. ----------------------------------Insert Table 1 about here ----------------------------------Measures Personality. The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI), a self-administered 25-item 4-point Likert scale adjectival personality questionnaire (Lachman & Weaver, 1997) was Personality and well-being 6 mailed to each participant. Adjectives were chosen from existing measures (e.g., Goldberg, 1990). We used the five previously defined scales (University of Wisconsin Institute of Aging, 2004). Briefly, each personality scale was constructed by summing up the items defining that dimension: Neuroticism was defined by moody, worrying, nervous, and (reversed) calm; Extraversion was defined by outgoing, friendly, lively, active, and talkative; Openness to Experience was defined by creative, imaginative, intelligent, curious, broadminded, sophisticated, and adventurous; Agreeableness was defined by helpful, caring, warm, soft-hearted, and sympathetic; and Conscientiousness was defined by organized, responsible, hardworking, and (reversed) careless. Subjective well-being. We assessed subjective well-being using three questions from the MIDUS sample that were similar to those used in other scales (see, e.g., Diener, Suh, Lucas, & Smith, 1999). Questions were part of a telephone interview. The first question asked how satisfied participants were with life at the present, the second asked how much control subjects felt they had over their lives, and the third question asked how satisfied they were with life overall. As with the personality questionnaire, each question could be answered using a four-point Likert Scale with lower values indicating higher subjective wellbeing. For the purpose of the present study we reverse-coded and summed these items. Analysis A classical twin design, in which the resemblance of identical and non-identical twins is compared, was used. Based on previous findings indicating the possibility of nonadditive genetic effects and the lack of shared environmental effects (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996), we used structural equation models to specify the covariance of identical twins as additive (A) plus dominance (or non-additive) genes (D), and the covariance of non-identical twins as 0.5A plus 0.25D. Unique environmental (E) sources of variance are unshared between co- Personality and well-being 7 twins so only contribute to trait variance. Parameter estimates for A, D and E were estimated by maximum likelihood in Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie, & Maes, 1999). Multivariate genetic modeling began from the approach of Cholesky decomposition of additive genetic, dominance genetic, and unique environmental covariance between the measures. This specifies as many factors as there are variables for each source of variance, each factor having one loading less than the previous one. Reduced models (i.e., those with fewer parameters) are favored if the likelihood ratio chi-square comparing the models is less than the critical value (alpha = .05) of the chi-square distribution, indicating that there is no significant difference between the saturated model and the reduced model. Results Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations for the personality domains and subjective well-being and the MZ and DZ correlations. Monozygotic twin correlations were substantially greater than dizygotic twin correlations, consistent with prior findings suggesting a non-additive genetic component to subjective well-being and little evidence for shared environmental effects (Lykken & Tellegen, 1996). We therefore included genetic dominance rather than shared environmental effects in our base model. Testing for bivariate normality using the “%P” function in Mx (z < -3.5 or > 3.5) identified 8 outliers, which were removed from further analyses. We also controlled for gender and age as males had significantly lower levels of Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness and older individuals had lower levels of Neuroticism and Openness but higher levels of Agreeableness and subjective well-being (see Table 3). ----------------------------------Insert Tables 2, 3, and 4 about here ----------------------------------- Personality and well-being 8 The hypothesized model specified that all genetic influences on subjective well-being originated from a general genetic factor and genetic factors for the five personality domains. This model therefore posited general latent additive and dominance genetic effects underlying variance in all five personality domains and subjective well-being as well as specific additive genetic and dominance effects for the five personality domains, which also had the opportunity to explain variance in subjective well-being (see Figure 1). The fit of this model was assessed using the chi-square and AIC criteria. Table 4 shows the fit statistics for each model. There was no significant loss of fit between this model and the saturated model. ----------------------------------Insert Figure 1 about here ----------------------------------The hypothesised model was then further reduced by removing genetic paths from the Agreeableness and Openness factors to Subjective well-being, as these were non-significant. Finally, we tested whether dominance effects were significant; eliminating dominance effects did not significantly reduce model fit. Thus, the final model included a general additive genetic factor that contributed to variance in all five personality domains and subjective wellbeing, as well as specific genetic factors influencing each personality domain, and paths from the independent genetic influences on Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Conscientiousness to subjective well-being (see Figure 2). The corresponding E or unique-environment effects were modelled as a Cholesky decomposition, and are presented in Table 5. ----------------------------------Insert Figure 2 and Table 5 about here ----------------------------------- Personality and well-being 9 Discussion The genetic structure of subjective well-being can be modelled adequately without reference to genetic influences specific to subjective well-being. The model also indicated that the observed correlations between subjective well-being and the FFM domains have a genetic component, and that subjective well-being reflects differences in low Neuroticism, high Extraversion, and, to a lesser extent, high Conscientiousness. Our findings cast previous findings that showed a relationship between personality domains and subjective well-being (McCrae & Costa, 1991) in a new light by showing that a common genetic cause underlies variation in these important constructs. Moreover, our findings extend previous behavior genetic findings on personality and subjective well-being (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Lykken & Tellegen, 1996; Nes et al., 2006) and depression (Kendler et al., 2007; Kendler et al., 2006) by showing that a large part of the genetic architecture underlying individual differences in happiness, is provided by the genetic structure of the FFM. In this sample it was possible to drop the dominance effects without significantly reducing fit. While this is not consistent with the findings of Lykken & Tellegen (1996) it is consistent with other findings (Nes et al., 2006). One reason for the disparate findings of dominance effects on well-being is that classical twin designs have low power to detect dominance effects (Neale & Cardon, 1992). Thus an extended twin design or larger sample would be needed to definitively address the question of whether dominance variance or something like DZ sibling-contrast effects (Eaves & Silberg, 2005) are responsible for the substantially higher MZ twin correlations in subjective well-being. Finally, all models required a general latent genetic factor for all the five personality domains and subjective well-being. This may reflect either a general factor such as lifehistory strategy (Figueredo, Vasquez, Brumbach, Sefcek, Kirsner, & Jacobs, 2005), but may also reflect presentation bias or some other method factor. Future twin studies of personality Personality and well-being 10 and subjective well-being that incorporate multiple methods (see, e.g. Riemann, Angleitner, & Strelau, 1997) may clarify this issue. In conclusion, the present study supports the presence of a general genetic factor, possibly reflecting covitality (Weiss, King, & Enns, 2002), and additional effects of personality on subjective well-being. Just as significant insights into physical and mental illness have flowed from studies relating illness to normal variation in personality and from research on the causes of co-morbidity (see, e.g., Deary, Wright, Harris, Whalley, & Starr, 2004), further research on the general and specific influences on subjective well-being may lead to major advances in the understanding of happiness and other constructs of interest to positive psychology. Personality and well-being 11 References Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243-273. Cantor, N., & Sanderson, C. A. (1999). Life task participation and wellbeing: The importance of taking part in daily life. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Wellbeing: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 230-243). New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19-35. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333. Deary, I. J., Wright, A. F., Harris, S. E., Whalley, L. J., & Starr, J. M. (2004). Searching for genetic influences on normal cognitive ageing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 178-184 DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197-229. Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276-302. Eaves, L. J. & Silberg, J. L. (2005). Parent-child feedback predicts sibling contrast: using twin studies to test theories of parent-offspring interaction in infant behavior. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 8, 1-4. Figueredo, A. J., Vasquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Sefcek, J. A., Kirsner, B. R., & Jacobs, W. J. (2005). Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 1349-1360. Personality and well-being 12 Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative "description of personality": the Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216-1229. Kendler, K. S., Gardner, C. O., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2007). The sources of comorbidity between major depression and generalized anxiety disorder in a Swedish national twin sample. Psychological Medicine, 37(3), 453-462. Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., Gardner, C. O., & Pedersen, N. L. (2006). Personality and major depression - A Swedish longitudinal, population-based twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 1113-1120. Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., Gilman, S. E., & Kessler, R. C. (2000). Sexual orientation in a U.S. national sample of twin and nontwin sibling pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1843-1846. Kessler, R. C., Gilman, S. E., Thornton, L. M., & Kendler, K. S. (2004). Health, well-being, and social responsibility in the MIDUS twin and sibling subsamples. In O. G. Brim, C. D. Ryff & R. C. Kessler (Eds.), How healthy are we: A national study of well-being at midlife (pp. 124-152). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1997). The Midlife Development Inventory (MIDI) personality scales: Scale construction and scoring. Walthman, MA: Brandeis University. Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Reexamining adaptation and the set point model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527-539. Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2004). Unemployment alters the set point for life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 8-13. Lykken, D., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186-189. Personality and well-being 13 Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Mcgue, M., & Tellegen, A. (1990). The Minnesota Twin Family Registry - Some Initial Findings. Acta Geneticae Medicae Et Gemellologiae, 39, 35-70. Lykken, D. T., & Tellegen, A. (1996). Happiness is a stochastic phenomenon. Psychological Science, 7, 186-189. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1991). Adding Liebe und Arbeit - the full Five-Factor Model and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227-232. Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (1999). Mx: Statistical modeling (5th ed.). VCU Box 900126, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of Psychiatry. Neale, M. C., and Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Nes, R. B., Røysamb, E., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Tambs, K., & Harris, J. R. (2005). Happiness: Stability and change - Genetic and environmental contributions. Behavior Genetics, 35, 815-816. Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental influences on personality : A study of twins reared together using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449-475. Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 1-25. University of Wisconsin Institute on Aging. (2004). Midlife in the United States: A study of health and well-being. Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Enns, R. M. (2002). Subjective well-being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1141-1149. Personality and well-being 14 Author Note Alexander Weiss, Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology, and the Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh; Timothy C. Bates, Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology, and the Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh; Michelle Luciano, Queensland Institute of Medical Research. Acknowledgements??? Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alexander Weiss, Department of Psychology, School of Philosophy, Psychology, and the Language Sciences, The University of Edinburgh, 7 George Square, Edinburgh EH8 9JZ, United Kingdom. Email: alex.weiss@ed.ac.uk. Personality and well-being 15 Table 1 Mean Age for Each Twin-pair Type Zygosity MZ DZ Type Count Age (SD) Count Age (SD) Male1 171 44.5 (11.5) 136 44.2 (12.5) Female2 194 43.5 (12.2) 213 45.9 (12.4) 259 45.8 (11.9) Opposite Sex3 Note. 1Same-sex male pairs, 2Same-sex female pairs, 3Opposite-sex pairs Personality and well-being 16 Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of the Five Personality Domains and Subjective Well-being and their Correlations by Zygozity. Monozygotic Dimension MTI Neuroticism Dizygotic SDT1 M T2 SD T2 rT1,T2 3.51 .45 3.52 .48 .27 Extraversion 2.25 .72 2.23 .66 Openness 2.99 .52 3.00 Agreeableness 3.20 .58 Conscientiousness 3.43 .45 Subjective well-being 10.92 MTI SDT1 M T2 SD T2 rT1,T2 3.52 .47 3.54 .47 .12 .44 2.22 .65 2.28 .66 .19 .51 .30 2.94 .52 2.97 .55 .12 3.24 .54 .39 3.22 .54 3.23 .57 .16 3.47 .42 .35 3.43 .43 3.44 .42 .13 1.49 10.91 1.43 1.37 10.86 1.44 .09 .30 10.92 Note. MT1 = Twin 1 mean; MT2 = Twin 2 mean; SDT1 = Twin 1 standard deviation; SDT2 = Twin 2 standard deviation, rT1,T2 = twin correlation. Personality and well-being 17 Table 3 Modelled Gender and Age Effects Male Deviation from Female Mean bage Neuroticism -.11 -.008 Extraversion ns ns O ns -.004 A -.24 .002 C -.10 ns WB ns .005 Note. ns = p > .05. Personality and well-being 18 Table 4 AIC and χ2 Fit Statistics for Hypothesised model, and Reduced Models of Personality Domains and Subjective Well-being. Model -2LL df ² (df) p AIC Saturated ADE Model 13040.402 10343 Hypothesized Model 13046.094 10353 5.69 (10) 0.84 -14.31 Drop Additive Genetic Oaths from Openness and 13047.878 10357 7.48 (14) 0.91 -20.52 13067.877 10371 27.47 (28) 0.49 -28.52 Agreeableness to SWB Drop All Dominance effects Note. -2LL = -2 log-likelihood; ² (df) = calculated on change in -2LL and change in df between nested models; AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion. Personality and well-being 19 Table 5 Cholesky Decomposition of Unique variance– Standardised Path Coefficients Factor Factor 1 2 3 4 5 1 -.73 2 .05 .78 3 .12 .36 .64 4 .02 .44 .11 .75 5 .19 .11 .13 .11 .69 6 .20 .12 .04 .09 .06 6 .84 Personality and well-being 20 Figure Captions Figure 1. Hypothesised model in which genetic effects on measured subjective well-being are accounted for by a general covitality factor (AG) common to all traits, and by influences from the latent genetic effects of the five personality domains (AN, AE, AO, AA, AC). Figure 2. Final reduced model with standardized path coefficients. Values in parentheses are 95% confidence intervals. Personality and well-being 21 Figure 1 AG N E O AN AE AO A AA C AC W Personality and well-being 22 Figure 2 AG .15 (.02, .23) -.29 (-.21, -.38) .55 (.43, .65) N AN E O -.62 (-.54, -.68) .30 (.15, .41) .23 (.13, 33) .37 .30 (.18, .38) A .49 (.41, .55) .32 (.21, .41) C AO W .58 (.50, .64) .37 (.26. .44) (.27, .45) AE .45 (.37, .53) AA .09 (.02, .18) AC