A Psychoeducational Approach to Stress Management

advertisement

FACULTEIT PSYCHOLOGIE EN

PEDAGOGISCHE WETENSCHAPPEN

ONDERZOEKSGROEP VOOR GEZONDHEIDSPSYCHOLOGIE

A Psychoeducational

Approach to Stress Management.

An Implementation and Effectiveness Study

Tom Van Daele van de graad van Doctor in de Psychologie

Prof. dr. Omer Van den Bergh, promotor

Prof. dr. Dirk Hermans, copromotor

Prof. dr. Chantal Van Audenhove, copromotor

2013

Summary

There is a growing emphasis in high-income countries on the importance of mental health. However, the majority of people in need lack access to professional care and a significant number of those who do get professional attention, ironically receive overtreatment. A number of barriers need to be addressed in order to overcome these issues. These barriers are situated within the general public and the patients themselves, within the health professionals and within the policy makers. Addressing them successfully would allow mental healthcare to shift from an instance that primarily focuses on curing, to one that also pays attention to educating, coaching and promoting mental health. A first step in achieving this goal is to set up stepped care, which particularly requires extending the number of primary care interventions (e.g. psychoeducational group interventions).

In this doctoral dissertation we focused on three policy oriented research topics. A first topic is effectiveness, as we set out to evaluate the effectiveness of a local adaptation of a psychoeducational intervention for stress. A matched control design showed that people participating in the intervention showed a steady linear decline of stress and symptoms of depression. This improvement could be noticed up until 18 months follow-up and almost 30 percent of all participants experienced a clinically significant and reliable change. Furthermore, we wanted to gain additional insight in the overall evidence base for psychoeducational interventions that focus on stress reduction and we also wanted to chart possible moderators of effectiveness. A systematic review and meta-analysis was therefore conducted, which reported small but consistently positive effects of short-term effectiveness. For long-term effectiveness, results were less pronounced. As for the moderators, brief interventions for women appeared to be most effective.

A second research topic is implementation, of which the importance became apparent during the previously mentioned intervention study. A recurring topic for researchers and partners who collaborate in the field is the dealing with issues of fidelity and adaptation of interventions. We set out to help address this issue and our

personal experiences combined with a search in literature resulted in the construction of ‘empowerment implementation’, a framework for implementation research. The applicability of this framework was demonstrated using the psychoeducational intervention study mentioned earlier.

A third and final research topic is prediction. We also wanted to investigate whether we could predict to what extent participants could benefit from our psychoeducational group intervention. We therefore considered the use of overgeneral memory as such a predictor. In a first study, we found that the more specific responses participants provided on the Autobiographical Memory Task (AMT, which has the capacity to measure overgeneral memory), the more their problem solving strategies increased during the intervention. Because of these encouraging results, we set out to investigate whether the AMT could also be used to predict the evolution of a non-treated population, using the large convenience sample of control subjects from the effectiveness study. Results showed that, when controlling for baseline symptoms, overgeneral memory could predict long-term changes for both depression and anxiety after one year and 18 months.

After detailed reporting on each of these topics, the dissertation concludes with a general discussion in which a synthesis of the three topics is made. Furthermore, limitations and future perspectives are highlighted and we also reflect on the characteristics of a PhD focusing on policy oriented research. Finally, a number of general and specific policy recommendations are also made.

Samenvatting

In hoge inkomenslanden is er in toenemende mate aandacht voor geestelijke gezondheid. De meerderheid van de bevolking die met psychische klachten kampt en die nood heeft aan professionele zorg heeft hier echter geen toegang tot. Van zij die er wel beroep op kunnen doen, worden er ironisch genoeg een aanzienlijk aantal overbehandeld. Om deze problemen op te lossen moeten er op verschillende niveaus barrières worden aangepakt: bij de publieke opinie en de patiënten zelf, bij de zorgprofessionals en bij de beleidsmakers. Dit kan de geestelijke gezondheidszorg toelaten om te evolueren van een instantie met als hoofdfocus zorgen en genezen, naar een die ook aandacht besteedt aan gezondheidspromotie en preventie. Een eerste belangrijke stap is het implementeren van eerstelijnsinterventies – zoals psychoeducatieve groepsinterventies – in het kader van de uitbreiding van getrapte zorg.

Dit doctoraat behandelt drie beleidsgerelateerde onderwerpen. Een eerste is effectiviteit, waarbij we als doel hadden om de effectiviteit van een lokale aanpassing van een psycho-educatieve groepsinterventie voor stress te evalueren. Een matched control design toonde aan dat deelnemers een geleidelijke, lineaire daling van stress en depressieve symptomen vertoonden. Deze verbetering was merkbaar tot anderhalf jaar na de interventie en in die periode realiseerde zowat dertig procent van de deelnemers een klinisch significante en betrouwbare verandering. Daarnaast waren we ook geïnteresseerd in de algemene onderzoeksevidentie van psycho-educatieve groepsinterventies voor stress en of er moderatoren waren die een invloed hadden op hun effectiviteit. Een systematische review en meta-analyse vonden kleine, consistent positieve effecten op korte termijn, maar op langere termijn waren de effecten minder duidelijk. Wat de moderatoren betreft, leken kortdurende interventies voor vrouwen het meest effectief.

Een tweede onderwerp is implementatie, een topic waarvan het belang ons duidelijk werd tijdens het evalueren van de interventie die we eerder vermeldden. Een terugkerend aspect in de samenwerking tussen onderzoekers en lokale partners bleek namelijk de ogenschijnlijke tegenstelling tussen (het bewaren van) betrouwbaarheid en

(het toestaan van) flexibiliteit bij het implementeren van interventies. Onze persoonlijke ervaringen en een literatuurstudie lieten toe om een mogelijke oplossing voor dit probleem te formuleren: empowerment implementation, een wetenschappelijk kader voor implementatieonderzoek. De praktische toepasbaarheid van dit kader werd vervolgens geïllustreerd aan de hand van de psycho-educatieve groepsinterventie.

Een derde een laatste onderwerp is predictie. We wilden namelijk ook onderzoeken of het mogelijk was om te voorspellen in welke mate individuele deelnemers baat hebben van de psycho-educatieve groepsinterventie. We maakten hiervoor gebruik van overalgemeen geheugen als predictor. In een eerste studie vonden we dat hoe specifieker deelnemers hun antwoorden konden formuleren op de Autobiografische

Geheugen Taak (AGT, een instrument dat ondermeer overalgemeen geheugen kan meten), hoe meer hun probleemoplossende strategieën toenamen tijdens de interventie. Omwille van deze bemoedigende resultaten onderzochten we verder of de

AGT ook een rol kon spelen in het voorspelen van de klachtenevolutie bij personen die geen behandeling volgden. We konden hiervoor gebruik maken van de aanzienlijke hoeveelheid data van de participanten die deel uitmaakten van de controlegroep van de effectiviteitsstudie. Overalgemeen geheugen bleek de langetermijnsevolutie van de depressieve en angstgerelateerde klachten van deze deelnemers te voorspellen, wanneer we controleerden voor hun klachten bij aanvang.

Na een gedetailleerd overzicht van elk van deze onderwerpen wordt de doctoraatsverhandeling afgerond met een algemene discussie waarin we tot een synthese komen van deze drie onderwerpen. Vervolgens worden de beperkingen van de huidige studies opgelijst, samen met suggesties voor toekomstig onderzoek. We staan daarna nog even stil bij de karakteristieken van een doctoraat met als focus beleidsrelevant onderzoek om uiteindelijk ook een aantal algemene en specifieke beleidsaanbevelingen te formuleren.

Contents

1 General introduction ........................................................................... 1

1 Mental health in high income countries ............................................ 1

2 The current challenges for successful mental healthcare .................. 4

3 A paradigm shift from curing to educating, coaching and

promoting ......................................................................................... 11

4 The aim of this PhD ........................................................................... 29

Research Topic 1 : Effectiveness

2 Stress Reduction through psychoeducation: a meta-analytic review.. 33

1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 34

2 Method ............................................................................................. 40

3 Results .............................................................................................. 43

4 Discussion ......................................................................................... 49

5 Limitations and directions for future research ................................. 50

3 Effectiveness of a six session stress reduction program for groups ...... 53

1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 54

2 Method ............................................................................................. 56

3 Results .............................................................................................. 61

4 Discussion ......................................................................................... 66

Research Topic 2 : Implementation

4 Empowerment implementation: enhancing fidelity and adaptation in a

psychoeducational intervention ......................................................... 73

1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 74

2 Empowerment implementation ....................................................... 78

vii

Contents

3 Example: implementation of a psychoeducational group

intervention ...................................................................................... 83

4 Discussion ......................................................................................... 90

5 Conclusion ........................................................................................ 91

Research Topic 3 : Prediction

5 Reduced memory specificity predicts the acquisition of problem solving

skills in psychoeducation.................................................................... 95

1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 96

2 Method ........................................................................................... 100

3 Results ............................................................................................ 104

4 Discussion ....................................................................................... 107

6 Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts changes in depression

and anxiety in a community sample ................................................ 111

1 Introduction .................................................................................... 112

2 Method ........................................................................................... 117

3 Results ............................................................................................ 120

4 Discussion ....................................................................................... 121

7 General discussion ........................................................................... 125

1 Research topics and main results ................................................... 126

2 Limitations and future perspectives ............................................... 133

3 Characteristics of the research in this PhD ..................................... 142

4 Specific implications and general policy recommendations .......... 145

viii

List of Tables

Table 1 Operationalization and descriptive statistics for moderators ......... 37

Table 2 Summary of studies included in the review .................................... 42

Table 3 Moderator values and effect sizes for psychoeducational and stress reduction programs ...................................................................................... 45

Table 4 Effects for moderators ..................................................................... 47

Table 5 Sociodemographics for intervention group and matched control group in percent ........................................................................................... 62

Tabel 6 Evolution of intervention group and matched control group ........ 63

Tabel 7 Changes in self-reported complaints after course participation ... 105

Tabel 8 Means and standard deviations on the different categories of the autobiographical memory test ................................................................... 106

Table 9 Hierachical Linear Model and results of DASS-21-subscales of depression and anxiety as the dependent variables. ................................. 119

Table 10 Descriptive statistics for the DASS-21-subscales of depression and anxiety. ....................................................................................................... 120

ix

x

List of Figures

Figure 1. The mental health intervention spectrum (National Research

Council & Institute for Medical Research, 2009). ......................................... 13

Figure 2.

Flow chart of the search strategy .................................................. 43

Figure 3.

Overall effects of the interventions on stress at posttest ............. 48

Figure 4.

Overall effect of the interventions on stress at follow-up ............ 48

Figure 5. Comparison of DASS-scores between participants with a low- and high-level baseline of complaints. ................................................................ 65

Figure 6.

Relationship between the AMT percentage of specific memories and the SAD-MEPS change score for number of means. ........................... 107

Figure 7.

Relationship between policy, research and practice. .................. 143

xi

xii

Chapter 1

General introduction

1

Mental health in high income countries

The World Health Organization (WHO) proposed in 2005 that “there is no health without mental health” (WHO, 2005, p.11). This statement illustrates the growing emphasis in high-income countries on the importance of not only a healthy body, but also a healthy mind. But what is mental health exactly?

Among the many definitions available, Maercker and Zoellner (2004, p.42) consider mental health a broadly termed construct “... defined as a processing function not only to feel good about oneself, but also to develop good social relationships; engage in productive, creative work; and combat subsequent stress effectively”. The WHO (2001, p. 1) seems to concur with this point of view and defines mental health “as a state of well-being in which every individual realizes his or her own potential, can cope with the normal stresses of life, can work productively and fruitfully, and is able to make a contribution to her or his community”. W hen one or more of these conditions are not met, people face mental illness.

If such symptoms arise, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders (DSM-IV-TR, American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and the

International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10, World Health Organization,

2008) are classification systems that can be used to diagnose common mental disorders like depression and anxiety. Approximately 50% of the US-population meets criteria for one or more of these mental disorders in their lifetime, whereas about 25% of the population meets criteria in any given year (Kessler

& Wang, 2008). However, prevalence rates have high variability among countries. Because recent WHO studies showed that the highest prevalence estimates were documented in the United States of America (Kessler et al.,

1

General introduction

2009), these numbers should therefore be interpreted with caution. European data are available both from population studies and from primary care settings.

In ESEMeD, a European population study, Alonso et al. (2007) found a more modest number compared to that of the United States, with a lifetime prevalence of around 26% and a prevalence rate in the past year of almost

12%. Among all disorders, the most common mental disorders in the past year were anxiety disorders (6%) and mood disorders (4%). Although it may seem sensible to think that the actual prevalence rates are somewhere in-between the American and European ones, there are several reasons to assume that these still gravely underestimate the total rate of psychological dysfunction in the population.

A first reason is that several specific criteria are required in order to be officially diagnosed with a mental disorder. When failing to meet only one criterion, the person’s state would not be considered a mental disorder, but would still be associated with dysfunction or impairment. For depression, for example, there is a vast amount of evidence indicating that a dimensional approach is more valid than a categorical approach (Blatt, 1974; Haslam, 2003,

Kendler & Gardner, 1998; Ruscio & Ruscio, 2000; Solom et al., 2001, cited in

Luyten & Blatt, 2007). Several authors have therefore already suggested that, at least for this disorder, arbitrary consensus-based cut-off criteria should be open to re-evaluation (Brown & Barlow, 2005; First, 2005; Luyten & Blatt,

2007). A second point is made by Moffit et al. (2010), who remark that prevalence rates are often, if not always, determined using retrospective studies. However, such studies may undercount lifetime prevalence rates due to recall failure. In order to test this hypothesis, a prospective longitudinal study was conducted that determined the prevalence of lifetime disorder up to the age of 32 for anxiety and depression. Results showed that the prevalence rates were approximately doubled in prospective as compared to retrospective data for both disorder types. In general, there are strong indications that a vast

2

Chapter 1 part of the population is at least once in their lifetime confronted with some form of mental illness. Such a confrontation should not be taken lightly as mental disorders have a substantial contribution to disease. Their impact can be measured in terms of disability-adjusted-life years (DALYs), which is an integrative measure of overall disease burden (Murray & Lopez, 1996).

Worldwide, depression currently accounts for one tenth of all DALYs, which is similar to the 11% DALYs attributed to cancer (Prince et al., 2007). Projections of global mortality and burden of disease expect depressive disorders to be one of the three leading causes by 2030, together with HIV/AIDS and ischemic heart disease (Mathers & Loncar, 2006).

When primary care settings are considered in particular, screening is sometimes done here using the Diagnostic Manual of Mental Disorders Primary

Care Version (DSM-IV PC; American Psychiatric Association, 1995), but as

Williams, Noël, Cordes, Ramirez, and Pignone (2002) point out, more often short instruments are used like the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck,

Ward, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961), the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies

Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) or the Patient Health Questionnaire

(PHQ-9; Spitzer, Kroenke, & Williams, 1999). Although these instruments strictly cannot be used to diagnose mental disorders, these are often used as provisional ways of detecting them. Similar prevalence rates can also be found here, with depression in the past year for example between 1% and 10%

(Bartholomeeusen, Kim, & Mertens, 2005; Lamberts, Oskam, & Hofman-Okkes,

1994). The reason why these numbers appear lower compared to the larger epidemiological studies might be twofold. On the one hand general practitioners (GPs) can use slightly different criteria to determine specific mental disorders, which do not always correspond to those of manuals like the

DSM-IV-TR. Furthermore, GPs have to be vigilant for a very wide range of

(mental) disorders, some of which have low prevalence and incidence rates in the population and therefore might go undetected (Buntinx et al., 2004). On

3

General introduction the other hand, on average 69% of patients with depression and 76% of patients with mood or anxiety symptoms only report physical problems (De

Lepeleire, 2011). During a consult, time is often limited and both patients and

GPs have to prioritize which issues to address. When such competing demands are present, physical complaints receive more attention compared to psychological complaints (Klinkman, 1997), which reduces the number of detections of mental disorders.

In conclusion, mental disorders have a significant impact on the general population, and these are only expected to increase in the following decades.

This outlook is especially relevant for healthcare in high-income countries.

Although prevalence rates are also similar in low-income countries, the main priority there remains to address communicable diseases, whose share and impact is a much larger compared to high-income countries (WHO, 2010). For these high-income countries, mental healthcare (MHC) may already make up a larger portion of the healthcare system, but an additional increase in the number of people affected and seeking help should still be anticipated.

2

The current challenges for successful mental healthcare

Even when more conservative numbers are considered it appears that the majority of people with mental disorders lack access to professional care.

Currently, less than one in three receives treatment (Bebbington et al., 2000a;

Bebbington et al., 2000b, Kessler et al., 2005). In addition, a number of people who are currently receiving treatment might not even need professional help.

Lauber, Nordt, and Rössler (2005) presented mental health professionals with a vignette depicting either a person with schizophrenia, major depression or without any psychiatric symptoms (a ‘non-case’). For this non-case, over 50% proposed medical help, i.e. additional consults with psychologists and GPs.

Although a vignette study implies a hypothetical scenario and is therefore not completely similar to real-life situations, it does provide an indication that in a

4

Chapter 1 number of cases there might be a potential overtreatment in the absence of relevant symptomatology. A study by Tiemeier et al. (2002) that specifically aimed at the treatment of depression, showed more positive results, with only around 10% overtreatment. Such studies highlight the importance of reducing overtreatment, which implies – for one – a more accurate detection of mental disorders. However, the number of people who actually need help, but are lacking treatment is an even larger problem. Addressing this issue proves to be complex, as there are some barriers that need to be overcome. These barriers are situated at several levels: within the general public and the patients themselves, within the health professionals at the primary care level and within the policy makers. Each of these levels will be briefly discussed.

2.1

The general public and the patients

At the level of the general public, there are two main reasons as to why people are not receiving the necessary treatment: The first reason is a lack of knowledge. A relevant concept in this context is ‘mental health literacy’, which is defined as “the knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders which aid their recognition, management or prevention” (Jorm et al., 1997, p. 182). More specifically, mental health literacy consists of several components including knowledge and beliefs about risk factors and causes, available interventions, the possibility to recognize specific disorders, attitudes towards help seeking, and knowledge of how to seek information. One example of an intervention used to address this issue is the Australian ‘beyondblue’ campaign. In order to increase mental health literacy, public awareness activities were set up in a number of states, including the distribution of posters, pamphlets and postcards, a website with information, television advertising, advertisements in print media, and educational videos (Morgan & Jorm, 2007). When initiatives like ‘beyondblue’ are undertaken, the main idea is that this will lead to increased help seeking (Goldney & Fisher, 2008). The evidence for this claim is

5

General introduction mixed. Overall, such campaigns are successful, as different studies show that in recent years the public has gained more knowledge about causes and available interventions (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 2005; Jorm, Christensen, & Griffiths,

2005a), that they have become better at recognizing mental disorders

(Goldney, Fisher, Dal Grande, & Taylor, 2005; Jorm, Chirstensen, & Griffiths,

2005b) and that willingness to seek professional help has also increased

(Angermeyer & Matschinger, 2005; Mojtabai, 2007). However, it seems questionable that these changes also had an impact on the general attitude towards people with mental illness. A study by Angermeyer, Holzinger, and

Matschinger (2009) showed that in recent years the desire for social distance from people with depression even somewhat increased.

Knowledge alone seems insufficient, and therefore, there is also a second main reason why people are not receiving the necessary treatment: negative attitude. A large study by Lasalvia et al. (2012) conducted in 35 countries showed that nearly eighty percent of people with MDD reported some experiences of discrimination and almost one third stopped themselves from doing something important in their life because of stigma or discrimination.

Stigma can therefore be considered a serious barrier to social participation and to help seeking (Corrigan, Larson, & Rusch, 2009; Fuller, Edwards, Procter, &

Moss, 2000). There are nevertheless ways to reduce the reticence of the general public to participate in preventive group interventions, for example through awareness campaigns and mass social contact interventions that aim to reduce (self-) stigma. One example is ‘Time to Change’, the largest antistigma campaign in the UK ever (Henderson & Thornicroft, 2009). It did not have a strong focus on educating, but rather aimed at mass level social contact events through which members of the public with and without mental health problems engage with each other in order to reduce discrimination and stigma.

There is already preliminary evidence that such interventions can work on a mass level (Evans-Lacko et al., 2012). Combined with mental health literacy

6

Chapter 1 programs, such an approach may help to overcome barriers at the level of the general public.

2.2

The health professionals

Health professionals at the primary care level, like GPs, pharmacists and community facilitators can all play a role in the early detection, referral and care of people with mental disorders. However, these tasks require knowledge or skills, which sometimes appear to be lacking. Some studies have already shown that about half of patients with depression in primary care are not detected (Cepoiu et al., 2008) and that professionals in primary care require guidance to address the social needs of depressed patients (Barley, Murray,

Walters, & Tylee, 2011). The rate of success varies according to the background of the health professional. GPs can for example adequately detect depression in most of their patients, with around 10% missed and 15% false positives

(Mitchell, Vaze, & Rao, 2009). On the other hand, 73% of pharmacists in a

Belgian study by Scheerder, De Coster, and Van Audenhove (2008) indicated their lack of education in mental health issues being a barrier for providing depression care. Finally, mental health issues are rarely a part of the education of most community facilitators like clergy, police officers and teachers. For them, there is a clear need for training in mental health issues (Farrell &

Goebert, 2008; McCrae et al. 2005; Vermette, Pinals, & Appelbaum, 2005;

Walter, Gouze, & Lim, 2006), just as there is for nurses (Ayalon, Area &

Bornfeld, 2008). However, like for the general public, providing knowledge is not sufficient. Attention also has to be paid to the attitude of professionals. A number of studies have already shown that GPs – despite their above average knowledge of mental health – do not hold fewer stereotypes compared to the general public (Lauber, Nordt, & Rössler, 2006) and seem less optimistic about prognosis and long-term outcomes (Caldwell & Jorm, 2001). Similar results have been found for pharmacists (Scheerder et al., 2009). Finally, the opinion

7

General introduction of nurses seemed overall in line with those of GPs, although they are generally more favourable towards the use of psychotherapy (Lauber et al., 2005).

As for addressing these issues, different interventions exist that incorporate both knowledge and attitude change, like a Dutch GP training program by van

Os et al. (2002), which showed – among others – increased knowledge, a limited (non significant) increase in the recognition of mental disorders and significantly more patients receiving treatment according to clinical guidelines.

Similar other interventions seemed to be successful in supporting GPs (Hodges,

Inch, & Silver, 2001), nurses (Eisses et al., 2005), pharmacists (Bell, Whitehead,

Aslani, Sacker, & Chen, 2006) and the police (Watson, Corrigan, & Ottati, 2004) in their role as gatekeepers for mental health.

2.3

Policy

One way to reduce the incidence of mental disorders, or to at least reduce their impact, is through prevention and mental health promotion. There is strong research evidence for the benefits of prevention, especially when physical conditions are considered. This is partially reflected in the policies of the highincome countries. Initiatives that require little effort and/or are mandatory have high compliance rates. By means of vaccination, for example, over 90% of parents take action to protect their children from infectious diseases like diphtheria, tetanus and pertussis (WHO, 2011). Preventive interventions that require more complex behaviour, like systematic screening in the case of breast cancer prevention, or making lifestyle changes to avoid metabolic syndrome, are far more difficult to achieve (Ahlin & Billhult, 2012). Furthermore, evaluating the merits of such interventions is difficult, and substantial time may be required before effects are noticed and interventions ‘pay off’. These are just some of the reasons that can account for why the budget for the prevention of non-communicable diseases is rather small, compared to that of sick care. The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development

8

Chapter 1

(OECD), comprising twenty high-income economies, estimates that on average

3% of the total health expenditure goes towards prevention, while the most is spend on sick care (OECD, 2010).

When the MHC budget of any European country is considered, it is dwarfed by the total healthcare budget. More specifically, around 6% is on average spent on mental health.

Within these constrained conditions, mental health promotion and the prevention of mental disorders have to operate. Drawing parallels with the prevention of physical conditions, not surprisingly, the available budget is minuscule: about or below 1% of the total MHC budget

(Regional Office for Europe of the World Health Organization, 2010). It goes without saying that such limited funding hampers the development of preventive interventions and is certainly one of the main reasons for the lagging of preventive interventions in the field of mental health. However, these data can also be interpreted in a positive way, as a significant part of the strong focus on sick care can actually be tackled at a policy making level. For example, if for any European country efforts lead to a shift or reallocation of

0.06% of the total healthcare budget to the field of the prevention of mental disorders, this would instantly double the available means. Such actions could certainly have a strong impact on the field of prevention and the development or improvement of preventive initiatives.

2.4

Conclusion

There are a number of barriers, outlined above, that hamper the access for people with mental disorders to professional care. A lack of knowledge and attitude in the general population, patients and professionals can be overcome through targeted interventions. Furthermore, at a policy level, more attention should be paid to the limited resources an important sector like MHC has to deal with. More resources could for example be used to increase the number of healthcare providers that – even with the small number of people actually

9

General introduction seeking treatment – are already actually overwhelmed by requests for help.

However, solely increasing the number of healthcare providers does not seem to be a valid solution. In most areas of primary care, short appointments are offered to a large number of patients. This is often referred to as ‘low contact, high volume’. Psychological therapies, on the other hand, offer one-toone interventions of substantial duration to relatively few patients. These are

‘high contact, low volume’ interventions (France, 1995, cited in Brown,

Boardman, Whittinger, & Ashworth, 2010). For both types of intervention, the number of people in need greatly outrank the number of MHC professionals, in the USA even at 107 to one (Hoge et al., 2007). This makes it impossible to reach all of those in need. Even doubling the work force might have little impact (Kazdin & Blase, 2011). One of the major topics in the current mental health policy therefore remains to create easily accessible facilities for people with mental health problems (WHO International Consortium in Psychiatric

Epidemiology, 2000).

One way to improve the access to and efficiency of MHC is through a stepped-care approach. This represents an attempt to maximize the efficiency of resource allocation in therapy. Low threshold and low cost interventions are offered first, and more intensive and costly interventions are reserved for those who are not sufficiently helped by the initial intervention (Everaert, Scheerder,

De Coster, Van Audenhove, 2007; Haaga, 2000). Studies that compared the effectiveness of a stepped care approach with care as usual (CAU) have shown that stepped care is more effective in reducing the risk of onset of mental disorders (van‘t Veer-Tazelaar et al., 2009) and is equally effective as CAU in treating mental disorders (van Straten, Tiemens, Hakkaart, Nolen, & Donker,

2006). Intensive and costly interventions are already well established (Andrews,

Issakidis, Sanderson, Corry, & Lapsley, 2004), but a further extension of the portfolio of models of delivery for primary MHC is needed. When extending the portfolio, attention should be paid not only to effectiveness of interventions,

10

Chapter 1 but also to the aspects of their dissemination and implementation. Most of all however, such an extension can only be successful if it is embedded in a broader paradigm shift. The MHC system currently has a strong focus on curating and although this is helpful for a substantial number of people, the sector will need to move out of its comfort zone in order to better reach all of those in need. This requires – at least partially – moving from curing to educating, coaching and promoting mental health which will be highlighted in the next chapter.

3

A paradigm shift from curing to educating, coaching and

promoting

The suggested paradigm shift for MHC in which the sector moves from curing to educating, coaching and promoting mental health is not only reasonable from a practical point of view, but can also be supported from theory. We will first take a look at some theoretical background. Subsequently, a number of aspects that are characteristic for the new paradigm will be highlighted, followed by some methodological implications when studying interventions within this new paradigm.

3.1

The theoretical background

The main rationale for implementing a new paradigm actually lies in how mental disorders are classified. Throughout the years, there has been increasing support for a shift from a categorical to a dimensional view on mental disorders. Unfortunately, there is only some support for this view in the new, fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder

(DSM-V). However a consensus has appeared in the discussions concerning its controversial conceptualization (Uher, 2012), which Brown and Barlow (2005) formulate as follows: “current psychosocial treatments have become overly

11

General introduction specialized because they focus on disorder-specific features … neglecting broader dimensions that are more germane to favourable long-term outcomes ”. In their opinion, higher order dimensions could be incorporated in the DSM, representing general vulnerabilities, which would make the instrument become highly relevant to the primary and secondary prevention of mental disorders. Stein et al. (2010) continue on this line of thought and indicate that the distinction already has eroded between psychopathology and normal psychological phenomena (e.g. sadness after a major stressful event).

Such a dimensional view on mental disorders therefore implies an extended focus beyond sick care, with an increase in the share of prevention and early intervention in MHC.

If the mental health intervention spectrum is considered however (Figure

1), most of our efforts are still in the ‘treatment’ category, whereas the categories ‘promotion’ and ‘prevention’ have previously received only limited attention (Muñoz, Cuijpers, Smit, Barrera, & Leykin, 2010). There is however substantial evidence that mentally healthy adults – individuals who were free of a mental disorder in the past year and were flourishing – miss less days of work, have the healthiest psychosocial functioning, the lowest risk of cardiovascular disease, the lowest number of chronic physical diseases with age, the fewest health limitation of activities of daily living and lower healthcare utilization. The prevalence of flourishing is only twenty percent in the adult population, though (Keyes, 2007). Making the efforts to create a more accessible and extended primary care is therefore a logical step, but should not be noncommittal, as more is involved than merely increasing the existing portfolio of services. In view of the paradigm shift, the position of MHC organizations in society actually evolves from instances that primarily treat, to services that (also) provide guidance and education. By changing MHC and offering an increased number of primary care services, MHC can convey a strong and positive message: mental health is more than the absence of mental

12

Chapter 1 illness and everyone can learn how to maintain or improve his or her mental health. However, for those who struggle and who need additional help or guidance, individual ambulatory consultation or even residential treatment is still available.

Figure 1.

The mental health intervention spectrum (National Research Council

& Institute for Medical Research, 2009).

3.2

A paradigm shift: characteristics of a new approach

Shifting from one paradigm to another is not something that can happen overnight, it is rather a gradual effortful process, as the new paradigm has to build on existing systems and structures. In order to successfully extend primary care services and give them a clear focus, there are a few characteristics of the current paradigm that impede such a shift and therefore

13

General introduction require to be changed. These are highlighted below and some suggestions are also made as how to successfully overcome each of them.

3.2.1

From referral to self-referral

In general, when people experience mental health problems and seek professional help, the first person they turn to is their GP. Not surprisingly, a large proportion of a GPs’ daily workload consists of ‘common’ mental health problems like depression and anxiety (Goldberg & Huxley, 1992). A significant part of their primary care function in MHC involves prescribing medication

(Metaforum, 2010) and referring to psychological therapies (National Institute for Clinical Excellence, 2009; cited in Brown, Boardman, Whittinger, &

Ashworth, 2010). However, only less than half of the GPs actually refer to such therapies, a study of elderly GPs in the UK has shown (Collins, Katona, & Orrell,

1997). One of the main reasons for this limited referring is a lack of knowledge about the different psychological therapies that are available. Furthermore,

GPs face daily challenges of urgent and competing demands for their limited attention and resources (Lin, Simon, Katzelnick, & Pearson, 2001). In such circumstances, prescribing medication takes only little effort and often also meets the demands of the patient, who is looking for a ‘quick solution’ and might not be interested in (costly) time consuming psychological therapies.

However, taking depression as an example, antidepressants are not that effective in treating mild to moderate depression (van der Lem, van der Wee, van Veen, & Zitman, 2012). So, in the long run, complaints may aggravate, patients return to their GPs. More intensive (psychological) treatments are required and also preferred by the patient at that point (Raue, Schulberg,

Moonseong, Klimstra, & Bruce, 2009; Steidtmann et al., 2012).

For the reasons mentioned above, targeted referral to interventions in the field of primary MHC has not yet reached its full potential. An alternative way for people to participate in such interventions is therefore often through self-

14

Chapter 1 referral: an intervention is promoted to the general public and attracts selfregistering community dwellers. This approach has some advantages: people are for example not required to disclose their issues to their GP or anyone else in order to participate, which otherwise might create an additional threshold

(Mischoulon et al., 2001). The people who are attracted by these interventions and make use of self-registration can nonetheless have very different profiles.

If they already have elevated levels of complaints and decide to self-refer to a low-intensity primary care intervention, the intervention might not fully meet their needs. This furthermore also creates high heterogeneity in the participant population, which is not beneficial for the group of participants as a whole.

Still, the benefits of self-referral do not seem to outweigh those of professional referral. Therefore, it would still be preferred to further stimulate specific referral of potential participants. One way is by informing (mental) health professionals like GPs on the effectiveness of preventive interventions and how to detect patients who are eligible for participation (Lin et al., 2001).

3.2.2

From individual to collective approaches

Individual psychotherapy is currently the dominant model of treatment delivery (Kazdin & Blase, 2011). In a confidential setting, a therapeutic alliance is created between a therapist and a patient in which they jointly try to overcome the issues the patient presents him- or herself with. As mentioned earlier, however, this is a very time consuming approach that is not really suitable for preventive interventions, which need to reach a vast amount of people. An alternative is therefore a collective approach, which allows one professional to simultaneously reach a large group of individuals. Well-known examples are stress management courses (Van Daele, Hermans, Van

Audenhove, & Van den Bergh, 2012) and (childhood) depression prevention programs (Stice, Shaw, Bohon, Marti, & Rohde, 2009), where the professional is considered the teacher and the participants are students. Such approaches

15

General introduction may entail certain benefits, aside from the more effective use of the limited means available. Validation and social comparison offer the opportunity for participants to see that they are not struggling alone. They may also learn additional tips and tricks from other participants, and when in need of assistance, they can also reach out to fellow participants for social support

(Burlingame, 2010). Not only have such interventions proven to be effective, but because of the efficient use of resources, they also offer good value for money (Mihalopoulos, Vos, Pirkis, & Carter, 2012; Van Daele et al., 2012).

Despite these advantages, the attractiveness of group preventive interventions remains limited. There are a number of aspects that may account for this, and an important one highlighted earlier is the stigma related to mental health issues. As previously mentioned, there are nevertheless ways to reduce the reticence of the general public to participate in preventive group interventions. One way to do this is through awareness campaigns and mass social contact interventions that aim to reduce (self)-stigma (Evans-Lacko et al.,

2012).

3.2.3

From traditional psychological treatment to education and skill training

‘Traditional’ psychological treatments start with patients seeking help for specific problems. These initial problems give treatments a clear focus: the target for patients is to learn how to deal with them or how to overcome them.

When they manage to do so, in a second phase, they can use the acquired knowledge or skills to further optimize and improve their quality of life.

Guidance by a professional is at this point not really necessary, but rather optional and a matter of personal choice. Preventive (group) interventions work the other way around. The initial focus of such interventions is quite large and not specific: participants do not necessarily have specific complaints or symptoms, and even when they do, these are (preferably) still subclinical.

16

Chapter 1

Contrary to traditional psychological treatment, people who participate in group preventive interventions are not required to share their deepest thoughts or feelings in group or with anyone in particular. During the intervention they are rather considered students, who are educated and who receive general information, for example on stress or depression. Furthermore, they are taught skills to deal with both future and current problems. Although these techniques can also be highly specific, teachers do not explicitly determine which techniques should be used in which situations and leave this up to the participants. When participants actually have to face their problems, no professional is present to provide guidance. With their background information and the techniques they have learned, participants are at that point expected be able to manage their issues themselves.

Preventive (group) interventions therefore require a substantial amount of initiative from individuals. A theoretical model like Prochaska and Diclemente’s

(1985) ‘Stages of Change’ can be used to illustrate this. In the first phase, which is coined ‘precontemplation’, individuals are unaware of the fact that their mental well-being is at risk and are not motivated to deal with their problems.

In a second phase, which is known as the ‘contemplation’ phase, participants have to acknowledge they are lacking skills or information to adequately deal with their everyday problems and that their problems might get out of hand if no action is undertaken. In a third phase, ‘preparation stage’, individuals take some limited action. Because this action is not systematic or coordinated, success is varying. At this point, individuals are susceptible for initiatives like preventive (group) interventions. If information on the intervention reaches them and there are no additional barriers to enrolling, they can become participants. Specific obstacles can for example be attitudes, norms, perceived control, self-efficacy, habits, etc. (Reasoned Action Approach, Fishbein & Ajzen,

2010). The next phase, ‘action’, comprises the actual intervention. Individuals need to participate and invest a significant amount of time and energy in

17

General introduction learning how to deal with these (future) problems. In the fifth and final phase,

‘maintenance’, what was taught and learned during the intervention should be kept in mind and trained, in order to prevent relapse.

It goes without saying that undertaking an endeavour as described above without additional encouragement and support from peers or professionals, requires substantial self-insight, problem solving skills, and courage. Ironically, these are just the qualities that the people who are most in need of such interventions are lacking and that the interventions are intended to help foster.

A relevant concept in this context is ‘empowerment’. Empowerment can be defined as "… an intentional, ongoing process through which people lacking an equal share of valued resources gain greater access to and control over those resources" . It offers individuals the opportunity to gain control over their lives and over democratic participation in the life of their community (Berger &

Neuhaus, 1977, cited in Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Empowerment recognizes the vulnerabilities and limitations of individuals, but primarily focuses on their potential and strengths. Empowering in MHC implies 1) a positive basic attitude and 2) suitable participation. A positive basic attitude implies that all people have an inherent capacity to learn, grow, and change.

Professionals should be open, available and respectful towards their patients. A hierarchical relationship is not imposed, because the goal is to establish a true partnership. Suitable participation means that deciding on treatment or help is a collaborative effort which is established through an intensive dialogue between professionals and help seekers (Van Regenmortel, 2009). Yet again, the importance of competent referrers, preferably (mental) health professionals like GPs, is therefore emphasized. Together with peers and family and by listening to people who seek help, they can stimulate and motivate potential participants by giving them the necessary information and courage to deal with their problems.

18

Chapter 1

3.2.4

Illustrations of primary care interventions in line with this new paradigm

There are a number of primary care interventions that fit well within this new paradigm. Two categories will be discussed and for both categories one example will be highlighted.

Group interventions.

A first category of primary care interventions are group psychoeducational interventions, whose goal is twofold: transfer of knowledge and the acquisition of skills. Both goals are aimed at in group sessions and through homework assignments. Groups can be drawn from school classes, associations, companies, primary healthcare units, or neighbourhood organizations. In some cases, groups are even self-registered through media advertisements. Psychoeducation can be considered an independent intervention within the framework of a cognitive-behavioural approach.

According to Bäuml, Froböse, Kraemer, Rentrop, and Pitschel Walz (2006) a

‘proper’ group psychoeducational approach has the following characteristics: teaching should be key, while other techniques – as relaxation, for example – only serve to support these teaching activities. The teaching is provided through standardized, non-individualized formats for each participant. During the course, participants first receive information about the topic and how to cope with it. In a second phase, they independently need to process and implement this information. Although they are empowered to apply the information to their personal lives and to develop skills that can help to improve their situation, it is the responsibility of each participant to put into practice what has been learned in the psychoeducational course.

One example is a psychoeducational intervention for stress called ‘Stress

Control’, a local adaptation of a program developed by White (2000; adaptation by ISW-Limits, 2006). Psychoeducational interventions for stress are aimed at reducing (perceived) stress, rather than preventing it. Nevertheless, these can

19

General introduction still be considered as preventive interventions, for instance, given the link between high levels of stress and the subsequent onset of mental disorders like depression (van Praag, 2004). This specific intervention comprises six weekly lessons of two hours. In these lessons participants are offered general information on stress and related psychopathology, and are learned basic selfhelp tips and techniques for short-term stress reduction. Examples are controlling bodily sensations through progressive relaxation and breathing exercises, cognitive techniques like challenging dysfunctional thoughts, and also some practical techniques on problem-solving and ending safety behaviours. Through homework assignments participants learn to apply the general information to their own personal situation, and to create a personalized frame of reference for the course content.

Online self-help interventions.

Online self-help interventions fall under emental health, which can be seen as “… a generic term to describe the use of information and communication technology – in particular the many technologies related to the internet – when these technologies are used to support and improve mental health conditions and mental healthcare” (Riper et al., 2010). E-mental health interventions have proven to be effective, efficient and cost-effective for depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress, eating disorders and a wide variety of other forms of psychopathology (Griffiths,

Farrer, & Christensen, 2010). Most of the time, interventions are based on cognitive-behavioural therapy (CBT; Andrews, Cuijpers, Craske, McEvoy, &

Titov, 2010), although recently the first studies based on psychodynamic therapy have also been conducted, which show similar positive results for depression and anxiety (Andersson, et al. 2012; Johansson et al., 2012). In addition to treatment, e-mental health interventions can also be used for prevention and in primary care, in which they have a large potential to reduce disease burden primarily because they permit a low-cost, widespread dissemination (Christensen & Hickie, 2010). Their delivery can occur through a

20

Chapter 1 partnership within existing primary care structures (Hickie et al., 2010) or through a virtual clinic environment supervised by health professionals

(Andrews & Titov, 2010). The first option has the advantage of being a part of the regular mental health services and can also offer a first step towards access to more traditional person-based services for those who might not otherwise seek care (Ruggiero et al., 2006). The latter has the advantage of anonymity and easy access for people who experience stigma concerning their mental health problems. Both options are not mutually exclusive as an intervention can be tailored for both delivery methods.

One example is the ‘Kleur je Leven’ (Colour your Life) intervention.

Originally developed by the Trimbos Institute and based on the psychoeducational ‘Coping with Depression’ course (Cuijpers, Muñoz, Clarke, &

Lewinsohn, 2009), a Flemish version has been adapted by ISW-Limits (2009).

Participants can enrol independently on http://www.kleurjeleven.be or can be targeted towards the intervention by their GP. This online cognitive behavioural self-help intervention helps them cope with mild to moderate depressive feelings. In eight weekly lessons (and one booster session) participants are taught insights and skills to deal with depressive feelings. The website makes use of streaming video material of ‘models’ (actors playing participants), voice-overs, interactive exercises, a mood diary, homework and a workbook. Each lesson has a fixed structure in which 1) the topic of the lesson is introduced, 2) the participant completes a questionnaire on the homework from the previous lesson, and 3) the curriculum with some assignments is run through. Afterwards 4) instructions are delivered for the new homework assignment and 5) the lesson is evaluated. The specific knowledge and skills consist of: 1) information concerning the origin of depressive complaints and the relationships between, thinking, acting and feeling, 2) techniques to improve relaxation, 3) techniques to plan and undertake more fun activities, 4) techniques to reduce worrying, 5) skills in constructive thinking, through

21

General introduction detecting mood deteriorating thoughts and challenging these in order to improve participant’s mood, 6) skills in coping with problems experienced with

(significant) others, and 7) increased assertiveness by expressing feelings and thoughts and standing up for themselves (ISW Limits, 2009).

3.3

The methodological implications when studying primary care interventions like group interventions

There are roughly two different ways of conducting research to determine the effects of interventions similar to the ones described above: in carefully controlled clinical trials or in a more applied context. The first, focusing on efficacy is known for its high internal validity, but results are more difficult to generalize. The latter, paying attention to effectiveness is high on external validity, but is not as carefully controlled. Efficacy has primarily been favoured by the research community, whereas the practice community prefers effectiveness (Stricker, 2000). Because research is often conducted in highly controlled academic settings, the efficacy of most interventions is well documented. There is however a sharp contrast with the limited knowledge as to how effective they are in everyday practice. Over the years, there has been an increasing demand for research that bridges this gap (Glasgow, Lichtenstein,

& Marcus, 2003) and that evaluates the effectiveness of interventions (Jane-

Llopis, Hosman, & Saxena, 2004). This requires additional implementation in real-life circumstances. When primary care interventions are evaluated however, a number of issues that hamper the research process can be encountered: not only for the actual implementation of the intervention, but also for the interpretation of results. A number of these issues, both from literature and own experience, are highlighted below.

22

Chapter 1

3.3.1

The participant profile

People who are attracted by a preventive group intervention can have very different profiles. Without systematic screening, a group may for example include participants who are simply interested in learning about a specific mental health problem, but also those who already have elevated symptoms and are hoping for quick symptom reduction. The variety of participants’ interests matches the variety of goals of these interventions, because their main focus is not on curing, but rather on educating and giving people the necessary support and tools to self manage and safeguard their mental health.

Because such interventions are often set-up in the local community with local partners, this provides a low threshold for participation. Participants can therefore easily commence such a course, but when the intervention does not meet their expectations, they can also as easily cease to participate and drop out.

Furthermore, within the group of participants, mixed results are often obtained. Some participants can show strong declines in symptoms and experience great benefits, whereas the symptoms of others remain stable or even deteriorate (Van Daele et al., 2012). Because an intervention can also attract a large number of participants who choose to follow the course out of curiosity, this translates in few cases with quite low symptom profiles at baseline, possibly causing floor effects. An intervention might therefore have seemingly little effect, both in the short and in the long term. In such case, it would nevertheless be premature to conclude that the intervention is ineffective, because also for this particular subgroup of participants, there may be long-term advantages. When they are facing life events or aversive conditions in the future, they may be able to rely on knowledge from the intervention to better cope with them. Because of the low frequency of such events, it is however difficult to measure such effects. In conclusion, the participant profile for effectiveness studies has two main challenges for

23

General introduction researchers: 1) to have a clear idea of which participants are attracted by a specific intervention and 2) to try to accurately predict the effectiveness for an intervention not as a whole, but for the individual participant.

3.3.2

Determining intervention effects

Because of this divergent participant profile, there may also be some unclarity as to when an intervention is considered successful. Without a doubt, the most suitable outcome for a preventive intervention should be the long-term reduction of the probability of (the targeted) mental disorder for intervention participants. This requires follow-up of sufficiently long duration. Because means are typically limited, follow-up periods are often insufficiently long to fully measure the effects of a preventive intervention. Therefore, the shortterm reduction of (secondary) symptoms like stress, worrying, or ruminating are also considered, as high levels of these symptoms are known to precede more serious forms of psychopathology. A combination of both short-term (not necessarily lasting) and long-term effects being measured and documented is to be preferred (Stice et al., 2009).

3.3.3

A suitable research design

The randomised controlled trial (RCT) is often referred to as the ‘gold standard’ of research designs, the most convincing form of evidence to show that an intervention is truly effective (Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine,

2011). The concept behind RCTs is simple: study participants are randomly assigned to an intervention or a (waiting list or placebo or non-intervention) control group. Subsequently, one or multiple changes are applied to the intervention group (for example participating in a stress management course), whereas all other factors are held constant for the control group. Comparing measures before and after a period of intervention administration subsequently allows for causal inferences of intervention effectiveness. The

24

Chapter 1 methodology of an RCT basically mimics the methodology of laboratory experiments, implying full control of the environment of the participant and truly random (and blinded) allocation of participants to the experimental conditions.

There is no reason to contest the use of an RCT, if the context allows it. In the context of applied knowledge of primary care interventions, however, an

RCT is sometimes not preferable or possible. However, valuable alternatives exist. Examples are the matched control design, the interrupted time-series

(ITS) design or the multiple baseline design. In a matched control design participants from the intervention group are matched with controls based on a number of variables believed to be confounders (Rothman & Greenland, 1998).

An important step in this design is the selection of confounding variables to use as matching variables, as a wrong match may result in a loss of efficiency (Rose

& van der Laan, 2008). Time-series design makes use of a string of consecutive observations which are interrupted by the introduction of an intervention to see whether the slope or level of the series changes following the intervention

(Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002). This design is especially appropriate when it is known at what specific point an intervention will occur in prospective studies, or when it occurred in retrospective studies (Mercer, DeVinney, Fine,

Green, & Dougherty, 2007). Finally, a multiple baseline design is a form of ITS design, most often used during the development of interventions, when combinations of components within effective interventions are being tested

(Biglan, Ary, & Wagenaar, 2000). For a multiple baseline design it is crucial to have instruments that are suitable for repeated measurements and to have a clear idea what a stable baseline, is and how far apart intervention should be staggered (Mercer et al., 2007). As highlighted above, just like with the RCT, these designs have their strengths and merits in specific situations, but they also require a number of conditions to be met in order to produce valid results.

These designs can therefore be revalued for intervention studies in the field of

25

General introduction prevention by giving more and explicit attention to the design aspects, for example by using tools like the ‘TREND Statement’ (Des Jarlais, Lyles, Crepaz, & the Trend Group, 2004). This checklist is designed for intervention evaluation studies using nonrandomised designs and emphasizes 1) describing of the intervention, 2) including the theoretical base, 3) describing of the comparison condition, 4) full reporting of outcomes and 5) inclusion of information related to the design needed to assess possible biases in outcome data. The ultimate goal is to improve the quality of data reporting in peer-reviewed publications so that the conduct and findings of studies are transparent. This in turn could lead to an increased appreciation of non-randomised intervention studies as a valid source for evidence-based practice and also allows for comparable information across studies to be more easily consolidated and translated into general knowledge and practice.

3.3.4

The implementation considerations

When pre-developed interventions are implemented in real-life contexts, there is a strong tension between the principles of fidelity and adaptation. The concept of ‘implementation fidelity’ refers to “the degree to which an intervention or program is delivered as intended” (Carroll, 2007). Specifically, a successful implementation is one that abides four components of fidelity: adherence, exposure, quality of program delivery, and participant responsiveness (Dane & Schneider, 1998). Mihalic (2002) describes each of these components as follows: 1) ‘adherence’ refers to whether interventions are delivered as intended, 2) ‘exposure’ refers to the number of sessions implemented, session length, frequency of implementation of program techniques, 3) ‘quality of program delivery’ refers to the manner in which staff delivers a program, and 4) ‘participant responsiveness’ refers to the extent to which participants are involved in program content. Hasson (2010) has suggested two additional factors that moderate implementation fidelity,

26

Chapter 1 notably ‘recruitment’ and ‘context’. The concept of 5) ‘recruitment’ refers to procedures that are used to attract potential program participants, whereas 6)

‘context’ refers to surrounding social systems, such as structures and cultures of groups, inter-organizational linkages, and historical as well as concurrent events. All these factors should be evaluated when conducting a process evaluation. On the other hand, focusing on fidelity has also been criticized for being rigid, as it assumes full compliance with the program as prescribed by the program developer (Gresham et al., 2003). The fact that any change to the program made by implementers is considered a bias and a threat to implementation quality is at odds with the value placed on stakeholder involvement and participation in health promotion (World Health Organization,

1986; Levy, Baldyga, & Jurkowski, 2003). An alternative approach to program implementation is therefore to encourage adaptation rather than limit it. When an intervention is implemented in the field, this therefore requires careful balancing both principles. It is even more important when the implementation of an intervention is also combined with evaluating its effectiveness. In such a context, any unwanted or undocumented change to the intervention could inadvertently lead to the invalid conclusion that an intervention is less effective than it really is.

3.3.5

The importance of process evaluation

When an intervention is adapted, it is subjected to subtle changes. Therefore, an important part of intervention research is the process evaluation. This is necessary to understand the intervention results and enables program management and accountability, which are essential to effective implementation (Bartholomew, Parcel, Kok, & Gottlieb, 2011). This is especially relevant when an intervention is evaluated in the field, because the research component, which is necessary during the evaluation, is not always present during standard implementation. There will therefore often be a slight

27

General introduction difference between an intervention which is being investigated and the one that is afterwards broadly disseminated. This points at the importance of carefully considering factors that might influence outcomes and otherwise could lead to faulty conclusions. A process evaluation of a study can help to overcome these issues and bring some clarity.

As an illustration, one important aspect to consider in the research on primary care interventions is the determinants of non-participatory behaviour.

The rationale for such analysis was found in a study by Brouwer et al. (2009), who evaluated the motivation for people to use an online cognitivebehavioural intervention. According to them – and in line with McGuire’s

(1985) ‘Persuasion Communication Matrix’ and Rogers’ (2003) ‘Diffusion of

Innovation theory’ – the following determinants for participating were applicable: 1) user characteristics, 2) intervention characteristics, and 3) characteristics of the source. Azjen (1988) found that user characteristics comprise personal characteristics (like age and gender), individual cognitions

(like attitudes, perceived control and the intention to participate) and motivation. Characteristics of the intervention are complexity, first impressions and advantages for participating and finally, characteristics of the source are credibility and reliability. Aside from typical barriers for primary care interventions, which were highlighted in the previous chapter, performing research on these interventions adds additional barriers. As for user characteristics, some potential patients might for example have a negative attitude towards research. Although these attitudes are generally positive, some people do not prefer to participate in research, primarily because of a fear of the unknown and resentments towards randomisations (Madsen et al.,

2002). These randomisations, a typical characteristic of intervention research, might also be one of the most radical differences between an intervention with and without a research component. As this implies the possibility of a waitlist control, an alternative (placebo) treatment or a non-intervention control, this

28

Chapter 1 can create a threshold for people who might initially be interested, but are reluctant to participate in the prospect of ‘possibly ending up in the wrong condition’. Finally, as for the characteristics of the source: when conducting an intervention with a research component, a research centre is often involved, which might not be well known to the public, or might appear to be less reliable or credible as opposed to other primary care services. This may scare away potential participants. This is one of the easiest barriers to remedy, as a strong involvement of partners in the field, also in the promotion and communication concerning the study, may help overcome this issue.

4

The aim of this PhD

As highlighted earlier, the choice for an increase in primary care interventions is a bare necessity, as it may help to address to continuing lack of professional care for a significant amount of people with mental health issues. Furthermore, it also helps to facilitate a paradigm shift, in which a more positive view of mental health is portrayed and people learn how to rely on their own strengths and skills in order to safeguard their mental health.

A main goal of this PhD was to evaluate the effectiveness of a local adaptation of a psychoeducational intervention for stress. As such primary care interventions are still a rare commodity in Flanders, this effectiveness trial could provide policy makers with relevant, local information as to how such interventions perform in their local contexts. However, the goal of the PhD extended beyond this. Aside from the evaluation of the Stress Control course, we also wanted to gain additional insight in the overall evidence base for psychoeducational interventions that focus on stress reduction. Therefore we decide to conduct a systematic review of the literature, followed by a meta-

29

General introduction analysis. The results of both endeavours are a first part of the publications, labelled ‘effectiveness’.

During the evaluation, we were also confronted with several implementation issues. These made us reconsider how researchers and their partners in the field collaborate. We thought about how their (sometimes conflicting) views could be reconciled, while still maintaining the high standards of scientific research. This tension between adaptation and fidelity during intervention implementation, and how to resolve it, is discussed in the second part of the publications, labelled ‘implementation’.

As a third point of attention, an attempt was made to predict individual success for intervention participants. The autobiographical memory task, a measure for autobiographical memory specificity, was used to predict individual changes in problem solving skills for intervention participants. Furthermore, this measure was also explored as a possible tool to predict the evolution of mental health in a natural context, for individuals from the general population. Both attempts are discussed in greater detail in the third part of the publications, labelled

‘prediction’.

30

Research topic 1

Effectiveness

Van Daele, T., Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2012). Stress reduction through psychoeducation: a meta-analytic review. Health Education &

Behavior, 39 , 474-485. doi: 10.1177/1090198111419202

Van Daele, T., Van Audenhove, C., Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., & Van den Bergh,

O. (2013). Effectiveness of a Six Session Stress Reduction Program for Groups.

Manuscript submitted for publication

31

32

Chapter 2

Stress reduction through psychoeducation: a metaanalytic review

The aim of this meta-analysis was to evaluate the effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions in reducing stress and to gain more insight in determining features moderating the magnitude of effects. Relevant studies were selected from 1990 to

2010 and were included according to predetermined criteria. For each study, the standardized mean difference was calculated for the outcome measure primarily related to stress. Nineteen studies met the inclusion criteria; for 16 studies,a standardized mean difference could be calculated. The average effect size was .27 (95% confidence interval = *.14, .40+) at posttest and .20 (95% confidence interval = *−.04,

.43]) at follow-up. To determine possible moderators of intervention effects, all 19 studies were included. Only interventions that were shorter in duration provided better results. When a model with multiple moderators was considered, a model combining both intervention duration and the number of women in an intervention was significant and accounted for 42% of the variability found in the data set. Specifically, interventions with more women that were shorter in duration obtained better results.

Keywords meta-analysis, psychoeducation, reduction, review, stress

33

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

1

Introduction

Worldwide, people of different ages and backgrounds are facing stress.

Researchers found a vast increase of stress for adults as well as teenagers and children in the past decade. As an example, nearly a quarter of the respondents who were interviewed by the American Psychological Association (APA) for their annual national stress report indicated they were experiencing a high level (8, 9, or 10 on a 10-point scale) of stress (APA, 2009). In 2010, about 44% of the Americans said they had experienced an increase in stress over the past

5 years (APA, 2010). Although a certain amount of life stress is inevitable and can be beneficial for an individual, it is now widely acknowledged that chronic stress is a major health burden, both physically and mentally. High levels of self-perceived stress are, for example, closely related to the metabolic syndrome (Chandola, Brunner, & Marmot, 2006), to coronary heart disease

(Rosengren et al., 2004), and to ischemic stroke (Jood, Redfors, Rosengren,

Blomstrand, & Jern, 2009). There is also a clear link between high levels of stress and the subsequent onset of mental health disorders such as depression

(van Praag, 2004; Wang, 2004). One way to improve the efficiency and access to MHC is through stepped care, in order to use healthcare resources at an optimal level. Low-cost interventions are offered first, and more intensive and costly interventions are reserved for those who are not sufficiently helped by the initial intervention (Haaga, 2000). Because intensive and costly interventions are already well established (Andrews, Issakidis, Sanderson,

Corry, & Lapsley, 2004), further extension of primary MHC through interventions with low financial and accessibility thresholds are needed

(Bebbington, Brugha, et al.,2000; Bebbington, Meltzer, et al., 2000). A technique often used to manage stress is psychoeducation. The goal of psychoeducation is to help people acquire competencies to manage stress and

34

Chapter 2 preserve their mental health. The transfer of knowledge and the acquisition of skills are reached in individual encounters, in group sessions, and/or through homework assignments. Preventive psychoeducation is primarily offered to groups. Oftentimes healthcare providers make use of group sessions, but the

Internet or self-help groups are also valid options. Groups can be drawn from school classes, associations, companies, primary healthcare units, or neighborhood organizations. In some cases, groups are self-registered through media advertisements.

Psycheducation can be considered an independent intervention within the framework of a cognitive–behavioural approach (Bäuml, Froböse, Kraemer,

Rentrop, & Pitschel-Walz, 2006). In line with these authors, we adopted the following criteria for what should constitute a ‘proper’ group psychoeducational intervention: Teaching should be key, whereas other techniques — such as relaxation, for example — only serve to support these teaching activities. The teaching is provided through standardized, nonindividualized formats for each participant. During the course, participants first receive information about stress and how to cope with it. In a second phase, they independently need to process and implement this information. Although they are empowered to apply the information to their personal lives and develop skills that can help improve their situation, it is the responsibility of each participant to put into practice what has been learned in the psychoeducational course. Psychoeducational interventions for stress are aimed at reducing (perceived) stress rather than preventing it. Nevertheless, these can still be considered as preventive interventions, given, for example, the link between high levels of stress and the subsequent onset of a mental health disorder such as depression (van Praag, 2004). Preventive psychoeducation, in general, has been the subject of a large number of reviews, but the main focus has mostly been the prevention of depression in specific populations, such as children and adolescents (Andrews & Wilkinson,

35

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

2002; Gladstone & Beardslee, 2009; Merry, 2007; Merry, McDowell, Hetrick,

Bir, & Muller, 2004; Merry & Spence, 2007; Neil & Christensen, 2009).

Sometimes adults are targeted (Barrera, Torres, & Muñoz, 2007), even though reviews on the effects of psychoeducation on stress have typically focused on occupational stress (van der Klink, Blonk, Schene, & van Dijk, 2001). The present meta-analysis will focus on psychoeducation for the reduction of stress in the general population (i.e., participants with no predetermined or specific

[risk for] pathology). Both overall effects and specific moderators of effects will be analyzed. For the latter, the study of Stice, Shaw, Bohon, Marti, and Rohde

(2009) has been used as a source of inspiration. In their review, a broad array of features that may influence the effectiveness of interventions to prevent depression were listed. Given their relevance for our purpose, most of these moderators were retained and few new moderators were added. All moderators, their descriptions, and coding are listed in Table 1. They can be classified in three categories: 1) participant features: gender (percentage of females), ethnicity (percentage of Whites), age (in years); 2) intervention features: relaxation, intervention duration (in hours), whether the intervention makes use of homework, group size ( N in each group), whether there is room for interaction between teacher and students and among students; and 3) design features: randomization (whether participants were randomly assigned to intervention and control conditions) and follow-up duration.

1.1

Gender

It is hypothesised that interventions including a high number of women will produce larger effects. Women typically report more stress than men (Matud,

2004). It seems plausible that their high levels of initial stress and the stronger need for stress relief would make it easier to find improvements in stress responses, not only because of the effect of regression on the mean but also in terms of actual improvements.

36

Chapter 2

Table 1 Operationalization and descriptive statistics for moderators

1.2

Ethnicity

There is a clear connection between ethnicity and (work) stress, independent of work characteristics and sociodemographic, socioeconomic, and occupational factors (Smith et al., 2005). As with gender, it is hypothesised that groups with higher number of non-Whites will produce larger effects, because of the higher initial level of stress.

1.3

Age

The targeted interventions cover a large age span. It is well known that there is a steady increase in cognitive abilities from adolescence into adulthood.

37

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

Studies have furthermore shown that older adults are also more effective in solving everyday problems (Blanchard-Fields, Mienaltowski, & Seay, 2007).

Because knowledge and skill transference require well-developed cognitive abilities, a linear relationship between age and intervention is expected, right up until early old age.

1.4

Relaxation

Various psychoeducational interventions include a relaxation component. Very early on, the relevance of relaxation for stress reduction was already illustrated by Carrington et al. (1980). Further research consolidated these finding, for example, Esch, Fricchione, and Stefano (2003). Based on the evidence in the literature, we hypothesize that interventions including this component will be more effective.

1.5

Intervention Duration

The more time spent working on and learning about stress and stress-related problems, the more knowledge transfer and skill development is expected to ensue. We therefore expect a linear relationship between duration and effectiveness.

1.6

Homework

We hypothesize that homework assignment will add beneficial effects, especially for longer lasting interventions. This may enhance consolidation of acquired knowledge, induce skill training, and bridge the gap between the learning context and real life.

38

Chapter 2

1.7

Group Size

We hypothesize that students in smaller groups will be less distracted, more involved, and have more possibilities to ask questions and receive additional, personally relevant information. Therefore, interventions that make use of small group sizes are expected to generate larger effects compared with interventions with large groups, similar to effects found in classroom situations

(Ehrenberg, Brewer, Gamoran, & Willms, 2001).

1.8

Interactive

In some types of interventions there is room for interaction during the sessions among group members. This aspect may work both ways: either it may enhance social support mechanisms, create modeling effects, and so on, or it may create an environment in which the participant feels pressure to open up to fellow participants and/or the teacher. The latter may create tension that subsequently interferes with the learning process. In general, it nevertheless appears that interaction is beneficial during the learning process (King, 1990).

Therefore, we hypothesize that interventions in which interaction is present will produce larger effects than interventions in which interaction is absent.

1.9

Randomization

We hypothesize that studies in which participants were randomly assigned to the intervention and control conditions will produce smaller effect sizes. The adequate and equal distributions of participants to the different conditions provide perfect control for evolutions in the intervention group. This is a superior alternative to research designs with nonrandomised controls and

39

Stress reduction through psychoeducation minimizes allocation bias and possible confounding factors, both known and unknown (Moher et al., 2010).

1.10

Follow-Up Duration

Similar programs typically produce the strongest effect sizes at posttest, followed by a gradual decrease at each follow-up assessment (Stice, Shaw, &

Marti, 2007). We therefore hypothesize that the later on the follow-up is conducted, the smaller the reported effect sizes will be.

In sum, the goal of this review is to provide an overview of the short- and longterm effectiveness of psychoeducation for stress and their possible moderators.

2

Method

2.1

Search Strategy

A comprehensive search on the literature was set up. First, major database search engines were used, including MEDLINE, Web of Knowledge, Wiley

Interscience Journals, PubMed, Cochrane Library, Ovid, and Embase, to search with predefined keywords. A detailed table with the keywords can be found in the appendix. Second, relevant journals were searched by hand. These included the International Journal of Stress Management , Work and Stress , and Anxiety,

Stress and Coping . Additionally, references of the studies included were searched by hand, together with available reviews. If necessary elements for data analysis were missing, authors were contacted for additional information.

40

Chapter 2

2.2

Inclusion Criteria

To identify relevant studies on the effectiveness of psychoeducation, that is, having a focus on transmitting information on stress in a teaching format, seven search criteria were determined. To be eligible a study had to be published in the past 20 years (January 1990 to January 2010), had to be published in an international (English language) journal, and needed to have a preventive aim with a main focus on stress. Furthermore, each study had to include a valid outcome measure of stress. Finally, it had to use methodology that included quantitative longitudinal measurement and a quasi-experimental or experimental design with a control condition. No participant age–related exclusion criterion was used for any of the interventions.

2.3

Statistical Methods

2.3.1

Overall effect size estimation

As a primary outcome measure, the scores on different scales all measuring

(perceived) stress were used and were evaluated in a similar way to earlier work by Martin, Sanderson, Cocker, and Hons (2009). Treatment effect sizes were calculated using Hedge’s g, later on referred to as standardized mean difference ( SMD ). This is the difference between posttreatment means, divided by the pooled standard deviation, with adjustment for small sample bias. Each study was coded so that a positive SMD indicated a superiority of the intervention Group over the comparison group. Overall effect size was calculated using the RevMan program (The Cochrane Center, The Cochrane

Collaboration, Copenhagen, Denmark). A random effects model was preferred to a fixed effects model for the meta-analysis. Because not all the interventions and outcome measures were exactly the same, this was the most suitable method for evaluating the overall effect size (Higgins & Green, 2008).

41

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

2.3.2

Moderator analysis

Moderators were analyzed for the following: 1) participant features: gender

(percentage of females), ethnicity (percentage of whites), age (in years); 2) intervention features: intervention content (knowledge transition, skill transition, relaxation), intervention duration (in hours), whether the intervention makes use of homework, group size ( N in each group), whether there are booster sessions, whether there is room for interaction between

Table 2 Summary of studies included in the review

42

Chapter 2 teacher and students; and 3) design features: follow-up duration. All data concerning the moderators were entered into SAS software (version 9.1, SAS

Institute, Cary, NC). Because moderators are possibly confounded, analyses were not only undertaken for each moderator separately but also for the group of moderators as a whole using a sample size weighted regression model. If the effect size was not reported, it was generated from the available data using

ClinTools (version 4.1; Devilly, 2005).

3

Results

3.1

Search Results

The search strategy generated 221 studies that met the inclusion criteria. The inclusion and exclusion processes are summarized in Figure 2. Due to the large scope of the search strategy, many of the initially retrieved studies were not retained. Sixty-one articles appeared relevant after an initial screening, of

Figure 2.

Flow chart of the search strategy

43

Stress reduction through psychoeducation which 44 were excluded after closer inspection for not meeting one or more of the predefined inclusion criteria. Finally, 17 articles – accounting for 19 studies – were accepted. Sixteen were used in the effect size estimation, whereas all 19 studies could be included for the moderator analysis, which required less stringent preconditions. Table 2 presents a brief summary of all the studies included with a description of the sample and the intervention, the intervention group size, the relevant outcome measure, and general findings.

3.2

Effect Size Estimation

The SMD s at posttest for each of the 16 studies included are presented in

Figure 2. These varied from a small negative effect of −.03 to a large effect of

.89. An overall positive effect was found. The inverse variance weighted SMD was small, but significant with an SMD of .27 (95% confidence interval [CI] =

[.14, .40], p < .0001). Alternatively, effect sizes were also weighted using their sample size ( N ). This produced similar results, with an SMD of .21 (95% CI =

[.12, .30]). Statistical heterogeneity is assessed using I 2, a common method for measuring the magnitude of between-study heterogeneity. Higher heterogeneity makes it more difficult to interpret results. Generally, percentages of around 25%, 50%, and 75% are considered, respectively, as low, medium, and high heterogeneity (Huedo-Medina, Sanchez-Meca, Marin-

Martinez, & Botella, 2006). In this case, with 35%, medium statistical heterogeneity is present. The study by Kirby, Williams, Hocking, Lane, and

Williams (2006) compared multiple interventions with the same control group.

Because this could introduce bias in the results, a sensitivity analysis was undertaken. In this analysis, only the intervention with the most comprehensive treatment group was included. This produced a similar overall result with an SMD of .27 (95% CI = [.13, - .41]), indicating that including all three Kirby et al. (2006) studies does not bias the results.

44

Chapter 2

45

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

Another form of bias is publication bias. To take this into account, a weighted

‘fail-safe N ’ statistic was calculated using Fail-Safe Number Calculator, a software program based on the methods described in Rosenberg (2005).

Rosenberg’s fail-safe number using a random effects model was 22.8, indicating at least 23 unpublished studies finding no effect would be needed to produce an overall noneffect. Not all the aforementioned studies included a follow-up measurement. The overall effect size estimation for relevant studies can be found in Figure 3. Effect sizes varied from −.10 to .78. Contrary to the results at posttest, the effect was not overall positive. Only a small effect was found, and the results had a high amount of statistical heterogeneity ( I ² = 73%), which makes them difficult to interpret. In general, the conclusion is that there is mixed evidence when it comes to the long-term effects of psychoeducational interventions for stress.

3.3

Moderator Analysis

When the results for the effect size estimation are taken into consideration, there is little difference between weighing according to inverse variance and weighing according to sample size. Therefore, we opted to weigh according to sample size for the moderator analysis, taking benefit of the fact that all 19 studies – of which the sample size was known, but not always the variance – could be included in the analysis. A schematic overview of the moderator values and effect sizes for each study can be found in Table 3. Results of the regression analysis are presented in Table 4.

46

Chapter 2

Table 4 Effects for moderators

3.3.1

At posttest

One moderator reached significance at posttest: ‘Intervention duration’.

Contrary to what could be expected, studies that evaluated interventions that were short in duration found significantly better effects. When a model with multiple moderators was considered, a model combining both intervention duration and the number of women in an intervention was significant and accounted for 42% of the variability found in the data set. Specifically, interventions with more women that were shorter in duration obtained better results. Other (combinations of) moderators did not produce significant effects.

3.3.2

At follow-up

A moderator that has a certain amount of variance and therefore still can be of particular interest is ‘Time of follow-up since course end’. Apparently there is a negative relationship between the follow-up effect found and the duration of the follow-up ( p < .0001). This could mean that psychoeducation does not stand the test of time and beneficial effects tend to fade out. Moderator effects found for follow-up results should be interpreted with caution though, due to the small number of studies and the limited variance across the studies.

For the latter reason, no conclusions can be drawn for the – significant –

47

Stress reduction through psychoeducation moderators ‘Participant Ethnicity’, ‘Participant Age’, and ‘Follow-up Duration’.

As with the effect size estimation, a sensitivity analysis was conducted for the

Kirby et al. (2006) studies. Again, all reported estimates were within the confidence intervals reported in Table 4. As such, it could be concluded that inclusion of the three Kirby et al. studies did not create bias.

Figure 3.

Overall effects of the reviewed interventions on stress at posttest

Figure 4.

Overall effect of the reviewed interventions on stress at follow-up

48

Chapter 2

4

Discussion

The first question was whether psychoeducational interventions are effective in reducing stress. The effect sizes reported in this review are small, but consistently positive, indicating effectiveness for this type of psychoeducation.

The overall effect ( SMD = .27) is larger than in similar meta-analyses, for example, the study by Martin et al. (2009) on the effects of health promotion interventions for depression and anxiety symptoms ( SMD = .05) or the study by

Stice et al. (2009) on depression prevention programs for children and adolescents ( r = .15). Learning about stress and extending techniques to cope with it seems to contribute positively to mental health.

Despite a large variety in intervention formats, it appears that psychoeducation is effective for people of varying ages, from different backgrounds, and with different interests to follow a psychoeducational course.

Some remarks do have to be made, though. First, the results at follow-up are relatively weak ( SMD = .20) and – on average after 6 months – the confidence interval of the overall SMD even reaches a negative effect size. This is contrary to the idea that psychoeducational interventions provide people with skills to continuously improve their mental health. On the other hand, because only half of the reported studies record follow-up data, the evidence base for this conclusion is in itself much weaker.

The second question was whether there were characteristics of a psychoeducational intervention that would make it less or more effective. Only intervention duration appeared as a significant moderator. A model including intervention duration and participant gender explained 42% of the variance in effects. Apparently, short lasting psychoeducational interventions for women are most effective. These results are correlational. Therefore, we refrain from making firm causal interpretations and advancing specific suggestions for interventions. Several findings require further research: 1) Women appear to benefit more than men from this type of intervention. This suggests that these

49

Stress reduction through psychoeducation interventions should therefore primarily target women and that seeking an alternative approach for men would be premature. This should be further investigated, preferably in an RCT, dividing men and women at random over an intervention group and a waiting list control/placebo/alternative approach group. 2) Shorter interventions obtain better effects, which is contradictory to what was hypothesised originally. Future research could focus on two alternative hypotheses: that a shorter intervention is more effective in transferring a set of knowledge and skills than a longer lasting intervention and that people who opt to participate in shorter interventions generally benefit more from this type of intervention because of specific characteristics. With the worldwide expansion of primary care, preventive interventions for groups that are short lasting and easily accessible are quickly emerging. Although mostly focusing on depression, stress-related interventions are also on the rise.

Together with this rise, a clear need emerges for evaluating the effectiveness of these interventions. The goal of this review was to provide some insight in the nature of these interventions and their target groups, as well as to map what is currently subject to research. Last, but not least, some additional, more general recommendations for future research are provided.

5

Limitations and directions for future research

The major limitation is that this article made use of published articles only. This may have made the review prone to bias, as interventions finding no effect probably are not easily reported. Still, some nuance can be made. Although sometimes controversial, the failsafe N statistics does provide a certain ground to account for publication bias. The reported results are considered relatively solid, given the large number of studies reporting no effect needed, to generate an overall non effect.

Another limitation is the design used in (some of) the reported studies.

50

Chapter 2

Follow-up measurements are paramount when trying to assess long-lasting

(behavioral) changes. Without them, there is no way to know whether interventions do add something substantial to the lives of participants or whether they only scratch the surface. As such, this review is also a plea to include at least one follow-up measurement in any design that intends to evaluate an intervention with the potential for realizing long-lasting change.

The initial setup required including only RCTs. Therefore, most of the control groups are waitlist controls or no treatment controls. Although sometimes an alternative program was set up as a pastime, the current evaluation cannot compare psychoeducation with other means of intervention and conclude psychoeducation is to be preferred. We can only state that it is more effective in reducing stress than undertaking no action at all.

Our recommendation concerning the information reported in articles is especially interesting in light of moderator analyses. It would be considered a big advantage for meta-analyses if these would move beyond reporting standard information such as average age of participants and their gender and also start including other characteristics that are not commonly reported, such as group sizes. We are still unaware of what the exact factors are that contribute to the effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions. Therefore, as many intervention characteristics as possible should be taken into account when setting up an intervention and these characteristics should subsequently be documented in publications. In the long run these data will have the potential to provide us with valuable information for adjusting and redirecting future interventions.

51

Stress reduction through psychoeducation

52

Chapter 3

Effectiveness of a six session stress reduction program for groups

Objectives. The goal of the present study was to determine the effectiveness of sixweek cognitive-behavioural stress reduction course for large groups (two hours per week). Methods.

Two groups (intervention group N =47; matched control group N =47) completed self-report questionnaires on stress, depression, anxiety, worrying, and stress management skills at pre- and post-intervention and at six months and one year follow-up. Results.

Linear declines for the intervention group were found for all symptoms from pre-intervention to post-intervention and further on through follow-up

(linear trends ps < .05), whereas stress management skills remained stable. Clinically significant and reliable change (in almost 30% of participants) confirmed these findings.

No such change was found for the control group. The strongest effect occurred for those participants who presented themselves with higher levels of initial symptoms.

Conclusions. Overall, the data showed small but reliable and long-lasting effects on selfreported stress, worrying and symptoms of depression and anxiety.

Keywords stress reduction, psychoeducation, cognitive-behavioural, intervention, matched control design

53

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program

1

Introduction

In 1984, Lazarus and Folkman (1984, p. 19) defined psychological stress as “... a particular relationship between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her well-being.“ Almost three decades later, chronic stress is considered a major burden in modern society, compromising both physical and mental health (American Psychological Association, 2010). High levels of selfperceived stress are, for example, closely related to several adverse health conditions like metabolic syndrome (Chandola, Brunner, & Marmot, 2006) and coronary heart disease (Jood, Redfors, Rosengren, Blomstrand, & Jern, 2009;

Rosengren et al., 2004; Xu, Zhao, Guo, Guo, & Gao, 2009). There is also a clear link between high levels of stress and the subsequent onset of mental disorders such as depression (van Praag, 2004; Wang, 2004).

Considering the scope of the burden of stress, no health service will ever be able to provide adequate treatment for all, even in more affluent countries

(van ‘t Veer-Tazelaar et al., 2009). This emphasizes the need for large scale prevention, for example by reducing stress in the general population. In MHC, prevention can be situated within a stepped-care approach. This represents an attempt to maximize the efficiency of resource allocation in therapy: low threshold and low cost interventions are offered first, and more intensive and costly interventions are reserved for those who are not sufficiently helped by the initial intervention (Haaga, 2000). A recent meta-analysis including a variety of programs confirmed that the average participant of a stress reduction program obtains a significant reduction of perceived stress. When long-term changes are considered, however, results are less clear. The limited number of studies that include follow-up for up to six months or less find mixed results

(Van Daele, Hermans, Van Audenhove, & Van den Bergh, 2012).

The current study therefore aims at consolidating the evidence base for

54

Chapter 3 stress reduction programs, both in the short and long term. In the present case, we are interested in how the intervention performs in the real-life context of communities, resembling common practice. This provides a more accurate view of intervention effectiveness in everyday life. The intervention itself is a stress reduction program, developed within the CBT tradition as an adaptation of a program by White (2000) that was originally developed to reduce anxiety. It is being offered to large groups of self-registering community dwellers. Since they self-register, participants may have various initial complaints and motivations constituting a heterogeneous group of participants with ‘typical’ elevated stress symptoms, but also participants with low levels of stress whose main interest is to learn more about stress and how it may affect them. Whereas

White’s course was more focused on curing participants with elevated complaint levels, the current course has therefore more characteristics of a selective preventive intervention.

The goal of the program is to reduce stress by altering the relationship between the person and the environment. More specifically, stress reduction is intended to occur through two main routes. One focuses on strengthening the participants’ resources through developing social and self-management skills.

The other attempts to change cognitive representations through targeting negative appraisals and unhelpful perseverative thinking, such as worrying and ruminating which may mediate the relationship between stressors and psychopathology (Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer, 2006). Because the program aims to initiate a learning process, the reduction of stress-related symptoms is expected to occur gradually and to continue in the months following the intervention.

Changes were assessed through self-report questionnaires. Stress scores were considered as the primary outcome measure, depression and anxiety as secondary outcome measures, and reduction in worrying and increase in stress management skills as the means for stress reduction. We used a pre-post

55

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program matched control design with two follow-up moments, one after six and one after 12 months. Because participants needed time to process all the information and practice the skills taught during the course, it was hypothesised that in the months following the intervention, a steady, gradual decline in worrying and a gradual increase in stress management skills would be accompanied by a decline in stress and depression and anxiety. The strongest effect is expected to occur for those participants who present themselves with higher levels of initial symptoms.

2

Method

2.1

Recruitment and screening

In order to participate, respondents had to reside in one of three regions in

Flanders (Belgium). In each region, local organizations were contacted to help distribute information leaflets through their own networks and communication channels, including GPs, (sports) clubs, libraries and local press. Exclusion criteria were defined and potential participants who met at least one of these were informed that the current intervention might not completely suit their needs and that additional professional help might be necessary. Subsequently, they could decide to continue following the course or not, but they were always advised to contact the local centre for ambulatory MHC. The centres were informed about these potential contacts and agreed to give these requests priority. If participants continued to follow the course, they were removed from the study sample. The exclusion criteria were the answers on 1) question 15 of the Web Screening Questionnaire (WSQ; Donker, van Straten,

Marks, Cuijpers, 2009) indicating suicidal tendencies (Answering ‘I would do it given the opportunity’ on the question whether the idea of harming yourself or taking your life, recently came into their mind), 2) the General Anxiety Disorder

56

Chapter 3

Questionnaire-7 (GAD-7; Spitzer Kroenke, Williams, & Lowe, 2003) showing they suffered from a severe generalized anxiety disorder (15+ on a 21 point scale), 3) three questions of the Alcohol Use Disorders Screening Test (AUDIT;

Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Puente, & Grant, 1993) pointing to problematic substance abuse (which could lead to alcohol induced violence, endangering fellow participants). During the course, participants could also be excluded if the teacher-therapist noticed signs of psychotic disorders or severe deviant behaviour.

To study long-term effects, the original goal was to randomly allocate participants to a stress management course or to a one-year non-intervention control group. This, however, raised practical and ethical concerns in local partners endangering course implementation: local partners were reluctant to advertise the study when half of the participants would be denied treatment for twelve months or would receive some kind of placebo treatment. A matching procedure was therefore used to collect control data instead of using randomized non- or pseudo-intervention controls. In the matching procedure, a large sample was recruited from the general population through local newspapers, answering an advertisement to participate in a questionnaire study concerning their general well-being. Subsequently, a selected number of them were matched one-on-one to the course participants according to predetermined criteria: stress scores, depression and anxiety, as well as age, socioeconomic status and gender. Participants in this control group were not aware of the intervention and had not expressed an explicit desire to participate in the stress course. This design proved to be acceptable for local partners. It was subsequently also approved by the ethics committee of the

Faculty of Psychology and Educational Studies of the KU Leuven. Controls received € 10 per data collection wave for participating.

57

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program

2.2

Intervention

The intervention was an adaptation of a program called ‘Stress Control’

(White, 2000; adaptation by ISW-Limits, 2006). Teachers were trained psychologists from local centres for ambulatory MHC. They led this course, which comprised six weekly lessons of two hours during which participants mostly listen and are not required to interact. In lesson 1, participants were offered general information on stress; it served as a general course introduction, followed by two homework assignments. For the first homework assignment participants needed to evaluate their own stress level, reaction patterns, and general well-being. This was followed by determining concrete goals they wanted to reach during the time of the course. A second homework assignment implied reading an overview of basic self-help tips and techniques that could help them with short–term stress reduction, including distraction, practicing sports, breathing exercises, and a proper diet. The goals of these homework assignments were to familiarize participants with the course, to help them apply the general information to their own personal situation, and to create a personalized frame of reference for the course content. This allowed course participants to select specific, relevant techniques presented in the following lessons. During lesson 2 they focused on the effects of stress on the body and controlling bodily sensations. After some theoretical background, participants learned the techniques of progressive relaxation and breathing exercises. Furthermore, the importance of active recreation was emphasized.

In lesson 3 cognitive techniques were demonstrated and participants focused on becoming aware of fallacies and challenging dysfunctional thoughts. In lesson 4 techniques were taught from problem-solving and participants learned how to confront their fears, end safety behaviours, and increase their assertiveness. In lesson 5 and the first part of lesson 6 the knowledge from

Lesson 1 and the techniques learned in the previous lessons were rehearsed.

58

Chapter 3

These were subsequently applied to problems of anxiety, panic, sleeping disorders and feelings of depression, and tension and burn-out. In the second part of lesson 6, guidance was provided on how to control future stress.

2.3

Measures

The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995;

Dutch version by de Beurs, Van Dyck, Marquenie, Lange, & Blonk, 2001) is a 21item self-report questionnaire measuring stress (7 items, α = .89) in the past week. Symptoms of depression (7 items, α = .94), and symptoms of anxiety (7 items, α = .91) are also considered as secondary outcome measures.

The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, &

Borkovec, 1990; Dutch version by van Rijsoort, Vervaeke, & Emmelkamp, 1997) is a 16-item self-report questionnaire used to measure worrying on a five point

Likert scale ranging from 1 ‘Not at all typical for me’ to 5 ‘Very typical of me’.

The questionnaire has a high internal consistency both for normal (α = .90) and clinical (α = .86) populations.

The Coping Strategies Indicator (CSI; Amirkhan, 1990, Dutch version by

Bijttebier & Vertommen, 1997) is a 33-item self-report questionnaire that measures three coping styles: problem-solving (11 items, α = .87), social support seeking (11 items, α = .90), and avoidance (11 items, α = .73). Because the course supports problem-solving and social support seeking and tries to diminish avoidance when faced with stress, this questionnaire is therefore suited to evaluate the change in stress management skills. The questionnaire uses a three point Likert scale (3 ‘a lot’, 2 ‘a little’, 1 ‘not at all’). The higher the scores, the more commonly a strategy is used. High scores are therefore considered positive for problem-solving and social support seeking, and negative for avoidance.

59

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program

2.4

Course implementation

The intervention was implemented using a framework called empowerment implementation. This strategy offers interventions room to be flexible to local needs, while still maintaining adherence to strict implementation guidelines.

More details on the actual implementation and the underlying framework can be found in Van Daele, Van Audenhove, Hermans, Van den Bergh, & Van den

Broucke, 2012.

2.5

Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using SPSS (SPSS 16.0, IBM). Group by time interaction was evaluated using a generalized linear model (GLM) and compared selfreported symptoms and skills at each measurement point of the design.

Furthermore, when group by time interactions were at least marginally significant ( p < .10), group by trend-over-time interactions were also conducted, testing for linear and quadratic trends in the time variable.

Reliable and clinically significant changes were used as secondary measures for course effectiveness. According to Jacobson and Truax (1991), the optimal way to determine clinically significant change is according to ‘criterion C’. This method assumes that both the normal and the clinical population are normally distributed. In order to achieve clinically significant change (CSC), symptoms severity of participants should be closer to the mean score of the normal population than to those of the clinical population. Therefore, a cut-off score is determined. Because such a cut-off score is arbitrary and small changes may also lead to a change from one side of the cut-off score to the other, reliable change (RC) is used as an additional criterion. RC indicates that the change reported is larger than expected by chance. Both types of change are independent of one another, but the change aimed for is one that is both

60

Chapter 3 reliable and clinically significant. For this analysis, DASS stress scores were used to determine treatment effects, because for this questionnaire Dutch normative data were available both for the normal ( M = 8.5, SD = 8.0) and clinical population ( M = 15.8, SD = 9.8; de Beurs, personal communication,

October 28, 2007).

3

Results

3.1

Participants

Questionnaires were administered pre-intervention, post-intervention, after six months, and one year after course completion. A total of 77 participants completed the first questionnaire. Two participants who had completed the first questionnaire dropped out after the first session, one due to lack of interest, the other due to unexpected surgery. Furthermore, based on the exclusion criteria, one participant with a suicide risk was informed that the intervention might not suit her needs and she was referred to a local centre for ambulatory MHC. This participant nevertheless decided to continue following the course, but was removed from the study sample. Overall, 75 participants from three different locations ( N = 28, N = 34 and N = 13) completed the course, of which 47 participants (63% of the completers) filled in the questionnaires at all four times. Their mean age was 44. 1 years ( SD = 10.1, range 21-63).

From a total of 158 controls that completed the questionnaire at preintervention, 139 (88%) completed the questionnaires at all four times. From these 139 controls, 47 were matched with the intervention participants within one standard deviation on depressive, anxiety and stress scores, as well as for age, socioeconomic status and gender. The mean age was 39.15 years ( SD =

13.4, range 20-69). Other socio-demographics for both groups can be found in

Table 5.

61

Table 5 Sociodemographics for intervention group (N = 47) and matched control group (N = 47) in percent

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program

3.2

Missing data

By the end of the study 28 (37%) of the 75 completers had not returned one or more questionnaires and were therefore left out of the final analyses. These non-responders were compared to responders on pre-intervention scores for the DASS, PSWQ, and CSI. Only for stress management skills, a significant difference was found with non-responders having higher problem-solving scores, CSI-problem-solving: F (1,73) = 4.20, p = .04. Other measures did not show a difference between both groups. Overall, these analyses showed little difference between both groups, which makes the results outlined below relevant for the group of participants as a whole.

3.3

Course effectiveness

An overview of the data for all measurements can be found in Table 6. The analyses apply to all four repeated measures, which were done over the course of one year.

62

Chapter 3

Tabel 6 Evolution of intervention group (N = 47) and matched control group

(N = 47)

3.3.1

Stress, depression and anxiety

When the group by time interaction was evaluated using GLM, no significant effect on stress scores was found F (3, 276) = 2.07, p = .10. For the secondary outcome measures, a significant effect was found for depressive symptoms,

F (3,276) = 2.72, p = .05, but not for anxiety, F (3,276) < 1. Additionally, the group by trend-over-time interactions did show a clear trend for stress scores,

F (1,92) = 5.26, p = .02, with a linear decline in the intervention group F (1,46) =

7.60, p =.01, which was absent in the control group F (1,46) < 1. This was also

63

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program the case for depressive symptoms, F (1,92) = 5.39, p = .02, with a linear decline in the intervention group F (1,46) = 7.08, p = .01, whereas the control group remained stable F (1,46) < 1. No such changes were found for anxiety, F (1,92) <

1.

Furthermore, the intervention group could be split into two: one group with higher and one group with lower initial symptoms. For each measure, the split value is determined as the value of the intersection point between the distributions of the normal and clinical population (Figure 5). When low ( N =

12) and high ( N = 35) stressed participants were compared using GLM, there was a strong group by time interaction, F (3, 135) = 4.24, p = .007. Similar results were found for low ( N = 22) and high ( N = 25) depressed participants, F (3,135)

= 4.70, p = .004, and for low ( N = 25) and high ( N = 22) anxious individuals, F (3,

135) = 6.05, p = .001. Additional analyses on the group by trend-over-time interaction showed clear linear trends for stress F (1, 45) = 9.61, p = .003, depression, F (1. 45) = 11.39, p = .002, and anxiety, F (1, 45) = 6.76, p = .013, with participants with high levels of initial symptoms showing a decline, whereas patients with low initial levels remained stable or showed a limited increase.

3.3.2

Worrying

An analysis of the group by time interaction with GLM showed a strong effect on worrying, F (3,276) = 5.60, p < .001. This was confirmed with an analysis on the group by trend-over-time interaction, F (1,92) = 13.20, p < .001, which showed a clear decline for the intervention group F (1,46) = 21.12, p < .001, whereas the control group remained stable F (1,46) < 1.

64

Chapter 3

Figure 5. Comparison of DASS-scores between participants with a low- and high-level baseline of complaints.

3.3.3

Stress management skills

When the group by time interaction was evaluated using GLM, a significant effect was found for problem-solving, F (3,264) = 2.95, p = .03, in which scores for the intervention group increased from pre to post followed by a limited decline at follow-up whereas those of the control group remained relatively stable. No such differences were found for social support, F (3,264) < 1, or avoidance, F (3,264) = 2.26, p = .08. Analyses on the group by trend-over-time interaction for stress management however, were not significant for problem solving, F (1,88) < 1, social support seeking F (1,88) = 1.23, p = .27, nor for avoidance, F (1,88) = 3.06, p = .08.

65

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program

3.4

Clinical significance

Clinical significance was evaluated using reliable and clinically significant change. DASS-stress-scores were used to determine treatment effects. The cutoff score for clinically significant change was determined at 11.7, which indicated that anyone above this score was considered within the range of the clinical population and anyone below this score was within the range of the normal population. Furthermore, in order for a change to be considered reliable – not attributable to chance – it had to be larger than 6.3. Initially, 85% of all participants reported stress symptoms within the range of the clinical population. At post intervention, 13% of these participants showed both a reliable and clinically significant change. After six months this number had increased to 23% and by the one year follow-up, 28% of the participants had undergone a reliable and clinically significant change. Because clinically significant change implied that participants had to be within the range of the clinical population, it is not surprising that 92% of the participants that underwent a clinically significant and reliable change were part of the group with higher initial symptoms mentioned earlier.

4

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate whether a six-week CBT stress reduction course for groups was effective in the long term in reducing stress and depression and anxiety, in decreasing worrying, and in increasing stress management skills. The results of the trend analyses indicated a linear decline of stress and depression in the intervention group from pre-intervention to post-intervention and further on through follow-up. These effects were not observed in the nonintervention group. There was also a strong linear decline in worrying, but only little change in stress management skills. Furthermore, measures of clinical

66

Chapter 3 significance showed that almost 30 percent of all course participants experienced a clinically significant and reliable change in the year following the course. Finally, the strongest effect occurred for those participants who presented themselves with higher levels of initial symptoms.

The overall modest mean effects seem to be due to the high amount of variation on all measures between participants. Evidence for this explanation can be found in the comparison of stress, anxiety and depression between participants with low and high initial symptoms, in which participants with high initial levels show a much stronger continuous and gradual decline of symptoms, whereas participants with low initial symptoms remained stable.

This is in line with other stress management programs that intend to realize long-term changes and which find effects up to four years after the intervention (Rowe, 2000, 2006). The course therefore appears to initiate a long-term process characterized by gradual declines in self-reported symptoms.

However, these declines were not strong enough to find significant effects for all symptoms at all times using a GLM.

There was nevertheless a strong decline in worrying, one of the two mechanisms targeted by the intervention. The other mechanism focused on the improvement in stress management techniques of participants, which showed little difference. The absence of an effect could be because the CSI might not be that well suited as a questionnaire to measure general changes in stress management skills. Since participants were asked to report how they managed a specific problem they encountered in the month preceding the time of completing the questionnaire, it might be that stress management in highly specific situations was reported, as opposed to the more general stress management skills that were intended. An alternative explanation is that the primary focus of the intervention was on psychoeducation and less on the actual skill training: participants were expected to practice at home in real life situations. Since this was not a controlled environment, feedback could not be

67

Effectiveness of a stress reduction program delivered immediately and teachers had little control over whether course participants actively used the acquired stress management techniques in their home situations. This would imply that it might be the reduction in worrying that is primarily responsible for the decline in symptoms. Because the current design does not allow making causal inferences, a focus for future research would therefore be to determine the mechanisms through which the symptom reduction is accomplished and potentially improve the skill training component in order to make it (more) effective.

Finally, the present study did not make use of random allocation of participants, because looking into the long-term effectiveness of the intervention would have required withholding (wait list) control participants from following the course for over a year, which was not acceptable for the organizations promoting the course locally. In addition, random allocation to an intervention or control group is difficult for any study planning to do a longterm follow-up. A matched control design therefore turned out to be the most practical and ethical solution. While in an RCT initial conditions in both groups are equalized, including the motivation to participate in a course, the latter aspect was not present in our control group. Neither did they receive some kind of bogus or placebo treatment. However, we used a matched control design in which we tried to control for a wide range of variables related to participant’s initial level of symptoms, as well as their socio-demographics.

Balancing both the need for long term follow-up and randomized allocation of participants to non-intervention conditions may prove to be a difficult task.

Overall, the main conclusion of this study is that there are substantial indications that the intervention has long-term effects on participants’ stress, anxiety and depression, especially for those who have higher initial symptoms and as far as up to one year after the intervention. It furthermore also shows

68

Chapter 3 that the effectiveness of ‘interventions in the field’ that focus on selfregistering community dwellers should be interpreted carefully. The heterogeneous groups they attract might not only include people who participate in order to obtain an immediate reduction of high symptoms, but also those who have low level symptoms and follow the course in order to prevent future symptoms. Especially the latter group could be responsible for an underestimation of intervention effectiveness if they are not specifically taken into consideration, as this might cause floor effects.

69

70

Research topic 2

Implementation

Van Daele, T., Van Audenhove, C., Hermans, D., Van den Bergh, O., & Van den Broucke,

S. (2012). Empowerment implementation: Enhancing fidelity and adaptation in a psychoeducational intervention. Health Promotion International.

Advance online publication. doi: 10.1093/heapro/das070

71

72

Chapter 4

Empowerment implementation: enhancing fidelity and adaptation in a psychoeducational intervention

Implementation is an emerging research topic in the fieldof health promotion. Most of the implementation research adheres to one of two paradigms: implementing interventions with maximum fidelity or designing interventions that are responsive to the needs of a local community. While fidelity and adaptation are often considered as contradictory, they are both essential elements of preventive interventions. An innovative program design strategy is therefore to develop hybrid programs that ‘build in’ adaptation to enhance program fit, while also maximizing the implementation fidelity.

The present article presents guidelines for this hybrid approach to program implementation and illustrates them with a concrete psychoeducational group intervention. The approach, which is referred to as ‘empowerment implementation’ on the analogy of empowerment evaluation, builds on theory of implementation fidelity and community-based participatory research. To demonstrate the use of these guidelines, a psychoeducational course aimed at stress reduction and the prevention of depression and anxiety was implemented according to these guidelines. The main focus lies on how an intervention can benefit from adaptations guided by local expertise, while maintaining the core program components and still respecting the implementation fidelity.

Keywords implementation science, effectiveness, empowerment, mental health promotion

73

Empowerment implementation

1

Introduction

Implementation is an emerging research topic in the field of public health. This growing interest reflects a changing view on what counts as evidence. For a long time, ‘evidence’ was a synonym for empirical data supporting either the scale or cause of a health problem, or the causal relations between interventions and outcomes. For both kinds, the level of evidence provided is a key quality feature, with an RCT traditionally regarded as the ‘golden’ – but often unachievable – standard for evaluation. However, over the last decade it has become clear that public health needs other types of research evidence as well (Aro et al., 2005; Rychetnik et al., 2002). It is not sufficient to know the magnitude, severity and causes of public health problems and the relative effectiveness of specific interventions to inform public health policy and practice. It is also necessary to know how a specific intervention should be implemented and under which circumstances it can be successful. Accordingly,

Rychetnik et al. (2004) distinguish between three types of evidence in public health: evidence pointing to the fact that ‘something should be done’, to determine ‘what should be done’ and informing on ‘how it should be done’.

The attention for the latter kind of evidence has given rise to a research investigating the quality and the processes of implementation (Breitenstein et al., 2010; Glasgow, Lichtenstein & Marcus, 2003; Palinkas et al., 2011; Rabin et al., 2010). Although there is no consensus with regard to the conceptual and methodological frameworks to be used to study implementation, various strategies have been proposed to enhance the quality of implementation.

These strategies often draw upon the literature on the diffusion of innovation from the 1970s and 1980s (Dusenbury, Brannigan, Falco, &, Hansen, 2003). The most influential strategy is to maximize the fidelity of intervention delivery

(Dumas, Lynch, Laughlin, Smith, & Prinz, 2001). The concept of implementation fidelity refers to ‘the degree to which an intervention or program is delivered as intended’ (Carroll et al., 2007). Specifically, a successful implementation is one

74

Chapter 4 that abides with four components of fidelity: adherence, exposure, quality of program delivery and participant responsiveness (Dane and Schneider, 1998).

Mihalic (2002) describes each of these components as follows: 1) ‘adherence’ refers to whether interventions are delivered as intended; 2) ‘exposure’ refers to the number of sessions implemented, session length, frequency of implementation of program techniques; 3) ‘quality of program delivery’ refers to the manner in which staff deliver a program; and 4) ‘participant responsiveness’ refers to the extent to which participants are involved in program content. Hasson (2010) has suggested two additional factors that moderate implementation fidelity, notably ‘recruitment’ and ‘context’. The concept of 5) ‘recruitment’ refers to procedures that are used to attract potential program participants, whereas 6) ‘context’ refers to surrounding social systems, such as structures and cultures of groups, inter-organizational linkages and historical as well as concurrent events. All these factors should be evaluated when conducting a process evaluation.

Multiple methodologies have already been developed to measure the implementation fidelity (Blakely et al., 1987). Their main goal is to identify the factors that lead to (the lack of) intervention success. In addition, they also focus on the mechanism and processes that must be taken into consideration when implementing complex interventions (Breitenstein et al., 2010; Campbell et al., 2000; Oakley et al., 2004; Toroyan et al., 2004). Several attempts have also been made to increase the implementation fidelity (Basen-Engquist et al.,

1994; Burns, Peters, & Noell, 2008; Macaulay, Gronewold, Griffin, Williams, &

Samuolis, 2005; Vitale & Romance, 2005).

On the other hand, focusing on fidelity has also been criticized for being rigid, as it assumes full compliance with the program as prescribed by the program developer (Gresham et al., 1993). The fact that any change to the program made by implementers is considered as bias and as a threat to implementation quality is at odds with the value placed on stakeholder

75

Empowerment implementation involvement and participation in health promotion (WHO, 1986; Levy, Baldyga,

& Jurkowski, 2003). An alternative approach to program implementation is therefore to encourage adaptation, rather than limit it. Drawing on the principles of community-based health promotion—which emphasizes the participation of the community in program planning, implementation, evaluation and dissemination—the program adoption approach holds that users or local adopters should be allowed to reinvent or change programs to meet their own needs and derive a sense of ownership.

The tension between the fidelity and adaptation approach is a recurrent theme in the implementation literature (Blakely et al., 1987; Dusenbury et al.,

2003). Berman (1981) proposed a contingency model of implementation, in which choosing between the strategies of fidelity and adaptation should depend on the nature of the intervention. The fidelity approach would be most suited for highly structured interventions, whereas the adaptation approach would work better in less structured interventions. Yet, the dominant view remains that both approaches have competing objectives: implementing interventions with maximum fidelity, versus designing interventions that are responsive to the cultural needs of a local community. However, these two objectives are not necessarily contradictory (Castro et al., 2004; Weissberg,

1990). In fact, fidelity and adaptation are both essential elements of preventive interventions. Moreover, both of them are best addressed by a planned, organized and structured approach (Shen, Yang, Cao, & Warfield, 2008).

An interesting attempt to unite both approaches has been proposed by

Backer (2001). Based on a literature review, this author formulated six recommendations for implementers. For a successful intervention implementation one should 1) identify and understand the theory behind the program; 2) locate or conduct a core components analysis of the program; 3) assess fidelity/adaptation concerns for the implementation site; 4) consult with the program developer; 5) consult with the organization and/or community in

76

Chapter 4 which the implementation will take place; and 6) develop an overall implementation plan based on these inputs. Adding to this discourse, Castro et al. (2004) suggest that an innovative program design strategy would be to develop hybrid programs that ‘build in’ adaptation to enhance program fit, while also maximizing the fidelity of implementation and program effectiveness. Unfortunately, however, the authors do not provide any guidelines as to how exactly such programs should be developed and validated, only stating that it would require 'rigorous science-based evaluation and testing’. The basis for such guidelines may, however, be found in similar, related theoretical frameworks like empowerment evaluation.

Empowerment evaluation is grounded in empowerment theory

(Zimmerman, 1995). Empowerment can be defined as “... an intentional, ongoing process through which people lacking an equal share of valued resources gain greater access to and control over those resources” . It can exist at community, organizational and individual level and can be viewed both as a process and as an outcome, reflecting the achieved level of empowerment.

This process offers individuals the opportunity to gain control over their lives and over democratic participation in the life of their community (Berger &

Neuhaus, 1977, cited in Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Zimmerman’s view of an empowerment approach to intervention design, implementation and evaluation redefines the professional’s role as that of a collaborator and facilitator, rather than an expert and counselor (Zimmerman, 2000). Fetterman

(1996) applied these principles to the evaluation of interventions, referring to this as the empowerment evaluation. It is defined as “... an evaluation approach that aims to increase the probability of achieving program success by providing program stakeholders with tools for assessing the planning, implementation and selfevaluation of their program, and mainstreaming evaluation as a part of the planning and management of the program”

(Wandersman et al., 2005, p. 28). In this sense, empowerment evaluation is

77

Empowerment implementation closely linked to capacity building. According to Fetterman, building the capacities of others to evaluate their own programs involves several steps: 1) determining where the program stands, including strengths and weaknesses; 2) focusing on establishing goals with an explicit emphasis on program improvement; 3) helping participants determine their own strategies to accomplish program goals and objectives; and 4) helping program participants determine the type of evidence required to document progress credibly toward their goals.

The present article aims to extend these guidelines to program implementation. In the next section, this theoretical framework will be introduced, followed by the presentation of the guidelines. Finally, a psychoeducational course aimed at reducing stress and at preventing depression and anxiety will serve as an example of how these guidelines can be put into practice.

2

Empowerment implementation

While empowerment evaluation is mainly concerned with the evaluation of a program, the same principles can also be applied to implementation. Such an

‘empowerment implementation’ could well reconcile the opposing fidelity and adaptation approaches to implementation. Indeed, involving the community in program implementation as an equal partner does not have to be at the cost of fidelity. It only requires providing the community with the concepts, tools and skills to identify the core components of the intervention, to adapt the intervention to their context and culture, and to assess, monitor and maintain the implementation quality.

The steps of an empowerment implementation approach would be as follows: 1) developing a core component; 2) selecting partners; 3) assessing the fidelity/adaptation concerns with partners; and 4) developing an overall implementation plan. The way in which these steps are executed is inspired by

78

Chapter 4 community-based participatory research (CBPR), a collaborative approach to research, involving all partners equitably in the research process, recognizing the unique strengths that each brings (Minkler & Wallerstein, 2003, p. 4;

Minkler, Vásquez, Warner, Stuessey, & Facente , 2006) and empowerment evaluation. The content of each step is defined by the key elements of high fidelity implementation, and by research concerning the balancing of program fidelity and adaptation. The consecutive steps will now be explained in detail.

2.1

Developing a core component

Prior to implementation, an intervention program is developed based on a sound theoretical framework. One example is Intervention Mapping (IM), a tool for the planning and development of health promotion interventions. It maps the path from the recognition of a need or problem to the identification of a solution ( Kok, Schaalma, Ruiter, & Van Empelen, 2004). The first four steps of

IM could be run through as follows: 1) needs assessment; 2) preparing matrices of change objectives; 3) selecting theory-informed intervention methods and practical strategies; and 4) producing program components and materials. The resulting intervention is tested in a controlled setting and empirically adapted until researchers end up with an effective intervention. Subsequently, researchers empirically) define which aspects of the intervention are especially important for its efficacy and label these as the core components of the intervention.

2.2

Selecting partners who will implement the intervention

Implementing an intervention requires the participation of partners in the field in order to guarantee its success. Given that the main goal is to adapt a previously developed intervention to the unique conditions of the real-life context in which it is implemented, the notion of participation in this context

79

Empowerment implementation does not refer to the involvement of the end users, but to that of the implementers. In terms of Fetterman’s (1996) framework, implementers can indeed be considered as primary stakeholders, whose role is not that of an expert or professional, but of a facilitator and enabler (Laverack & Wallerstein,

2001). As such, the focus of the participation of this study is more placed on the participation of the active ‘can affect’-stakeholders than of the ‘affected’ parties (Freeman, 1984, cited in Achterkamp & Vos, 2008). These partners preferably dispose of existing networks related to the intervention topic. For example, an interesting partner for interventions related to cancer in the USA would be the Cancer Information Service, a network of health education offices

( Glasgow, Marcus, Bull, and Wilson, 2004 ). However, if potential partners show limited interest in the program, their participation is of little use. Even if they can be persuaded to implement the program, they will be unlikely to strive for quality, and this will probably lead to poor results, i.e. inadequate implementation, weak fidelity and limited evidence-based actions ... In order to avoid this, it is important to carefully select the partners who will implement the intervention. A first step is therefore to make an overview of local partners who are available. Subsequently, these potential partners must be consulted to ascertain if they are interested to participate and whether they subscribe to the scientific basis of the intervention to be implemented. The most suitable partners for the intervention can then be selected through an evaluation of their strengths and weaknesses in the function of the intervention.

2.3

Assessing the fidelity/adaptation concerns with partners

The next step is to assess the concerns with regard to fidelity and adaptation together with the partners. This implies two aspects.

80

Chapter 4

2.3.1

Deciding on practical intervention aspects

To implement an intervention, a large number of practical aspects need to be taken into consideration. Although not all of these are crucial for the intervention to be effective, they play a large role in how the intervention can be perceived and received by the target population. Aspects that are not included in the core components of the program as defined in step 1 should therefore certainly be open for discussion, as they can have a significant impact on intervention dissemination. In the discussion about these non-core components, local partners should take the lead, as they know the situation and the target population best. Researchers should acknowledge their expertise, but remain available as resource persons. Together, researchers and implementers can tailor the intervention to suit present needs. Examples of more ‘practical’ aspects that – depending on the particular program and situation – might be open to discussion are: the recruitment of participants, the intervention location and context, the number/length/frequency of sessions …

2.3.2

Deciding on content-related intervention aspects

It is very important that partner participation is not limited to practical aspects only and that partners are also offered the possibility to give advice on content, as the impetus for partners wanting to participate depends on the opportunities that are presented by a project ( van der Velde, Williamson, &

Ogilvie, 2009 ). In this regard, researchers and local partners are considered as equal, each with their unique expertise. Both often have a substantial theoretical background, experience and personal affinity for the intervention, although these may vary in amount and in specific content. For intervention implementation, researchers can mainly rely on their theoretical background, whereas local partners most of the time have a better understanding of the specific implementation setting. Letting local partners change content may be a

81

Empowerment implementation sensitive matter for researchers, but if this is done through an open and respectful dialogue, the intervention can benefit greatly from this collaborative action.

The starting point should be that local partners have the possibility to change everything of the intervention, as long as the core components remain untouched. Examples of more ‘content-related’ aspects are participant responsiveness, means of program delivery, cultural sensitivities or preference ... Only some examples of aspects that are open to change were highlighted above. This list is therefore not exhaustive: there may be additional aspects to be taken into account, depending on the target group or context of the intervention. These can easily be chosen in collaboration between researchers and partners.

2.4

Developing an overall implementation plan

To ensure a successful implementation of the intervention, it is necessary to develop an implementation plan. This plan should specify the role of all partners involved and provide a clear timeline with an overview of what actions need to be undertaken when. One way to assure such quality management and avoid unintended effects at the phase of intervention realization is for researchers to actively monitor intervention implementation by the partners in the field, document potential deviations and subsequently go through these together with them to avoid future mistakes at later stages of the implementation process. Given the commensurability between efficacy and effectiveness (Stricker, 2000), this framework increases the chances for an effective intervention when implemented in practice. Furthermore, it helps to avoid certain problems that are common to CBPR, such as the lack of skills in research methods of community members, differences in the appraisal of intervention criteria between the community and funding agencies, and

82

Chapter 4 shifting levels of community involvement throughout the research process

(Levy, Baldyga, & Jurkowski, 2003; Wallerstein & Duran, 2006).

In the following sections, empowerment implementation will be illustrated by describing the implementation process of a psychoeducational group intervention aimed at reducing stress and preventing depression and anxiety.

This will show how each step of empowerment implementation can be put into practice. First, the context is described, followed by the method and the intervention. These descriptions are followed by an overview of the implementation process. During this overview, attention is directed to specific points of interest to illustrate main aspects of the framework.

3

Example: implementation of a psychoeducational group

intervention

3.1

Context

The project on the implementation of a psychoeducational group intervention was commissioned by the Flemish government in 2007 to the Policy Research

Centre Welfare Health and Family (SWVG), a consortium of Flemish research and expertise centers on health and well-being whose task is to support the

Flemish minister in pursuing an effective evidence-based policy. Its aim was to determine whether a psychoeducational group intervention to reduce stress and prevent depression could support the organizations in primary MHC to reduce the growing burden of mental health problems. The primary MHC sector in Flanders consists of a large number of organizations, each with their own goals and ways of working. Because of this diversity, a concern of the

Flemish government was not only to evaluate the effectiveness of the intervention, but also whether such a course could be organized through ad hoc partnerships. The feasibility of such an implementation would offer perspectives for large-scale implementations in the future. Local organizations

83

Empowerment implementation would be at liberty to select partners they consider most appropriate in order to achieve their (mutual) goals.

Three Flemish cities and their communities were selected as intervention sites: one larger (Antwerp) and two smaller ones (Ypres, Genk). All organizations participated voluntarily and by means of their own funding.

Although there were some differences between regions, key partners were provinces, local organizations for health consultation (LOGO), local governments and local centres for ambulatory mental healthcare (CGG). The location for the course was either provided by the provinces or by the local governments. Teachers were psychologists from local centres for ambulatory mental healthcare. Although all partners were involved in course promotion, the local organizations for health consultation had the most suitable and extended network at their disposal to promote the intervention and took the lead by, e.g. distributing the majority of flyers.

3.2

Method

Questionnaires were administered to the participants before and after the intervention. These included socio-demographic variables, depression, anxiety and stress scores (DASS-42; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995; Dutch version by de

Beurs, Van Dyck, Marquenie, Lange, & Blonk, 2001 ) and course evaluation at the level of participant reactions ( Kirkpatrick, 1975, Dutch version by Baert,

De Witte, & Sterck, 2001 ). This course evaluation was also used as a semi structured interview conducted with the course teachers after course completion.

84

Chapter 4

3.3

Intervention

3.3.1

Background

The psychoeducational course used for the intervention is a Flemish adaptation of a Scottish program called ‘Stress Control’. It was first described by White

(White, 2000, p. 57) as “... a six-session didactic cognitive behavioural ‘evening class’. It aims to: [1] teach students about anxiety and associated problem sdepression, panic and insomnia; [2] teach self-assessment skills to allow individuals to learn how these problems affect them [3] teach a range of techniques designed to enable individuals to tailor their own treatment with minimal therapist contact” and has been developed within the CBT approach close to Beck (1981) and Meichenbaum (1985). The Flemish version is not an exact replica of the original course. The goal of the Flemish version is more to preserve mental health than to restore it. Therefore, it focuses primarily on the phenomenon of stress than on anxiety. Since 2003 this intervention is being offered to the Flemish population, primarily by one of the major public health insurance companies. After paying an entry fee, both their members and nonmembers have the ability to attend the course.

3.3.2

Core component

The core component of the intervention is the course material containing a relaxation CD and various booklets. Each of the six weekly lessons has its own separate booklet with all (and even more) of the information presented during the evening course. One additional booklet contains all homework assignments. Teachers received course material from SWVG and were instructed to distribute this to course participants in their original format, without any changes.

85

Empowerment implementation

3.4

Implementation

3.4.1

Developing a core component

In this particular case, the stress control course was developed within a CBT approach, tested in a setting with college students, and found efficacious in this controlled environment. For the subsequent larger intervention with multiple partners in different regions, the course material was defined as the core component of the intervention which had to guarantee a qualitative intervention.

3.4.2

Selecting partners who will implement the intervention

The psychoeducational course was implemented in three Flemish regions, selected from the regions in which the Policy Research Centre Welfare Health and Family is active (KU Leuven, UGent, VUG, & KHK, 2007). To assure an optimal reception of the intervention by all partners, concept mapping was used. This research method is based on Gray’s (1989) collaboration model. A selection of potential partners for each region was brought together in focus groups. Through prioritizing their goals and interests, the three regions that were most suited for the implementation of this intervention were determined.

The partners in these regions showed positive attitudes toward psychoeducation and prevention. For each region, researchers and partners together determined the strengths and weaknesses of all involved. After this evaluation, four main tasks were determined and distributed among each other: 1) organizing administration and data collection; 2) providing the teacher; 3) providing the location; and 4) promoting the intervention. One example of the strengths and weaknesses analysis is that in each region it was agreed upon by all involved that the centres for ambulatory mental healthcare were best placed to provide teachers for the course. Researchers, partners and the centres themselves did not consider it wise to also involve them in

86

Chapter 4 promoting the intervention, though. Because of the stigma that is often still associated with MHC (providers) in Flanders ( Reynders, Scheerder,

Molenberghs, & Van Audenhove, 2011 ), it was decided to look for alternative channels.

3.4.3

Assessing the fidelity/adaptation concerns with partners

In each of the three regions researchers and partners gathered to address both practical and content-related intervention aspects. The researchers already had suggestions for different aspects that were open for discussion, but some also emerged during the meetings. Partners were hereby considered as the experts, the researchers only took the role of facilitators to moderate discussions between them. The goal was hereby not to create homogeneous implementation conditions across regions, but rather to determine and adapt specific and relevant aspects for each region. Because the three regions each had their unique context, this resulted – as expected – in some level of heterogeneity across regions.

3.4.4

Deciding on practical intervention aspects

These aspects were 1) the time of course commencement. The course was set up as an evening course, but partners decided themselves which day and at which exact time they preferred the course to start, taking into account the habits and possibilities of the target population in their region. All regions opted for Tuesday, with the time of commencement at 6 p.m., 7 p.m. and 8 p.m. 2) The course location. Both researchers and partners quickly agreed that the location should be neutral, not providing a threshold for people with any specific background to participate. Governmental buildings (respectively, a cultural centre, a lecture hall and even a city hall) were chosen as the most suitable locations. 3) Which channels would be used to promote the course and

87

Empowerment implementation by which means. In each region, partners had specific ideas and opportunities to promote the course: distributing flyers in public locations like libraries and to their personnel, posting advertisements on their websites, publishing announcements in official city papers, contacting specific organizations which aim at the target population, and setting up meetings between the researchers and local stakeholders. 4) Which of their staff members would be most suited to teach the course. The researchers left this completely open to the partners.

They all chose psychologists with considerable professional experience, both on psychopathology and in teaching (psychoeducational) courses. Finally, 5) manner of distribution of the course material. The course material could be distributed to the participants in parts, one for each lesson, or given as a whole at the commencement of the course. For didactic purposes, two regions decided to distribute the course material in parts, whereas in one region the course material was distributed as a whole, mainly for practical concerns.

3.4.5

Deciding on content-related intervention aspects

To support teachers during the course, default PowerPoint presentations were made available by the researchers. However, if preferred; 1) teachers could change the presentations to suit their own needs and the particular condition.

Other content-related aspects also apply to teachers, since they could exert most influence on the intervention content; 2) they could decide whether they wanted to read the relaxation exercise aloud themselves or play it from the CD;

3) they had the possibility to introduce interaction in the course, provided they felt like the course participants needed this; and finally 4) they could also add their own additional examples during the course in order to make them more relevant for the group.

88

Chapter 4

3.4.6

Developing an overall implementation plan

Together with partners, researchers wrote down all arrangements, decisions and agreements in an overall implementation plan. The researchers took the final responsibility and assured in each region and for each lesson that everything was implemented as decided upon. If – for one reason or another – there were deviations from the original plan, these were carefully documented and subsequently communicated to the partners. When the course would be evaluated, these could then be taken into consideration.

3.4.7

Actual implementation

The course was subsequently implemented in the three regions, for two groups of 34 and one group of 18 participants. The total number of participants was not that high, but partners indicated that it was still larger than when they set up similar courses in the past.

The average age of participants was 43.0 years ( SD = 10.3) and the majority was currently employed (85%). Close to 80% of participants were women, which is a high number, but not uncommon for this type of intervention (Van

Daele, Hermans, Van Audenhove, & Van den Bergh, 2012). Participants’ depression, anxiety and stress measures were high, approaching the clinical threshold (normative data provided by E. de Beurs, personal communication,

28 October 2007).

In general, both course participants and course trainers were pleased with the outcome. In a written questionnaire, 70% of course participants agreed that the course was useful and a total of 91% of participants indicated they would recommend the course to a friend. The three trainers were interviewed and were also favourable to the intervention. Based on their experiences, they did have some remarks, both concerning practical aspects and content. All

89

Empowerment implementation these remarks were discussed during the interview, carefully written down and will certainly be taken into consideration for future implementations.

4

Discussion

In this paper, we have introduced a framework to program implementation which reconciles the competing paradigms of maximizing implementation fidelity versus adapting programs to the needs of the local stakeholders.

Starting from the premise that fidelity and adaptation are both essential elements of implementation ideally addressed in a planned, organized and structured way, we have proposed a four-step framework to implement prevention programs, balancing program fidelity with adaptation. The framework is based on CBPR and on empowerment evaluation, which we have extended to program implementation.

This ‘empowerment implementation’ was illustrated by applying the framework to the implementation of a psychoeducational Group intervention in Flanders. The example showed that empowerment implementation offers the possibility of implementing the core components of an intervention with high fidelity, while allowing for the adaptation of the intervention to local needs, thus enhancing ownership by local stakeholders. It was seen that local partners not only prefer this flexibility, but consider it as necessary for any intervention which they are offered or required to implement. Whereas previously the adaptations made by local stakeholders to existing ‘standard’ intervention programs were mostly considered as ‘flaws’ in the implementation process, empowerment implementation provides an opportunity to redefine these adaptations as useful additions with a high ecological validity and relevance, which do not interfere with the core elements of the intervention.

The aim of the psychoeducational course was to strengthen the resilience of participants to deal with daily stressors, and to empower them to take charge of their own mental health. Whether or not this aim was achieved was not

90

Chapter 4 addressed in this study. However, what this study did show is that the participatory approach to implementation that was followed for this program led to a better understanding of the intervention, its goals and its core elements by the local health workers who implemented it, and stimulated them to develop, adapt and implement future interventions. As such, the effects may extend beyond the stated outcomes of the program, despite the fact that it was essentially conceived as a top-down intervention. In that way, the approach can be considered as truly empowering.

The fact that empowerment implementation is characterized by a high level of collaboration, mutual respect and program flexibility does not mean that anything goes. It remains important for researchers and stakeholders to control the outcome to assure that the intervention is implemented according to plan, maybe even more compared with ‘traditional’ frameworks for implementation.

The main difference is that implementation fidelity is not determined by the strict implementation of the entire intervention, but of its core components.

Deviating from the original intervention is allowed, even required and stimulated, as long as the core components remain untouched.

Such an implementation is ideally followed by empowerment evaluation. In the current study this was not possible, because clear research objectives were already formulated by policy makers prior to the start of the project. However, even in that situation it remains important to involve partners in the evaluation process. Taking time to consider the strengths and weaknesses of the actual implementation in comparison with the ideal scenario can serve as leverage for improvement. It also creates a strong intrinsic motivation for change among the partners and may offer opportunities for further collaboration.

5

Conclusion

Empowerment implementation provides a new look at the concept of implementation fidelity and intervention effectiveness. In this framework an

91

Empowerment implementation intervention consists of two parts: a core component and less important intervention aspects. The core component is proved effective in clinical trials and remains untouched throughout the implementation process. Less important intervention aspects are decided upon through an intensive collaboration between researchers and local partners. The framework therefore consists of three main phases: partner selection, deciding upon practical aspects and deciding upon content-related aspects. As such, it addresses and overcomes the apparent contradiction between implementation fidelity and adaptation.

Current research concerning implementation fidelity often considers partner input and variability as bias. The strength of the current framework is that it offers the possibility to take this disadvantage and turn it into an advantage. All those who are involved in the program benefit from increased stakeholder participation: researchers can evaluate an intervention implementation in more realistic circumstances, whereas local partners have the ability to control and to adapt an intervention (as much as possible) to their needs, to enhance their ownership of the intervention, and to increase their capacities to develop, adapt and implement interventions in the future.

92

Research topic 3

Prediction

Van Daele, T., Van den Bergh, O., Van Audenhove, C., Raes, F., & Hermans, D. (2013).

Reduced Memory Specificity Predicts the Acquisition of Problem Solving Skills in

Psychoeducation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 44 , 135-

140. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.12.005

Van Daele, T., Griffith, J., Van den Bergh, O., Hermans, D. (2013). Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts changes in depression and anxiety in a community sample. Manuscript submitted for publication

93

94

Chapter 5

Reduced memory specificity predicts the acquisition of problem solving skills in psychoeducation

Background and objectives . Research has shown that overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) is a valid predictor for the course of depression. It is not known, however, whether OGM also moderates information uptake and consolidation in a psychoeducation program to prevent stress, anxiety and depression. The present study was designed to investigate whether the Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT;

Williams, & Broadbent, 1986) is a valid predictor for the actual unfolding of skills learned through psychoeducation. Methods . The questionnaire included primarily the

AMT and the Stress Anxiety Depression Means-Ends Problem Solving Questionnaire

(SAD-MEPS). It was filled in prior to and after the psychoeducational course by 23 participants. Results.

Correlations were calculated for the AMT at baseline and the differences between the pre and post measurements on the SAD-MEPS. Significant correlations were observed between the number of specific responses and the changes in the number of relevant means ( r = .49, p < .01). Limitations.

The sample size was rather small, but several checks were able to reduce the chance of spurious findings.

Conclusions : These findings may have important implications for the guidance to and the setup of psychoeducational interventions. Suggestions include screening and memory specificity training prior to course commencement.

Keywords autobiographical memory, psychoeducation, prevention, depression

95

Autobiographical memory and problem solving

1

Introduction

In the past twenty years a large amount of research has focused on the predictive validity of overgeneral autobiographic memory (OGM) for the course of depression. OGM is the difficulty to recall specific memories from one’s past life. Instead of reporting memories that refer to a specific context and last less than a day, overgeneral memories are reported. These last longer than a day or are recurring events (Gibbs & Rude, 2004). Within the population, there is some variance in the level of specificity between individuals, but people who suffer from depression or posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) generally have a less specific autobiographic memory (Harvey, Bryant, & Dang, 1998; Williams et al., 2007). Reduced memory specificity has been found to be a predictor for a worse course of depressive and trauma-related anxiety disorders. An early study by Brittlebank, Scott, Williams, and Ferrier (1993) with patients meeting the criteria of major depressive disorder (MDD) reported that overgenerality was highly correlated with failure to recover from depression (also see: Raes et al., 2006). Gibbs and Rude (2004) found that students with high frequencies of stressful life events and high OGM demonstrated higher levels of depressive symptoms four to six weeks later. In a study by Raes et al. (2008) OGM predicted an unfavourable course of depression in an electroconvulsive therapy, with patients high in OGM having increased depression scores at follow-up. Furthermore, for patients with PTSD, poor recall of specific memories of their trauma accounted for 25% of the variance of PTSD severity 6 months post trauma (Harvey et al., 1998).

Memory specificity also appears to be a valid predictor for the clinical status at follow-up for patients with MDD. According to Hermans et al. (2008), for these patients, high levels of OGM increased the probability of still being diagnosed with MDD three to four weeks later. In a recent meta-analysis,

Sumner, Griffith, and Mineka (2010) evaluated the available data for a total of

96

Chapter 5

15 studies. They concluded that OGM is predictive of more elevated depressive symptoms at follow-up.

Several explanations can be offered for why reduced memory specificity is associated with a worse prognosis in a variety of mental health disorders (see Raes, Williams, & Hermans, 2009). First, reduced memory specificity is known to block efficient social problem solving strategies. As mentioned by Baddeley (1988), when referring to non-clinical samples, specific memories of past situations are used as a frame of reference to guide problem solving in the present. So when people face a problem, they tend to think back to comparable situations and use their knowledge of what worked earlier on for such a problem. Overgeneralization, i.e. not being able to access these specific memories, leads to poor problem solving, both in non-clinical and clinical samples (Evans, Williams, O’Loughlin, & Howells, 1992; Goddart,

Dritschel, & Burton, 1996; Pollock & Williams, 2001; Raes et al., 2005). For depressed individuals, not being able to solve social problems can contribute to the continuation of depressive feelings and, in turn, lead to more persistent depression.

Second, individuals who are overgeneral about the past also appear to be overgeneral about their future. Williams, Ellis, Tyers and Healy (1996) reported that the degree of difficulty in generating specific images of the future was found to correlate with the extent to which subjects failed to retrieve specific autobiographical memories from their past. Such an aspecific, blurred perspective on the future might engender feelings of indifference and hopelessness.

Third, reduced memory specificity might lead to poor emotional processing.

After being faced with a negative event, thinking and talking in a specific manner about such an experience is an easy way to reexpose oneself, allowing the individual to emotionally process the negative emotional experience

(Littrell, 1998). Not being able to access specific memories of past situations

97

Autobiographical memory and problem solving may lead to a much lower level of natural exposure.

A fourth pathway suggested by Raes et al. (2006) is a reciprocal interplay of

OGM with rumination. Both may represent two different aspects of a common underlying process. This process would be discrepancy-driven and attempts to use analytical thinking (rumination) and overly general self-referent knowledge

(OGM) to solve personal problems. Two aspects of this process are key: 1) verbal-abstract analytic reasoning e.g. questions, ‘Where did it all go wrong?’ combined with 2) self-relevant material in the form of categoric statements

(‘Always alone’, ‘Feeling unhappy’, ‘Never relaxed’). Both of them interact and fuel each other, creating a spiral interaction, producing fruitless attempts resolving problems, thus enhancing depressive feelings and feelings of hopelessness (Williams et al., 2007).

The issue of how reduced memory specificity impacts the course of disorders like depression is both theoretically and clinically relevant. In addition to the four aforementioned pathways, we believe that there might be an additional process involved. More specifically, we argue that reduced memory specificity might impact the extent to which patients can benefit from psychotherapy. Most therapies include components of psychoeducation or include other elements of knowledge transfer or information that needs to be stored in memory (e.g. new insights, agreements and homework assignments).

Many of these elements play a crucial role in the effectiveness of interventions

(Stice, Shaw, Bohon, Marti, & Rohde, 2009). From our own experience, we know that patients can strongly differ in the extent to which they recall elements of previous therapeutic sessions. Some clients have very vivid and specific memories (e.g. ‘Last session, you mentioned how I make things more difficult by avoiding my sister. This has kept me busy. Observing myself last week, I think I must agree.’), whereas other patients are left with a mere general sense of what happened in the last session (e.g. ‘It was a good session’, we talked about stress.’). The extent to which specific memories can be

98

Chapter 5 retrieved with respect to therapy sessions may codetermine the efficacy of treatment. Patients who are more specific may benefit more, as they have better access to new perspectives and interpretations, suggested problem solving strategies, and will be able to implement these elements in between sessions (e.g. behavior change). Asa result, not being able to retrieve specific memories of therapy sessions might be an additional mechanism through which reduced memory specificity impacts the course of emotional disorders.

Some prospective studies have already focused on this the relationship between autobiographical memory specificity and treatment for depression

(Brittlebank et al., 1993; Peeters, Wessel, Merckelbach, & Boon-Vermeeren,

2002; Raes et al., 2006), finding some evidence that treatment effectiveness could be predicted by autobiographical memory specificity. Nevertheless, these have mainly focused on the predictability of memory specificity for complaints at a future time, whereas the current study focuses on whether memory specificity is predictive for the extent to which problem solving skills are acquired through a psychoeducational course.

In line with our hypothesis, the present study was therefore primarily designed to investigate whether differences in memory specificity are associated with the extent to which persons can benefit from a psychoeducational intervention for stress, anxiety and depression.

Psychoeducational interventions may be particularly sensitive to differences in memory specificity as they mainly rely on the transfer of information to impact behaviour outside the session. Because the psychoeducational program under investigation primarily focused on the acquisition of new problem solving skills, the dependent variable in this study concerned changes in problem solving skills from pre- to post-treatment. Rather than measuring problem solving through self-report, we employed a behavioural task (Mean Ends Problem

Solving task; MEPS, Platt & Spivack, 1975). The main advantage of using this behavioural task is that participants are not simply asked to report on how they

99

Autobiographical memory and problem solving solve problems in general, but actually have to solve presented problems. This test has been shown to provide more valid and accurate measures of participants’ problem solving capabilities. It was predicted that memory specificity would be associated with changes in problem solving skills, such that the more specific the participant (as measured at the start of the training), the more benefit in terms of problem solving skills acquisition. In addition, we also investigated changes in self-reported complaints, but these were considered as less relevant for the present research question given that such changes might be short-term effects that are unrelated to the process of acquiring long lasting problem solving skills (e.g. remoralization, hope, relief, social comparison). Two secondary short-term relationships were nevertheless expected. A first relationship was between changes in problem solving and selfreported complaints, with a higher increase in problem solving strategies being related to a larger decline in self-reported complaints. A second relationship was between memory specificity and self-reported complaints, such that the more specific the participant, the greater the decrease in self-reported complaints.

2

Method

2.1

Participants

Twenty-three students (17 women, 6 men) from two stress prevention courses organized in the spring of 2007, agreed to participate in this study. The average age was 21.7 years ( SD = 1.9; range 19-26) and all participants were Dutchspeaking. The courses were organized by the Psychotherapeutic Centre of the

KU Leuven and students were informed of these courses through the university’s website and/or by a general mailing to all Dutch-speaking students in the university. As an incentive for participation in the research, cinema tickets were raffled.

100

Chapter 5

2.2

Materials

2.2.1

AMT

Memory specificity was measured using the AMT. Based on the extended version of the Dutch AMT (Raes et al., 2008) two parallel versions were created containing 20 cue words each. The words were matched for familiarity, imageability and emotional intensity. In this version of the AMT, participants were asked to write down a specific memory in response to 20 cue words of alternating valence, with ten positive and ten negative words. Parallel version A contained the following words: pleasurable, angry, attentive, emotionally hurt, proud, angry, social, clumsy, enthusiastic, disappointed, self-confident, alone, competent, desperate, succeeded, jealous, surprised, ashamed, satisfied, and failed . Parallel version B consisted of: active, furious, interested, guilty, brave, powerless, safe, sorry, carefree, anxious, happy, scared, relaxed, lonely, successful, hopeless, brave, sad, helpful, and unhappy . These are all translations of the original Dutch words. Reported memories can be roughly divided in two groups. Specific memories are personal memories that refer to one particular event, localized moment in time, lasting less than one day (e.g. ‘The moment I heard I got the job at the pet shop’). Non-specific memories are subdivided in categoric and extended memories, omissions, same events and no memories.

Generalized categoric memories are memories that may have occurred more than once (e.g. ‘Every time I went to my friends’). Generalized extended memories on the other hand refer to an event that may have happened only once, but that lasted longer than one day (e.g. ‘During my vacation in Spain last year’). When memories were coded, this scoring procedure produced a good inter-rater reliability ( K = .79).

101

Autobiographical memory and problem solving

2.2.2

SAD-MEPS

The SAD-MEPS is an adapted version of the Means-Ends Problem Solving

Questionnaire (MEPS) developed by Platt and Spivack (1975). Like the original

MEPS the SAD-MEPS is used as assessment of problem solving skills. It consists of a series of short stories with interpersonal problem situations faced by a hypothetical protagonist. In each story, this protagonist is presented with a problem, which is immediately followed by the successful resolution.

Respondents have to provide the middle of the story with the means and strategies to reach its resolution. In the adapted version, stories are related to situations of stress, anxiety or depression (Hermans et al., 2008). Three parallel versions of the adapted format were used. In each version, three stories were presented: one concerning stress, one concerning anxiety and one concerning depression. Each story was scored for relevant means according to the manual of Platt and Spivack (1975). A relevant mean is defined as being a discrete sequential step enabling the protagonist to reach the goal described in the story. Furthermore, also the overall effectiveness of the stories was calculated on a 7- point Likert scale ranging from not at all effective to extremely effective following a method provided by Fischler, Kendall, and Vye (1982). For both scales, the average of all three stories was calculated. The inter-rater reliability was good for number of means ( ICC = .74) and acceptable for effectiveness ( ICC

= .62).

2.2.3

DASS-42

To assess levels of depression, anxiety and stress, the Dutch version of the

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-42 (DASS-42; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995;

Dutch version by de Beurs, Van Dyck, Marquenie, Lange and Blonk (2001)) was used. The internal consistency for the three subscales in this study was high

(.92 > α > .95).

102

Chapter 5

2.2.4

PSWQ

The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, &

Borkovec, 1990; Dutch version by van Rijsoort, Vervaeke, and Emmelkamp,

1997) was included in the study to measure beyond typical symptoms.

Worrying was therefore considered as an additional symptom and as an underlying process of psychopathology (Raes et al., 2005). The questionnaire showed an acceptable internal consistency ( α = .78) in this study.

2.3

Procedure

2.3.1

Intervention

The psychoeducational course ‘Stress Control’ is based on the work of Jim

White (Borkovec & Whisman, 1996; White & Keenan, 1990; White, Keenan, &

Brooks, 1992). It is aimed at groups and delivered by a trained clinical psychologist. For six sessions of two hour, twelve steps are run through. The first step corresponds with the first session and consists of general information concerning stress. This provides a framework for the session to follow. Steps 2 and 3 are linked to the first session and are homework assignments. The idea behind this is that participants learn to apply the general information to their own personal situation and create a personal frame of reference. In this way they will have less effort to select the information that is being presented in the subsequent sessions which can be relevant for them. Steps 4 to 12 are used to examine a few contents in a more detailed way. Step 4 (Session 2) takes a closer look to body control combined with breathing exercises; Step 5 discusses

(Session 3) cognitive techniques in combination with relaxation; and Step 6

(Session 4) teaches new skills in combination with relaxation. Step 7 to Step 10

(Session 5 and 6) used the knowledge from the first session and the learned techniques from Session 2 and 3 and apply them to anxiety, panic, sleeping problems and depressive feelings, and burnout. Finally, Steps 11 and 12

103

Autobiographical memory and problem solving

(Session 6) provide an overview as guidance for future stress control. Both courses were administered by the same clinical psychologist, who had received prior training by the organization that adapted the original course to the

Flemish situation.

2.3.2

Assessment

Questionnaires were provided using the online survey site SurveyMonkey.com

(SurveyMonkey.com, Portland, Oregon, USA). First the AMT was presented

(only before the intervention), followed by the DASS, PSWQ and MEPS

(administered before and after the intervention). These were made available one week before course commencement and one week after course completion. To monitor each individual’s evolution, participants had to enter a personal code before commencing with the questionnaire. After agreeing to an informed consent form, they were able to fill out the questionnaires.

3

Results

3.1

Participant characteristics

DASS scores before the intervention were 11.48 ( SD = 7.80) for depression,

12.48 ( SD = 9.05) for anxiety and 18.61 ( SD = 10.72) for stress. For worrying, a mean PSWQ-score of 48.74 ( SD = 8.42) was obtained. Overall, compared to

Dutch normative data (de Beurs, personal communication, 28 October 2007),

DASS scores were closer to those of a clinical sample than to scores of the normal population. Worrying, furthermore, was slightly higher compared to the normal Dutch population (van Rijsoort et al., 1997).

104

Chapter 5

3.2

Changes in self-reported complaints

DASS scores show a significant decline of complaints on all subscales, especially for depressive complaints (Table 7). Worrying, as measured by the PSWQ, shows a borderline statistically significant decline.

Tabel 7 Changes in self-reported complaints (N = 23) after course participation

3.3

Autobiographical memory and changes in problem solving skills

Correlations were calculated for the AMT categories at baseline and the differences between the pre and post measurements on the two SAD-MEPS scores. Significant correlations were observed between the number of specific responses and the changes in the number of relevant means. This was true for the overall level of specific memories ( r = .49, p < .01). Memory specificity went hand in hand with positive changes in the number of suggested problem solving strategies throughout the psychoeducational program (Figure 6). In addition to the associations with memory specificity, a correlation was observed between changes in ‘number of means’ on the SAD-MEPS and the number of general extended responses ( r = -.39, p < .05). Finally, a borderline statistical significant correlation was found for changes in overall effectiveness and the no memory category, ( r = -.35, p = .06).

105

Autobiographical memory and problem solving

Tabel 8 Means (percentages) and standard deviations on the different categories of the autobiographical memory test (AMT) (N = 23)

3.4

SAD-MEPS Performance & relationship to complaints

For all participants, the average number of means reported in the SAD-MEPS was 3.04 ( SD = 1.03) before the course and 3.09 ( SD = .98) after course completion. The quality of the reported meanswas 4.03 ( SD = 1.00) before course commencement and 3.87 ( SD = .82) afterward. Neither the change in means nor the change in effectiveness of the SAD-MEPS responses were significant, both F s < 1. Furthermore, correlations were calculated for the DASS and the PSWQ change scores and the two SAD-MEPS subscales. No statistically significant correlations were found ( r s < .33, p s < .13).

3.5

AMT Performance and relationship to complaints

Approximately 45% of the responses on the AMT were specific (Table 8).

Furthermore, 12% of the memories were general categoric and 30% were general extended. Change scores were computed by subtracting pre intervention scores from post intervention scores. Subsequently, correlations were calculated for the DASS and the PSWQ change scores, and the AMT baseline scores. Significant correlations were retrieved for the DASS-Anxiety change scores, with the number of specific memories provided ( r = .49, p =

106

Chapter 5

.009) and general extended memories ( r = -.48, p = .01). As such, the more specific memories and the less general extended memories a participant provided prior to course commencement, the larger the decrease of the score on the DASS Anxiety scale at post intervention.

Figure 6.

Relationship between the AMT percentage of specific memories and the SAD-MEPS change score for number of means.

4

Discussion

The goal of this study was to determine whether differences in memory specificity are associated with the extent to which persons can benefit from a psychoeducational intervention for stress, anxiety and depression. Although there is already evidence for the overall effectiveness for psychoeducational group interventions for stress, anxiety, and depression (Cuijpers, Muñoz,

Clarke, & Lewinsohn, 2009; Neil & Christensen, 2009; Van Daele, Van

Audenhove, Hermans, Van den Bergh, 2011), little is known about participant characteristics that may influence course outcome at the individual level.

107

Autobiographical memory and problem solving

For autobiographical memory, on average almost half of the answers were specific in nature, but there was a certain amount of variability between participants. No overall significant changes were reported when looking at the effects of the psychoeducational intervention on problem solving strategies.

When the relationship between the AMT and the change scores for the SAD-

MEPS was considered however, some significant relationships were observed.

More specifically, a high number of specific memories and/or a low level of general extended memories as measured by the AMT was found to predict an increase in the number of relevant means used in SAD-MEPS’ problem solving tasks. One could assume that this relationship might be accounted for by changes in a communal third, for example executive functioning. In a study with depressed patients Raes et al. (2005) already found that when SAD-MEPS effectiveness was regressed on rumination, memory specificity, and working memory functioning, memory specificity was the only significant predictor of

MEPS effectiveness. Whether this also applies to the acquisition of problem solving skills is not yet known and will require further research.

Looking at in self-reported complaints, a significant decline was found on all subscales of the DASS, indicating a short-term beneficial effect of the psychoeducational course. Although these changes are positive, they are considered to be only temporary and unrelated to the process of acquiring long lasting problem solving skills, which in turn would lead to an actual, more permanent reduction of complaints. This is in line with findings of previous studies e.g. by White and Keenan (1990) and White et al. (1992) and also with the current study not finding any correlations between changes in problem solving as measured by the SAD-MEPS and these short-term changes in complaints. Furthermore, there were only a limited number of significant correlations between the AMT baseline scores and the DASS- and PSWQ change scores. The absence of this relationship might seem surprising given the research evidence on the predictive validity of the AMT for the course of

108

Chapter 5 depression, which was presented in the introduction. It is nevertheless plausible that the time between pre and post measurement is not sufficiently long enough to detect actual, long lasting changes at the symptom level. This could explain the absence of these correlations.

As for other limitations: a first limitation the present study is its small sample size ( N = 23), which limited the power of the statistical tests. A second limitation is that no follow-up data could be provided. It is expected that individuals would continue to apply the problem solving skills taught during psychoeducation to their own situations and therefore these would improve over time. Because this assumption is dependent on further skill acquisition, consolidation and continued practice, it is uncertain which correlations could be expected when measured at a later point in time (e.g., six months after course completion). A third limitation is that no information was gathered on whether participants made clinical criteria for depression or if they were undergoing any treatment during the time of the intervention. A final limitation is the predominantly female composition of the study. Psychoeducational interventions nevertheless primarily attract women and a relatively higher proportion of women is associated with overall better results (Van Daele et al.,

2012). Therefore, having a primarily female composition does not make these findings less relevant for the target group.

Nonetheless, if subjected to replication, these findings may have important implications for the guidance and the setup of (psychoeducational) interventions that intend to transfer knowledge and skills to targeted groups. A first suggested step could be to setup an initial (self) screening of course participants by means of the AMT, in order to determine whether their memory specificity is sufficient to benefit from a psychoeducational intervention. This will of course require further research on the predictive capacities of the AMT, in order to determine what exactly corresponds with insufficient memory specificity (e.g. determine a cut-off score). A second, and

109

Autobiographical memory and problem solving even more important step, would be to subsequently provide memory specificity training to people with low memory specificity. This would enable them to maximize their potential and to benefit fully from a psychoeduational course.

110

Chapter 6

Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts changes in depression and anxiety in a community sample

This study investigated whether overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) predicts the course of symptoms of depression and anxiety in a community sample, after 3, 4, 6,

12 and 18 months. Participants ( N = 156) completed the Autobiographical Memory Test and the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21 (DASS-21) at baseline and were subsequently reassessed using the DASS-21 at four time points over a period of 18months. Using hierarchical linear modelling, we found that OGM predicted long-term changes at 12 months, B31 = 22.4, p = .005, and at 18 months, B41 = 13.1, p = .025, from baseline for depression. OGM furthermore predicted long-term changes for anxiety at

12 months, B31 = 16.3, p = .026. Similar to other studies, no short-term predictive effects were found. The results from the current study provide additional support for the hypothesis of OGM as trait-like characteristics, as it also appears to predict the evolution of symptoms of depression and anxiety in a community sample.

Keywords autobiographical memory, depression, anxiety, prediction, community sample

111

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression

1

Introduction

Overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) is defined as a difficulty in recalling specific memories from one’s life. It was first investigated by Williams and Broadbent (1986) in a sample of suicidal patients. The most common assessment tool for OGM is the Autobiographical Memory Task (AMT; Williams

& Broadbent, 1986), which exists in several different versions. Generally, participants are asked to provide specific memories in response to a set of cue words. The instructions state that a specific memory is a personal memory of one particular event that is localized in time lasting less than one day (e.g., ‘The moment I found out that I got the job’; Williams et al., 2007). The valence of the cue words is usually alternated between positive (e.g. ‘happy’) and negative

(e.g. ‘sad’). Although the goal is to retrieve specific memories, participants often generate various types of non-specific memories. Some of these nonspecific memories are ‘categoric’, which are events that have occurred more than once (e.g. ‘Every time I go to my favourite pub’). Other non-specific memories include ‘extended’ memories (lasting longer than one day, e.g.

‘During my vacation in Spain last year’) or are ‘non-memories’ (a.k.a. semantic associates). People suffering from clinical depression generally report fewer specific and more categoric memories (van Vreeswijk & de Wilde, 2004;

Williams et al., 2007). These are considered to be the best indications for OGM.

1.1

OGM and depression

There is evidence that OGM also predicts increases in depression following stressful life events, as shown by Mackinger, Loschin, and Leibetseder (2000) in a sample of pregnant women and by Gibbs and Rude (2004) in students experiencing high frequencies of stressful life events. Rawal and Rice (2012) showed similar findings in a sample of 277 adolescents who were at familial

112

Chapter 6 risk for depression. These participants were initially free from a depressive disorder, but OGM predicted the onset of MDD.

In depressed participants, however, OGM does not seem to be related to the level of symptoms, as it actually appears to predict the course of their depression (Brittlebank, Scott, Williams, & Ferrier, 1993). In a sample of 22 patients with MDD they found that OGM was highly correlated with the failure to recover from depression and accounted for 33% of the variance in the

Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, 1960) at seven months follow-up. Since this first study, similar results have been observed, for example in 25 patients with MDD in a study by Peeters, Wessel, Merckelbach, and Boon-Vermeeren (2002) and in a study by Raes et al. (2006) for 28 MDD patients, both after seven months of follow-up. In conclusion, OGM seems to predict the course of depression in specific populations including students and clinical populations (Sumner et al., 2011).

1.2

OGM and anxiety

Since mood and anxiety disorders are often comorbid (e.g, Mineka, Watson, &

Clark, 1998; Kessler et al., 2005), it is possible that OGM is related to anxiety and depression alike. A number of studies have already found evidence for an immediate relationship between OGM and PTSD. Studies by McNally Lasko,

Macklin, and Pitman (1995) and McNally, Litz, Prassas, Shin, and Weathers

(1994) reported that Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD exhibited more difficulty in recalling specific memories compared to those without PTSD.

Schönefeld, Ehlers, Böllinghaus, and Rief (2007) expanded upon these findings in a study with 42 assault survivors. They reported that participants with PTSD retrieved fewer specific memories and more overgeneral memories compared to assault survivors without PTSD, but only when they were explicitly instructed to suppress assault memories. For other anxiety disorders, the relationship between OGM and anxiety is less clear. For example, Wilhelm, McNally, Baer,

113

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression and Florin (1997) found that patients with obsessive compulsive disorder do not exhibit OGM, unless they suffer from concurrent MDD. Furthermore, OGM also does not seem to be associated with general anxiety disorder, social anxiety disorder or blood and spider fearful individuals (Williams et al., 2007).

There are some indications, however, that OGM might be related to changes in anxiety. Until now, the majority of these studies focused on OGM predicting anxiety in the context of trauma or stressful life events (Williams et al., 2007). Harvey, Bryant, and Dang (1998) found that participants who developed acute stress disorder (ASD) following motor vehicle accidents reported fewer specific memories in response to positive cue words, compared to those who did not develop ASD. van Minnen et al. (2005) reported that for women who failed an in vitro fertilization treatment, the number of reported specific memories at baseline was negatively related to depressive and anxiety symptoms after the treatment. Furthermore, Bryant, Sutherland, and Guthrie

(2007) conducted a study with fire fighters over a period of four years. They found that impaired retrieval of specific memories in response to positive cues prior to trauma exposure significantly predicted posttraumatic stress severity after exposure to trauma. Finally, a study by Kleim and Ehlers (2008) showed that for participants who were measured two weeks after an assault, OGM predicted PTSD six months later over and above what could be predicted from symptom severity and initial diagnoses of MDD, acute stress disorder, or assault-related specific phobia. As of yet, no studies have focused on the relationship between OGM and the occurrence of anxiety outside of trauma research. Thus, there is little evidence that OGM is related to current symptoms of anxiety. The current study sought to expand this area of research by examining the relationship between OGM and anxiety as well as depression.

114

Chapter 6

1.3

OGM and the evolution of depression and anxiety

When studies focus on OGM and its relationship with the evolution of emotional disorders, symptoms are often considered at one specific follow-up point. Examples are the study by Anderson, Goddard, and Powell (2010) in students with one follow-up after three months, the study by Dalgleish, Spinks,

Yiend, and Kuyken (2001) in patients with seasonal affective disorder with one follow-up after seven months and the study by Raes et al. (2006) in depressed inpatients which also included one follow-up after seven months. Although these studies provide insight into the relationship between OGM and psychopathology, they give little information on the trajectory of change over time. Studies that include multiple discrete time points have the advantage that they assess the form of the relationship over time. At present, few studies have included multiple follow-up moments. Two of them focused on depressed patients. A first was the study by Brittlebank et al. (1993) in which OGM in response to positive words showed stable correlations to depression measured at three months and seven months follow-up. A weaker relationship was found for negative cue words. A second study was conducted by Peeters et al. (2002) in 25 patients with MDD. In this study, the strongest effect was found for OGM in response to negative cue words, which predicted depression at three months. Borderline significant results were reported at seven months.

Furthermore, a study by Hipwell, Reynolds, and Pitts Crick (2004) with healthy pregnant women found that OGM did not predict post-partum depressive symptoms after two weeks. After eight weeks, however, OGM did significantly predict depression. Hermans et al. (2008) focused on students who failed their first exams at the university. No significant correlation was found between

OGM and change scores for depression at baseline and at a first follow-up after two weeks. However, after nine weeks, a significant correlation for OGM and change scores was found.

115

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression

In summary, there is evidence for a relationship between OGM and changes in symptoms of depression and anxiety, but the available studies are difficult to compare and few of them include long-term follow-up at more than one time point.

1.4

Current study

It is clear that OGM can predict the course of depression in some populations, and that OGM also predicts changes in anxiety. However, little is known on whether OGM can successfully predict symptoms of anxiety and depression outside the specific contexts of stressors and traumas. To redress these gaps in the literature, we recruited a community sample and set out to examine the potential of OGM to predict symptoms of depression and anxiety alike. Our sample received questionnaires at five different time points (3 months, 4 months, 6 months, 12 months and 18 months) to accurately determine the trajectory of this relationship. This follow-up is the longest known in literature for a community sample. It is also one of the longest follow-up studies on OGM in general, together with those by Spinhoven et al. (2006) and Bryant,

Sutherland, and Guthrie (2007), which included follow-up periods for 49 and 24 months respectively.

We hypothesised that OGM at baseline would predict the trajectory of anxiety and depression. Specifically, we hypothesised that a higher proportion of categoric memories and/or a lower proportion of specific memories at baseline would be associated with an increase in symptoms of depression and anxiety over time.

116

Chapter 6

2

Method

2.1

Participants

Our sample included 156 participants (53 men and 103 women; mean age 38.8 years, SD = 14.1, range = 18.9 – 68.8) from Flanders, the northern, Dutchspeaking region of Belgium. Potential participants responded to advertisements in local newspapers to participate in a questionnaire study concerning their general well-being. Participants received € 10 per data collection wave for participating. They were recruited as a convenience sample in a matched control study evaluating the effectiveness of a psychoeducational intervention.

In this context a number of them would be matched with intervention participants. For the current study, we only considered this control group, as we were interested in the natural course of depression and anxiety and whether OGM predicts this course.

2.2

Materials

2.2.1

Autobiographical Memory Task

Memory specificity was measured using the AMT. Participants were presented with ten cue words and asked to provide a different, specific memory in response to each of them. The words were of positive and negative valence and presented in alternating order. Translated from Dutch, the positive cues were: pleasurable, attentive, proud, social and enthusiastic; negative cues were: angry, emotionally hurt, angry, clumsy, and disappointed. Responses were scored as falling into one of six categories: specific memories, categoric memories, extended memories, no memory (a.k.a. semantic associates), repeated events, and omissions. Extended memories refer to an event that may have happened only once, but that lasted longer than one day. A memory is labeled as ‘same event’, when participants violate instructions and an event

117

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression is repeated at least once in response to different cue words. The ‘no memories’ category mostly concerns semantic associations and irrelevant answers.

Omissions are non-responses. Inter-rater reliability was determined for the responses of 30 participants. The level of agreement between raters was high

( K = .84; Viera & Garrett, 2005). In addition, multiple studies support good psychometric properties of the AMT (Heron et al., 2012; Griffith et al., 2009;

Griffith, Kleim, Sumner, & Ehlers, 2012, Griffith, et al., 2012).

2.2.2

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales

Participants completed the DASS-21, including the depression (DASS-21-D) and anxiety (DASS-21-A) subscales. Each subscale contains 7 items and sum scores range from 0 to 42. Although symptoms of depression and anxiety are often highly correlated, the DASS has adequate discriminant validity (Crawford &

Henry, 2003). Internal consistency for both the depression and anxiety subscale is excellent with αs of .95 and .90 respectively.

2.3

Procedure

Participants completed the AMT and DASS-21 at Time 1, which were subsequently sent back by mail to the investigators. During the follow-ups, the

DASS-21 was the only measure as participants no longer received the AMT.

Time 2 was five months after Time 1; Time 3 was six months after Time 1; Time

4 was one year after Time 1; Time 5 was 18 months after Time 1. Drop-out was limited, with a maximum of 9 percent at any time point: 156 participants completed the questionnaire at Time 1, 153 at Time 2, 144 at Time 3, 145 at

Time 4, and 145 at Time 5. As a part of the larger treatment effectiveness study, participants also completed additional measures, which were unrelated to the current study.

118

Chapter 6

2.4

Analyses

For both DASS subscales, if fewer than 20% of the items were missing (i.e. not more than one item), we prorated the scores by assigning the missing item the average value of the other items of the subscale. We used hierarchical linear modelling (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) to examine the effect of overgeneral memory on the trajectory of complaints of depression and anxiety. Because

OGM, anxiety and depression were not normally distributed in this sample, we used robust standard errors for significance testing. Participants had to complete at least the baseline and one additional time point in order to be included in the analyses. Full maximum likelihood estimation, which is robust in the presence of missing data, was used in the estimation of model parameters.

Table 9 presents both models.

Table 9 Hierachical Linear Model and results of DASS-21-subscales of depression and anxiety as the dependent variables.

119

120

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression

The level-1 models for depression and anxiety, respectively, contained the within-subject relationship between time point and the DASS-21. Time point was dummy coded such that one variable measured the change from baseline to T2 and from baseline to respectively T3, T4, and T5. Level 2 of the model contained the regression of the level-1 parameters on baseline AMT. At level 2, error terms were included for the four level-1 slopes to capture participant-toparticipant differences that might exist for the relationship between time point and depression or anxiety.

3

Results

3.1

Descriptive statistics

Table 10 presents means and standard deviations for DASS-21-subscales of depression and anxiety. At each time point, there was, on average, five percent of data missing (after scores were prorated). We furthermore calculated AMT proportions by dividing the number of responses for each category by the total number of actual responses minus the number omissions (i.e., the denominator of each proportion was the total number of responses possible with the number of non-responses subtracted).

Table 10 Descriptive statistics for the DASS-21-subscales of depression and anxiety.

Chapter 6

Participants reported 64% ( SD = 27) specific memories, 5 % ( SD = 10) categoric memories, 17% ( SD = 16) extended memories, 14 % ( SD = 19) no memories, and 1 % ( SD = 0) repeated events. In 2% ( SD = 8) of the cases, no response was given.

3.2

OGM and symptom changes

To test our hypothesis on the relationship between OGM and the change in depression, we used DASS-21-D as the dependent variable in the regression analysis. For the relationship between OGM and changes in anxiety, the DASS-

21-A was used as the dependent variable in the regression using robust standard errors. When OGM was operationalized as the proportion of specific memories, no significant results were found. However, when OGM was operationalized as the proportion of categoric memories we found it did not significantly predict depression at Times 1, 2, or 3, but did predict it at Times 4 and 5 (Table 9). Prediction of anxiety was significant at Time 4, and approached significance at Time 5. In terms of the size of the effects, the coefficients that indicate the effect of baseline categoric memories for depression were largest at Time 4, B31 = 22.4, p = .005, and decreased at Time 5, B41 = 13.1, p = .025.

For anxiety, the coefficients were also significant at Time 4, B31 = 16.3, p =

.026, but the effect only approached significance at Time 5, B41 = 12.5, p =

.063.

4

Discussion

We studied a large community sample at five time points, with the last time point 18 months after baseline. OGM predicted changes in symptoms of depression and anxiety in this community sample. The large number of time points allowed for a precise assessment of the form of the relationship.

Inspection of the size of the regression coefficients for each of these time

121

OGM predicts evolution of anxiety and depression points (Table 9) showed that the effects were small at first, increased gradually, and were strongest after one year. After one year, the effect sizes diminished.

Overall, effects are slightly stronger for depression compared to anxiety. A limitation of this study is that we did not assess life stress or trauma, which may also be related to anxiety and depression. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find significant correlations at six months, which has been reported in other OGM studies (Sumner, Griffith, & Mineka, 2010). A possible explanation is that OGM has less influence on non-clinical samples, which has been hypothesised by Raes, Hermans, Williams, and Eelen (2007). This would explain why a longer follow-up period is required for our community sample before significant (negative) effects of OGM on symptoms of depression and anxiety are found. The increasing influence of confounding factors (e.g. environmental or personal changes) in turn might be the reason why the strength of the effects diminishes after peaking at one year follow-up and are no longer significant at 18 months.

Most studies up until now have focused on the relationship between OGM and changes in depression, so it is important to extend this research into anxiety. Furthermore, previous studies on OGM and anxiety have always considered the effects of OGM for a select number of people who faced highly stressful or traumatic situations, whereas the current study broadens its applicability to a more general community sample. A final merit is that we were able to map changes across several time points.

In our sample, we did not find significant correlations between OGM and symptoms of anxiety and depression at baseline, but hierarchical linear modeling allowed us to examine changes in anxiety and depression from baseline; these findings were consistent with our hypotheses. OGM may be an underlying process involved in the exaggeration of symptoms, which is consistent with the hypothesis of Williams et al. (2007) – that OGM may be a trait-like characteristic that serves as a vulnerability factor for depression. As

122

Chapter 6 pointed out by Sumner, Griffith, & Mineka (2010) there was already evidence that OGM was associated with the later increase of depressive symptoms in non-clinical samples, but only mixed evidence for OGM predicting the course of depression. This study adds additional support to the hypothesis of OGM as a trait-like characteristic, as it also appears to predict the evolution of symptoms of depression and anxiety in a community sample.

More research is needed on OGM across several time points. This could provide a better view on changes in the strength of the relationship between

OGM and depression and anxiety. If our hypotheses are correct, a gradual incline in the relationship between OGM and symptoms of anxiety and depression, followed by a slight decline could be found in clinical samples in the short-term (e.g. six months to one year) and in non-clinical samples in the long(er) term (e.g. one year to one year and a half, or even longer). Finally, further replication of these results in non-clinical samples could confirm the potential of the AMT as a screening measure for internalizing psychopathology in the general population. If similar results can also be found in clinical samples

(for example in patients with MDD), this would provide additional evidence for

OGM as a trait-like characteristics that can act as a significant vulnerability factor for depression.

123

124

Chapter 7

General discussion

Mental health is becoming increasingly important in high income countries

(World Health Organization, 2005; World Health Organization, 2012). However, these countries also face more people with mental disorders (Mathers &

Loncar, 2006). Furthermore, even when conservative numbers are considered, it appears that the majority of these people lack access to professional care.

Currently, less than one in three receives treatment (Kessler et al., 2005).

Ironically, we simultaneously also face the issue of overtreatment, due to significant numbers of false positive detections of mental disorders in primary care setting (Mitchell, Vaze, & Rao, 2009). If MHC ever wants to be able to meet the demands and provide sufficient, efficient and adequate care for all, a paradigm shift is required. In this paradigm shift, the position of MHC organizations in society has to evolve from instances that primarily treat disorder to services that also provide education, coaching and prevention. A practical implication of this paradigm is the creation of a more accessible and extended primary care, a goal to which this doctoral dissertation hopes to contribute on a number of levels.

In this general discussion, we will offer a synthesis of the three main research topics and their results. Next, we will discuss some limitations of the research in this doctoral dissertation, together with future perspectives based on these specific studies as well as on available literature. We will conclude this general discussion with some considerations on the characteristics of policy oriented research and a number of general and specific policy recommendations.

125

General discussion

1

Research topics and main results

The research topics of this doctoral dissertation might seem diverse, but are all united under the goal of policy oriented research. The first topic was

‘effectiveness’, which set out to gain additional insight in the overall evidence base for psychoeducational group interventions that focus on stress reduction.

We furthermore evaluated the effectiveness of a local adaptation of a psychoeducational group intervention. The second research topic was

‘implementation’ and focused on means to increase both fidelity and adaptation when researchers and their partners in the field collaborate in evaluating intervention effectiveness. A third and final research topic considered the possibilities for ‘prediction’. Within this research topic, the potential of autobiographical memory was investigated in predicting intervention success for individual participants and in predicting the evolution of mental health in a community sample.

1.1

Effectiveness

A first focus was to evaluate the effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions for stress reduction in general. The goal was twofold: to provide an overview of the short- and long-term effectiveness of psychoeducational group interventions for stress and to chart possible moderators of intervention effectiveness. The systematic review and meta-analysis reported small, but consistently positive effect sizes for 16 studies at post-treatment (Cohen’s d =

.27). This provided a clear indication for the short-term effectiveness for this type of interventions, with a larger overall effect compared to similar metaanalyses (Martin, Sanderson, Cocker & Hons, 2009; Stice, Shaw, Bogon, &

Marti, 2009). Long-term effectiveness, measured for nine studies after on average six months, was less pronounced (Cohen’s d = .20). Furthermore, as for the moderators, a model including intervention duration and participant

126

Chapter 7 gender was found to explain 42% of the variance in effects. Apparently, short lasting psychoeducational interventions for women were most effective. These results are of course correlational and we therefore had to refrain from making firm causal interpretations.

The finding that shorter interventions were more effective could be considered surprising. One could have expected that the more time spent working on and learning about stress and stress-related problems, the more knowledge transfer and skill development would occur. However, in the literature, this relationship is also not consistently reported. Richardson and

Rothstein (2008) for example reported similar results as they found that shorter interventions produced better results. They even reported a distinct pattern in which interventions that relied solely on relaxation techniques benefit from longer duration, whereas interventions that were multimodal (a description that fits almost all of the interventions in our meta-analysis) appear to lose effect as their length increases. Aside from their (slightly) larger effectiveness, shorter programs are also likely to be more cost effective and practical to implement. Therefore, we tend to agree with the authors when they state that short-term interventions just might be sufficient – and perhaps even better – than programs of longer duration. As surprising as the results on intervention duration might have been, as unsurprising is the finding that interventions with larger numbers of women appear to accomplish better results. Several explanations can be found; a first is that women typically report more stress than men (Matud, 2004). The higher their initial level of symptoms, the more change there can occur and the less chance there is for floor effects.

A second reason is that they have a more positive attitude concerning psychological openness and they have more favourable intentions to seek help from mental health professionals, compared to men. As Bertakis, Azari, Helms,

Callahan, and Robbins (2000) state, these might be just some of the reasons why they also make more frequent use of (mental) healthcare services. This is

127

General discussion also in line with findings by Pyne et al. (2003) who reported that their primary care intervention for depression was cost-effective for women, but not for men.

A second focus was to evaluate the effectiveness of ‘Stress Control’ (White,

2000, adaptation by ISW Limits, 2006). A total of 47 people participating in the intervention were first matched with an equal number of controls on sociodemographic variables and baseline levels of symptoms. Results of trend analyses showed a steady linear decline of stress and depression in the intervention group from pre-intervention to post-intervention and further on through follow-up. These effects were not observed in the non-intervention group. There also was a strong decline in worrying, but only little change in stress management skills. Furthermore, measures of clinical significance showed that almost 30 percent of all participants experienced a clinically significant and reliable change in the year following the course. Finally, the strongest effect occurred for those participants who presented themselves with higher levels of initial symptoms. Within the intervention group, there was a considerable amount of variation, which is why solely presenting the study outcomes for the group as a whole might be misleading. When we compared participants with low and high initials symptoms, participants with high initial levels showed a much stronger continuous and gradual decline of symptoms whereas most participants with low initial symptoms remained stable. Similar results have already been found in other interventions (Rowe, 2000, 2006).

Some might consider this heterogeneity as a design flaw: if we would have had a stricter participant recruitment policy, the participant group as a whole could have been more homogenous. However, we wanted to implement the intervention in real life circumstances in the field and measure how effective it is in these conditions. Therefore, we only controlled a limited number of parameters (i.e. the explicit exclusion criteria for potential high risk participants). Because of that, we ended up with large differences between

128

Chapter 7 participants, but we believe this represents a more accurate and better reflection of this stress management course when it is implemented in the field.

1.2

Implementation

The second research topic was a direct consequence of (struggles with) the evaluation of the effectiveness study. Personal experiences combined with a search in literature resulted in the construction of a framework for implementation research. Empowerment implementation provides a new look at the concept of implementation fidelity and intervention effectiveness, as it reconciles the apparent contradiction between adaptation and fidelity (Castro,

Barrera, & Martinez, 2004). More specifically, when implementing interventions in the field, researchers have always struggled keeping their interventions as pristine as possible, in order to safeguard their effectiveness

(Burns, Peters, & Noell, 2005). These well-meant attempts often had an adverse effect though, as local partners were not fully convinced of the proposed approach or decided to change vital intervention features anyway

(Levy, Baldyga, & Jurkowski, 2003). When commencing the effectiveness study in three regions in Flanders, we became aware of these issues, as we noticed that small changes were required to the initial implementation plan in order to accommodate to local needs, suggestions or demands. In small scale interventions that are tied to one particular setting it might be easy to make such changes: deviations are documented and reported as ‘condiciones sine quibus non’. This subsequently raises little issues for the gathering of data, reporting of results and the final evaluation. However, because our intervention had to be implemented in three different regions, it quickly became clear that small deviations from the initial plan in each region could possibly results in large differences between implementation settings. In the end, this could hamper comparability across regions. Not accommodating to

129

General discussion the needs and suggestions of local partners was out of the question, as the goal of the Policy Research Centre Welfare, Health and Family, is to obtain results that are highly relevant for both policy and practice (KU Leuven, UGent, VUB, &

KHK, 2007). Active involvement of partners in the field is considered key in this context.

We therefore developed empowerment implementation, which tries to overcome this issue. In this framework, an intervention consists of two parts: a core component and less important intervention aspects. The core component is proven effective in clinical trials and remains untouched throughout the implementation process. Less important intervention aspects are decided upon through an intensive collaboration between researchers and local partners. The framework therefore consists of three main phases: partner selection, deciding upon practical aspects and deciding upon content-related-aspects. As such, it addresses and overcomes the issues of implementation fidelity and adaptation.

Because this framework has a broad applicability, it is interesting for all researchers wishing to evaluate an intervention implementation in more realistic circumstances, while also using partner input to their advantage. In the end, all of those involved in the program benefit from increased stakeholder participation, including the local partners (World Health Organization, 1986).

Because of their involvement, they have the ability to control and to adapt an intervention to their needs, to enhance their ownership of the intervention, and to increase their capacities to develop, adapt and implement future interventions. This framework proved to be particularly useful for our effectiveness study and because we wanted to spread this knowledge, we decided to consolidate our findings in a publication in which we used the effectiveness study as an illustration of how to put this framework into practice.

130

Chapter 7

1.3

Prediction

Group psychoeducational interventions target large amounts of people and often make little distinction between different participant profiles. This can be considered an inherent strength of this intervention type, as it offers anonymity and does not force people to interact with other participants and professionals or to disclose personal information, which may lower the threshold for participation (Mischoulon et al., 2001). However, it also has a downside, as not all participants may benefit equally. Therefore, an important improvement would be to predict the success for each individual in advance and make use of targeted referral for this type of interventions. Because determining this chance of success should require little time and should also not be intrusive, the Autobiographical Memory Task (AMT; Williams &

Broadbent, 1986) was considered. This instrument is often used to determine both memory specificity and overgeneral memory, concepts that have previously shown their merits in predicting the course of mental health disorders in a clinical context (e.g. Raes, Williams, & Hermans, 2009). In a first study, evidence was found for memory specificity as a predictor for the acquisition of problem-solving skills during a group psychoeducational intervention ‘Stressbeheersing’ (ISW Limits, 2006), a local adaptation of the intervention ‘Stress Control’ (White, 2000). More specifically, questionnaires including the AMT and the Stress Anxiety Depression Means-Ends Problem

Solving Questionnaire (SAD-MEPS; Hermans et al., 2008) were administered.

This SAD-MEPS is used as assessment of problem solving skills. It consists of a series of short stories with interpersonal problem situations faced by a hypothetical protagonist. In each story, this protagonist is presented with a problem, which is immediately followed by the successful resolution.

Respondents have to provide the middle of the story which contains the means and strategies to reach its resolution. A total of 23 participants filled in these

131

General discussion questionnaires prior to and after the psychoeducational course. Subsequently correlations were calculated for the AMT at baseline and the difference between the pre and post measurements on the SAD-MEPS. Significant correlations were observed between the number of specific responses and the changes in the number of relevant means.

Because of the encouraging results in the psychoeducational group intervention, the AMT was also considered as a predictor for the course of depression and anxiety in a nontreated convenience sample, i.e. the larger community sample ( N = 157) from which the matched control group in the effectiveness study was sampled. Using hierarchical linear modelling

(Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) OGM managed to predict changes in the long-term for depression and anxiety. No effects were found after three, four and six months. However, categoric memories did appear to predict depression and anxiety after one year. After 18 months, only the significant relationship between categoric memories and depression remained.

Before both these studies were conducted, OGM had already successfully been applied in different situations. In clinical contexts it can serve as a predictor for the course of depression (e.g. Brittlebank, Scott, Williams, &

Ferrier, 1993) and for the clinical status at follow-up for patients with MDD

(e.g. Hermans et al., 2008). Furthermore, OGM can also tell us something about whether people are capable to have specific expectations and hopes about their future (Williams, Ellis, Tyers, & Healy, 1996) or if people can successfully manage negative emotional experiences (Littrell, 1998). These studies now add additional applications to the use of OGM as it might also tell us something about whether overgeneral patients can benefit from psychoeducation (or maybe even psychotherapy in general) and if people from a community sample might be vulnerable for developing (symptoms of) anxiety and depression.

However, questions still remain. For our study on the acquisition of problem-solving skills, it is for example not quite clear whether these changes

132

Chapter 7 are actually caused by OGM or if a communal third like executive functioning might also play a role. A study by Raes et al. (2005) already found that when the effectiveness of problem-solving skills was regressed on rumination, memory specificity and working memory functioning, memory specify was the only significant predictor of the effectiveness of problem-solving skills.

Whether this also applies to the acquisition of problem-solving skills, is nevertheless not yet clear. For our study with the community sample, alternative explanations for the observed effects also exist, as there might be a reciprocal interplay between OGM and rumination, both of which could represent different aspects of a common underlying process (Raes et al., 2006).

Such a process would combine continuous worrying with overgeneral thinking about one’s particular situation, as such creating a negative spiral interaction leading to emotional problems.

2

Limitations and future perspectives

Within this PhD, we have focused on three different research topics. For each of them, there are of course some methodological limitations, which will be discussed below. These limitations also draw our attention to opportunities.

Therefore, we will also make recommendations for future research.

Furthermore, this research will also be framed within the larger context of policy related research and the challenges this upholds.

2.1

Effectiveness

2.1.1

Limitations

The meta-analysis only used published articles, which made our study – just like any other meta-analysis – prone to publication bias. We tried to avoid this by using failsafe N-statistics (Rosenberg, 2005), but it remains highly likely that a

133

General discussion number of interventions finding no effects probably never have been published and therefore went undetected. Furthermore, in order to successfully conduct the meta-analysis only randomized controlled trials could be considered in which most studies made use of waitlist controls and no treatment controls.

We can therefore not conclude that psychoeducational group interventions for stress are better than other specific interventions. Furthermore, we can also not compare these interventions to treatment or care as usual, which could have been achieved by (including) non-inferiority trials (Chevalier, 2009), although such results are far more difficult if not impossible to include in a meta-analysis. We can therefore only conclude that these interventions are more effective in dealing with stress than undertaking no action at all.

As a first remark concerning the effectiveness study, we did not make use of random allocation of participants, which seems rather inherent of research in the field making use of long-term follow-up. Withholding (wait list) control participants from following an intervention – even a psychoeducational stress course – seemed deontologically incorrect. We found the matched control design to be the most practical and ethical solution. Using this design, we could have long-term follow-up because our control group did not require denying people treatment. However, the main limitation of the study is that the matching procedure was far from perfect. Although we managed to recruit a large number of participants for the control group and could successfully match people on socio-demographics like age, gender and socio-economic status, the level of complaints at baseline turned out to be far higher for in the intervention group compared to the control group. It therefore proved to be a difficult task to find enough people with sufficiently high complaints in the control group, which led to unequal baseline levels of complaints in the two groups.

A second remark concerns the fact that this primary care intervention was initially also presented as a primary preventive intervention. This is one of the

134

Chapter 7 three types of prevention, according to Caplan (1964, cited in Van den Broucke,

2001), together with secondary and tertiary prevention. Primary prevention tries to reduce the incidence of new cases of a specific mental disorder.

Secondary prevention aims at reducing the prevalence by means of early detection and treatment, whereas tertiary prevention aims at reducing the rate of residual disability of chronic ill patients and also attends to them. The goal of a psychoeducational group intervention with a focus on primary prevention is therefore primarily to teach people who are mentally healthy how to safeguard their mental health. A number of participants of the intervention actually belonged to that target group and could be considered – according to the definition of Keyes (2007) – as flourishing. However, the majority of participants were facing at least mild to moderate symptoms. The course could therefore not be considered as a primary preventive intervention. However, because complaints were often still subclinical and the course could help people to avoid crossing the ‘clinical threshold’, it may still be considered as a preventive intervention, although ‘only’ focussing on secondary prevention.

A third remark concerns the participant profile. As often, the people attracted by the intervention were on average middle-aged highly educated women. It goes without saying that this makes the results hard to generalise to other subpopulations. On the other hand, our meta-analysis had already shown that other psychoeducational interventions for stress were evaluated with similar participant groups, because in general (highly educated, employed) women make most use of these services.

2.1.2

Suggestions for future research

Both studies also have their merits: aside from their findings, they also offer suggestions for future research. The meta-analysis points out two major issues in current research. A first is that still too little studies make use of (sufficient) follow-up and effects are only considered in the period immediately following

135

General discussion an intervention. Even when follow-up is included, six months is often considered as strong evidence for long-term effects. More and longer follow-up is however required to truly determine long-term effects. Furthermore, a lot of studies report too little information on intervention characteristics. When conducting moderator analyses we often encountered unclarities or unspecified aspects of interventions. Since we are still unaware of what the exact factors are that contribute to the (lack of) effectiveness of psychoeducational interventions, documenting and reporting intervention characteristics as detailed as possible is paramount. Only if we move beyond reporting standard information like average age and gender, and also start including characteristics that are not commonly reported like group sizes, ethnicity, and presence of interaction, we will obtain a better view of factors influencing effectiveness.

The main conclusion of the effectiveness study is in line with that of the meta-analysis, but also offers some nuance. Our theoretical reflection remains correct that effectiveness research with longer follow-up is necessary. When implementing in the field however, methodology like the ‘gold standard’ RCT, seems to reach its limits. Withholding treatment from people for long periods in real life contexts not only seems, but actually is unethical. Alternatives like matched control designs have their merits, but their validity is highly dependent on the matching process and the pool of subjects from which they can be recruited. Because this premise is partially violated in our own study, conclusions had to be cautious. Future research studies will therefore have to find a balance between producing studies of high internal validity, like RCTs, and setting up studies of high ecological validity that make use of sufficiently long follow-up. Balancing both contradictory needs may prove to be a difficult task.

Furthermore, the general search throughout the literature that was a part of this doctoral dissertation also pointed out to the need for a more extensive

136

Chapter 7 way of conducting research. When reading articles on intervention studies, the strong emphasis on statistical significance of obtained effects cannot be ignored. In general it seems that for a large part of the scientific community it is even the major – if not the only – means to confirm effectiveness. However, the goal for almost any intervention should be to obtain improvements for participants or patients that truly matter for them . Realising a mere statistically significant change on a number of highly specific preselected symptoms might be insufficient. Making sure that change is not only reliable but also meaningful should be an important consideration. Paying more attention to clinical significance – in case of severe initial symptoms – might be one way to obtain this goal. This can be done by systematically monitoring the evolution of participants during interventions using validated questionnaires of which normative data is available for large population groups. On the one hand, this is advantageous for participants because it allows for detailed and personal feedback on their progress. On the other hand, the advantage for researchers is that this approach not only offers insight in whether any change is occurring for participants, but also whether that change is meaningful from a clinical perspective. Other monitoring efforts could also focus on paying more attention to changes concerning the perception of mental disorders by patients and to their general quality of life. These suggestions are without a doubt not only relevant to research; ideally they could also be incorporated in everyday practice.

Finally, in recent years there has been on a strong increase in the number of economic analyses of (preventive) interventions. These compare the estimates of economic burden to the costs of the program designed to reduce that burden and compared to the outcomes achieved by the intervention (Corso &

Lutzker, 2006). An optimal cost effectiveness analysis ideally targets an intervention that is well-defined, short-term and with a measurable outcome.

When Sefton (2000) considered social welfare interventions, he however found

137

General discussion most interventions had multiple, long-term outcomes, which were often qualitative in nature. The programs were also heterogeneous and often local variations existed. The overall interventions effects were rather small and user involvement appeared to be a determining factor for intervention effectiveness. Taking these considerations into account, the importance, the need and the possible merits of such analyses for preventive interventions in the field of mental health should be acknowledged. However, when considering the cost-effectiveness of interventions, results should always be interpreted with caution and within the larger contextual framework.

2.2

Implementation

2.2.1

Limitations

For true empowerment of stakeholders, involvement is ideally present throughout the whole research project (Levy et al., 2003; Wallerstein & Duran,

2006). Empowerment implementation should therefore be followed by empowerment evaluation (Fetterman, 1996). This implies that partners would be closely involved with the evaluation of the project. However, because clear research objectives were determined in advance by policy makers, this was unfortunately not possible in our study. Furthermore, although we made every effort to involve partners in the field as much as possible during the implementation, the explicit and detailed construction of the systematic framework was only done after the actual implementation. If we could have constructed this prior to the implementation, we might have obtained even more relevant information and could have used this to optimize the framework even further. Although we consider the current framework to be coherent and well constructed, it certainly should not be considered a final version, as there will always be room to add more detail and perfect it further.

138

Chapter 7

2.2.2

Suggestions for future research

We especially hope that this framework will be used by other researchers when conducting research with partners in the field. Not only to actively apply empowerment implementation in numerous similar contexts, but also to specifically adapt and improve it. One suggestion for such an improvement could concern the way in which interventions are presented towards participants during their implementation. Motivational research has already shown that there is a difference in how people perceive the pursuit of promotion versus the pursuit of prevention. Pursuing health promotion implies striving towards positive outcomes (gains) or trying to avoid the absence of positive outcomes (non-gains). Pursuing prevention, however, implies striving towards the absence of negative outcomes (non-losses) or trying to avoid negative outcomes (losses) (Molden, Lee, & Higgins, 2007). A common misconception is that prevention is all about avoiding undesired outcomes and promotion focuses solely on achieving desired outcomes. However, whereas both approaches are quite different, their outcome can be the same (Carver,

2004). For example, two individuals can strive to obtain the same positive goal, reducing their stress level. For one person, this might be in order to avoid becoming depressed (prevention concern), whereas for another it may well be a way to achieve more calmness (promotion concern).

Although the focus on prevention or promotion may shift within an individual depending on the circumstances or context, there is evidence for the fact that some people are chronically promotion- or prevention-focused (Lee et al., 2000). For now, this motivation research has mainly focused on individuals, but it may also have opportunities in the field of intervention implementation.

Our intervention was mainly presented as prevention: dealing with stress, in order to avoid depression and anxiety. This approach may appeal far less to people who have a health promotion-focused mindset. Could the reach or even the effectiveness of the intervention be influenced by changing the way an

139

General discussion intervention is portrayed, depending on the target group or even depending on each individual participant?

An intervention that better matches the motivational goals of individuals could perhaps improve their intervention adherence, their retaining of information, and the time and effort they invest in the intervention. Achieving such an improved match between intervention and individual would only require minor adaptations to the original intervention: the actual content does not require change, but only the way it is portrayed. Such adaptation might of course be difficult to achieve for group interventions as the intervention cannot be tailored to fit each and every individual. However, there are certainly interventions in which the way content is portrayed can be tailored more easily

(e.g. e-mental health interventions). It does not seem unlikely that this may have substantial effects. This could therefore be an interesting next step in improving the implementation strategy of preventive interventions.

2.3

Prediction

2.3.1

Limitations

The first study found that memory specificity could act as a predictor for the acquisition of problem-solving skills during a group psychoeducational intervention. There were several limitations to this study. As a first, its sample size was rather small. Furthermore, the participants were predominantly female. This limitation was however not a major drawback, given that we previously found in the meta-analysis that psychoeducational interventions actually attract mostly women and the results are therefore still relevant for the participant group as a whole. Furthermore, only post measurements were available. Given our strong emphasis on the need for follow-up in intervention studies, it is unfortunate that for this specific study, we do not dispose of longterm follow-up data. A limitation of the second study is that it made use of a

140

Chapter 7 convenience sample, which is not the same as a non-clinical sample, as we could not determine whether individual participants suffered from a DSM-IV disorder. Furthermore, we also were not aware of the occurrence of possible stressors, like aversive (life) events that may have influenced symptoms of participants.

2.3.2

Suggestions for future research

Because of the small sample size, a next step would be to replicate the findings from the first study. If replicated, these findings may have important implications. When guiding people towards this particular or other similar interventions, a first step could be an initial (self) screening during which potential participants can determine whether they might benefit from the intervention or not. A second and possibly even more important step is that memory specificity training (MeST; Raes, Williams, & Hermans, 2009) could be offered to people with low memory specificity prior to the psychoeducational intervention. This would enable them to maximize their potential and to benefit fully from the intervention. Such a training program consists of four weekly one hour sessions, offered to small groups of three to eight participants. The first session mainly concerns psychoeducation about memory functioning in relation to depression, after which participants are offered homework exercises that require them to generate specific memories for ten cues, in preparation of the second session. The second session starts with discussing the homework, after which participants are asked to recall detailed specific memories for four cues (two positive and two negative) and they again receive ten cues to prepare for the next session. Session three and four are similar to session two, but focus more on negative cues. In the end, a brief summary of the whole program is offered and participants can evaluate the course and share their personal experiences with the training with the other group members and the trainer. A study by Raes et al. (2009) showed that the

141

General discussion retrieval style of participants became significantly more specific following the training. Neshat-Doost et al. (2012) found similar results and also reported that lower levels of depression at two month follow-up compared to a control group. Finally, Raes, Schoofs, Griffith and Hermans (submitted) replicated these findings in a non-randomised controlled trial and furthermore also found that for MeST participants, increases in memory specificity were maintained at oneyear follow-up.

The second study provides preliminary indications that the concepts of memory specificity and OGM could be relevant for predicting the increase of symptoms of depression and anxiety. This offers opportunities for the AMT as a screening measure for internalizing psychopathology in the general population.

Because we furthermore know that memory specificity can also be trained, it may also be relevant to incorporate MeST as a component in more general preventive interventions in primary care.

Aside from these suggestions that focus on more applied research, both studies also highlight opportunities for basic research, as a number of theoretical questions remain. For each study, we like to highlight one. As for

OGM and the acquisition of problem solving skills, an interesting question is whether a communal third like executive functioning also play a role in the acquisition of problem solving skills. For the relationship between OGM and symptoms of anxiety and depression in a community sample, a hypothesis worth investigating is whether a reciprocal interplay between OGM and rumination could be the explanation for a negative spiral, leading to depression or anxiety. In both cases, future research is required.

3

Characteristics of the research in this PhD

The topics in this doctoral dissertation might seem quite diverse. This is a consequence of the focus of this PhD which was not solely to conduct in-depth

142

Chapter 7 research, but especially to advance general knowledge and my personal expertise across the domains of research, policy and practice. These three domains interact closely and decisions in one domain almost always influence the others. The interaction between these domains is visualised in Figure 7 and will be discussed in detail below.

For this PhD, Policy is represented by the Policy Research Centre Welfare,

Health and Family. It has the specific goal to support policy makers in pursuing an effective and innovative policy, and to do so with scientific results (KU

Leuven et al., 2007). Closely involved in the Research component are the

Research Group on Health Psychology, the Centre for Excellence:

Generalization in Ill Health and Psychopathology and LUCAS, the centre for

Care Research and Consultancy, all units of the KU Leuven. Last but certainly not least is Practice , which is embedded in the complex context of MHC in

Flanders (one of the regions of Belgium).

Figure 7.

Relationship between policy, research and practice.

143

General discussion

Policy interacts with research (1) as time and means are often limited and interests and opportunities have to be balanced. Research is done on a per project basis, of which both the duration and focus do not always correspond with the typical trajectory for obtaining a doctoral degree. Research on the other hand also interacts with policy (2) , as results obtained in research are translated into policy recommendations which can subsequently for example be incorporated in national guidelines or action plans. The results on the effectiveness study can for example be framed within the larger context of

Flemish action plan on prevention of suicide (Coppens, Scheerder, & Van

Audenhove, 2011). Research furthermore also influences practice (3) , as concepts that are found in basic research are translated to relevant tools and interventions which can be applied by partners in the field. Both articles on the

AMT have the literature on OGM and memory specificity as a basis. We used this evidence from basic research and translated it to the field, in a search for highly specific applications, like predicting the capability of stress course participants to acquire problem-solving strategies or for predicting the evolutions of symptoms of anxiety and depression in a community sample. The other way around, feedback and concerns from practice are also conveyed to research (4) . These concerns help shape both content and implementation of tools and interventions, for which empowerment implementation is an example. Finally, practice and policy also interact closely (5+6) and in this interaction they often rely on information from research to find suitable compromises between what is preferred by practitioners and with is possible for (government) administrators.

In conclusion, applied research may be broader and therefore less in depth, but it also has several merits. Because it is conducted in more realistic circumstances, obtained research evidence is more relevant for everyday practice and also for policy makers. This makes further translation and

144

Chapter 7 subsequent dissemination of findings easier and improves its chances for success.

4

Specific implications and general policy recommendations

4.1

Specific implications

The results from this doctoral dissertation have a number of specific implications for practitioners and for policy makers. First, it provides good prospects for the use of group (psychoeducational) interventions as a means of primary and/or secondary prevention. Although there is still some level of uncertainty concerning the effectiveness, we have found that different organisations in the field of primary (mental) healthcare are capable of successfully setting up primary care interventions through means of ad hoc partnerships. Second, throughout the dissertation it became clear that primary care interventions may provide an opportunity for MHC to redefine its services.

These interventions may not only reach a greater number of people in need of care, in some cases they may even be used to prevent (the deterioration of) subclinical mental disorders. As they are set up within the local community and with well known partners from outside of medical contexts, or virtually through means of the internet, this makes the threshold for participation lower in both cases. A key advantage is furthermore that they may be less associated with stigma, which still remains an important issue.

The main challenge for these primary care interventions seems to be in the actual reaching of the people in need or even the people not (yet) in need. The slogan we would like to propose in this context is: ‘reach out to where the people are’, an adage which can also be found in the context of neighbourhood work (Henderson & Thomas, 2013). Currently, MHC acts mostly as a service provider which attracts a limited number of people who struggle and reach out

145

General discussion themselves with a request for help. Healthcare professional subsequently try to accommodate their needs, alleviate their distress, or reduce their symptoms.

This concerns however a limited number of people, who professionals often only see when much harm has already been done. If MHC however wants to make optimal use of primary care interventions, it has to quit being a mere service provider and also be more proactive by promoting such services. This implies that mental health professionals should actively try to reach people who have increased risks for mental disorders.

At a structural level, two main components are required to evolve to a better MHC: 1) integration of mental health services into primary care and 2) an intense integrated and collaborative care between service providers (Collins,

Hewson, Munger, & Wade, 2010). Taken together, this doctoral dissertation portrays a positive and hopeful message, as it shows that both aspects are well within our reach and that these can already be realised successfully in small scale studies.

4.2

General policy recommendations

If we transcend our research further, some general policy recommendations and long term proposals can also be made. In a post-financial crisis period, means are scarce and everyone – including the MHC sector – has to be economical. It may therefore seem sensible to invest most (or maybe even all) of these limited means in those who currently require them most: the patients in need of (urgent) psychological care. However, although this group may certainly not be forgotten and their funding and support should not be interrupted, there is also a need for a future-oriented policy that extends beyond maintaining the status-quo. The current situation is far from ideal, with a significant number of people suffering from mental disorders because they are lacking professional care (Bebbington et al., 2000; Kessler & Wang, 2008).

146

Chapter 7

Without additional efforts, we should not expect any drastic change in the near future, if any even deterioration (Mathers & Loncar, 2006).

4.2.1

Beyond the healthcare sector

In order to address this issue, we first need to extend our scope beyond the healthcare sector. Unemployment, adverse neighbourhood characteristics, low income, education, socio-economic status (SES) and income inequality are all associated with mental disorders, as described in a meta-analysis by Lund et al.

(2010). It might therefore seem like a good idea to set up primary care interventions targeted at people from a lower SES aimed at teaching them how to cope with stress. However, as their stress may likely be caused by unemployment or by other factors related to their current standard of living, such initiatives might just be a bit perverse. Although there is ‘no health without mental health’ , a first prerequisite for everyone still remains to safeguard more basic needs, labelled as ‘ physiological’ and other ‘safety needs’ by Maslow (1954) already a long time ago. The primary goal for every policy maker should therefore still be to first address such issues. It seems however unrealistic to expect that such societal problems will be easily addressed or quickly resolved. Furthermore, not every mental disorder or all symptoms of depression, anxiety ... can be traced back to e.g. poor standards of living or to income inequality.

4.2.2

Innovations within MHC

MHC therefore still needs to evolve further. For people with severe mental disorders, an increased attention to outreaching is required and for the general population the attention should shift, preferably from curing to educating, coaching and promoting mental health.

Without a doubt, advanced care for people with severe symptoms and mental disorders will always remain an aspect of MHC. When such care is

147

General discussion organised, it is preferably done in a context of integral care, in which the needs, questions and goals of people who require care are taken into account in order to pursue good quality of life for each patient (SARWGG, 2012). Similar to these changes is the choice for more outreaching of care, in which patients are no longer always treated in specialised hospitals, but in their own living environment. Assertive community treatment (ACT) is such an intervention type in which care providers first focus on gaining patients’ trust in order to make them voluntarily engage in treatment (Mulder & Kroon, 2009).

As previously discussed in the introduction, there are furthermore some essential changes required in order to provide a better fit of MHC with the general population as a whole (Werkgroep Eerstelijnsgezondheidszorg, 2010).

At the level of the general public and patients, it is important to increase mental health literacy, but also to improve the general attitude, as stigma remains an important issue (Lasalvia et al., 2012). Also for health professionals, there should be increased attention to knowledge and attitudes towards mental health. One particularly promising initiative is the introduction of primary care psychologists. Primary care psychologists offer generalist, shortterm support in primary care in close collaboration with other professionals like

GPs. This initiative has already proven its merits in The Netherlands (Emmen,

Meijer, & Verhaak, 2008) and may also offer opportunities to promote and disseminate primary care interventions further to the broad general public. Last but certainly not least is the policy level: all of the realisations mentioned above required substantial funding, something MHC – and especially prevention in MHC – is lacking. Aside from the important role policy makers can play in providing additional funding, they can also help practitioners guide this transition. For this they can rely on both basic and applied research can help them to take the leap.

Because any policy should rely on the best available information, an important goal remains therefore to continue investing in policy research. This can help policy makers gather insights in the current state

148

Chapter 7 of MHC, how particular interventions work, for whom these are most effective and how they should be implemented. This knowledge has high practical value, as it can be used to shape current and future MHC and as it also helps to make optimal use of all resources currently available. Research evidence on the effectiveness of stepped care (e.g. van Straten, Tiemens, Hakkaart, Nolen, &

Donker, 2006; van‘t Veer-Tazelaar et al., 2009) and the use of primary care interventions like group psychoeducational interventions (e.g. Van Daele et al.,

2012), and e-mental health (e.g. Griffiths, Farrer, & Christensen, 2010) all has to be taken into account when shaping the MHC system of the future.

If we can manage to realize these prerequisites, MHC might be able to successfully restructure itself. During this restructuring, MHC has the opportunity to convey a strong and positive message: mental health is more than the absence of mental illness and everyone can learn how to maintain or improve his or her mental health. However, for those who struggle and who need additional help or guidance, individual ambulatory consultation and residential treatment is still available. Translating this stepped-care approach to any local situation will require a great deal of effort, but should preferably start today, rather than tomorrow.

149

150

References

Achterkamp, M. C. and Vos, J. F. J. (2008) Investigating the use of the stakeholder notion in project management literature, a meta-analysis. International Journal of

Project Management, 26, 749–757.

Alonso J., Angermeyer M. C., Bernert S., Bruffaerets, R., Brugha, T. S., Bryson, H.,

... Vilagut, G.(2004). "Prevalence of mental disorders in Europe: results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project". Acta Psychiatr Scand 109 (S420), 21–27.

Ahlin, K. & Billhult, A. (2012). Lifestyle changes - a continuous, inner struggle for women with type 2 diabetes: A qualitative study. Scandinavian Journal of Primary Health

Care, 30, 41-47.

American Psychiatric Association (1995). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorder Fourth Edition: Primary Care Version . Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychological Association. (2010). Stress in America findings [Electronic version]. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Amirkhan, J. H. (1990). A factor-analytically derived measure of coping: The Coping

Strategies Indicator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59 (S), 1066-1074.

Anderson, R. J., Goddard, L., & Powell, J. H. (2010). Reduced specificity of autobiographical memory as a moderator of the relationship between daily hassles and depression. Cognition & Emotion, 24 (4), 702-709.

Andersson, G., Paxling, B., Roch-Norlund, P., Östman, G., Norgren, Almlöv ... Silverberg,

F. (2012). Internet-Based Psychodynamic versus Cognitive Behavioral Guided Self-

Help for Generalized Anxiety Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychother

Psychosom, 81, 344-355.

Andrews, G., Cuijpers, P., Craske, M. G., McEvoy, P., & Titov, N. (2010). Computer

Therapy for the Anxiety and Depressive Disorders Is Effective, Acceptable and

Practical Healthcare: A Meta-Analysis. PLoS One, 5 (10), e13196.

Andrews, G., Issakidis, C., Sanderson, K., Corry, J., & Lapsley, H. (2004). Utilising survey

151

References data to inform public policy: Comparison of the cost-effectiveness of treatment of ten mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry , 184 , 526- 533.

Andrews, G. & Titov, N. (2010). Is internet treatment for depressive and anxiety disorders ready for prime time? MJA, 192, S45-S47.

Andrews, G., & Wilkinson, D. D. (2002). The prevention of mental disorders in young people. Medical Journal of Australia , 177 , S97-S100.

Angermeyer, M. C., Holzinger, A., Matschinger, H. (2009). Mental health literacy and attitude towards people with mental illness: A trend analysis based on population surveys in the eastern part of Germany. European Psychiatry, 24 , 225-232.

Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (2005). Have there been any changes in the public's attitudes towards psychiatric treatment? Results from representative population surveys in Germany in the years 1990 and 2001. Acta Psychiatr Scand,

111, 68-73.

Antoni, M. H. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention effects on anxiety, 24-hr urinary norepinephrine output, and T-cytotoxic/suppressor cells over time among symptomatic HIV-infected gay men. Journal of Consulting & Clinical

Psychology , 68 , 31-45.

Aro, A., Van den Broucke, S., & Rätly, S. (2005) Towards European consensus based tools to review the evidence and enhance the quality of health promotion practice.

Promotion and Education, 12, 10–14.

Ayalon, L., Arean, P., & Bornfeld, H. (2008). Correlates of knowledge and beliefs about depression among longterm care staff. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry,

23 , 356-363.

Azjen, I. (1988). Atittudes, Personality and Behavior.

Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

Backer, T. E. (2001) Finding the Balance—Program Fidelity and Adaptation in Substance

Abuse Prevention: A State-of-the Art Review.

Center for Substance Abuse

Prevention, Rockville, MD.

Baddeley, A. D. (1988). But what the hell is it for? In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R.

N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Memory in everyday life, Vol. 1 (pp. 3-18) Chichester, England: Wiley.

Baert, H., De Witte, K., & Sterck, G. (2001) Vorming, training en opleiding. Handboek voor een kwaliteitsvol VTO-beleid in welzijnsvoorzieningen [Formation, Training and

152

References education. Manual for a high quality training policy in welfare organisations].

Garant, Leuven.

Barley, E. A., Murray, J., Walters, P., & Tylee, A. (2011). Managing depression in primary care: A meta-synthesis of qualitative and quantitative research from the UK to identiy barriers and facilitators. BMC Family Practice, 12, 47.

Barrera, A. Z., Torres, L. D., & Muñoz, R. F. (2007). Prevention of depression: The state of the science at the beginning of the 21st century. International Review of

Psychiatry , 19 , 655-670.

Basen-Engquist, K., O’Hara-Tompkins, N., Lovato, C. Y., Lewis, M. J., Parcel, G. S., &

Gingiss, P. (1994) The effect of two types of teacher training on implementation of

Smart Choices: a tobacco prevention curriculum. Journal of School Health, 64, 334–

339.

Bartholomeeusen, S., Kim, C. Y., Mertens, R. (2005). The denominator in general practice, a new approach from the Intego database. Fam Pract, 22, 442-447.

Bartholomew,L. K., Parcel, G.S., Kok, G.J., Gottlieb, N. H. (2011) Planning health promotion programs. An intervention mapping approach.

San Francisco: Jossey-

Bass.

Bäuml, J., Froböse, T., Kraemer, S., Rentrop, M., & Pitschel-Walz, G. (2006).

Psychoeducation: A basic psychotherapeutic intervention for patients with schizophrenia and their families.

Schizophrenia Bulletin , 32 (Suppl. 1), S1-S9.

Bebbington, P. E., Brugha, T. S., Meltzer, H., Jenkins, R., Ceresa, C., Farrell, M., et al.

(2000). Neurotic disorders and the receipt of psychiatric treatment. Psychological

Medicine, 30, 1369-1376.

Bebbington, P. E., Meltzer, H., Brugha, T. S., Farell, M., Jenkins, R., Ceresa, C., & Lewis,

G. (2000). Unequal access and unmet need: Neurotic disorders and the use of primary care services. Psychological Medicine , 30 , 1359-1367.

Beck, A. T. (1981) Cognitive Therapy in Depression. Wiley, Chichester.

Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mock, J., Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4 , 561–571.

Bell, J. S., Whitehead, P., Aslani, P., Sacker, S. & Chen, T. F. (2006). Design and implementation of an educational partnership between community pharmacists and consumer educators in mental healthcare. American Journal of Pharmaceutical

153

References

Education, 70, 28.

Berman, P. (1981) Educational change: an implementation paradigm. In Lehming, R. and

Kane, M. (eds), Improving Schools: Using What We Know.

Sage, London, pp. 253–

286.

Bertakis, K. D., Azarai, R., Helms, J., Callahan, E. J., Robbins, J. A. (2000). Gender

Differences in the Utilization of Health Care Services, 49 (2), 147-152.

Biglan, A., Ary, D., & Wagenaar, A.C. (2000). The Value of Interrupted Time-Series

Experiments for Community Intervention Research. Prevention Science, 1 (1), 31-49.

Bijttebier, P., & Vertommen, H. (1997). Psychometric properties of the coping strategy indicator in a Flemish sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 23 (1), 157-

160.

Blakely, C. H., Mayer, J. P., Gottschalk, R. G., Schmitt, N., Davidson, W., Roitman,

D. B., & Emshoff, J.G. (1987) The fidelity-adaptation debate: Implications for the implementation of public sector social programs. American Journal of Community

Psychology, 15 (3) , 253-268.

Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., & Seay, R. B. (2007). Age differences in everyday problem-solving effectiveness: Older adults select more effective strategies for interpersonal problems. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Science, 62B , 61-64.

Borkovec, T. D., & Whisman, M. A. (1996). Psychosocial treatments for generalized anxiety disorder. In M. Mavissaklian, & R. F. Prien (Eds.), Long-term treatment of anxiety disorders (pp. 171-199).

Breitenstein, S. M., Gross, D., Garvey, C., Hill, C., Fogg, L., & Resnick, B. (2010)

Implementation fidelity in community-based interventions. Research in Nursing &

Health, 33, 164–173.

Brittlebank, A. D., Scott, J., Williams, J. M. G., & Ferrier, I. N. (1993). Autobiographical memory in depression: state or trait marker. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162,

18-121.

Brosschot, J.F., Gerin, W., Thayer, J.F. (2006). The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiolocial activation, and health.

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113-124.

Brouwer, W., Oenema, A., Crutzen, R., de Nooijer, J., de Vries, N.K., Brug, J. (2009).What makes people decide to visit and use an internet-delivered behavior-change

154

References intervention? A qualitative study among adults. Health Education, 109 (60),

460-473.

Brown, T. A. & Barlow, D. H. (2005). Dimensional Versus Categorical Classification of

Mental Disorders in the Fifth Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders and Beyond: Comment on the Special Section. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 114 (4), 551-556.

Brown, J. S. L., Boardman, J. Whittinger, N., & Ashworth, M. (2010). Can a self-referral system help improve access to psychological treatments? British Journal of

General Practice, 60, 365-371.

Bryant, R. A., Sutherland, K., Guthrie, R. M. (2007). Impaired Specific Autobiographical

Memory as a Risk Factor for Posttraumatic Stress After Trauma. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 116 (4), 837-841.

Buntinx, F., De Lepeleire, J., Heyrman, J., Fischler, B., Vander Mijnsbrugge, D., & Van den

Akker, M. (2004). Diagnosing depression: what's in a name? Eur J Gen Pract,

10 (4), 162-165.

Burlingame, G. M. (2010). Small group treatments: Introduction to special section.

Psychotherapy Research, 20, 1-7.

Burns, M. K., Peters, R., & Noell, G.H. (2008). Using performance feedback to enhance implementation fidelity of the problem-solving team process. Journal of School

Psychology, 46, 537-550.

Caldwell, T. & Jorm, A. F. (2001). Mental health nurses’ beliefs about likely outcomes for people with schizophrenia or depression: a comparison with the public and other healthcare professionals.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Mental

Health Nursing, 10, 42-54.

Calvert, J. (2006). What's Special about Basic Research?. Science, Technology, & Human

Values, 31 (2), 199-220.

Campbell, M., Fitzpatrick, R., Haines, A., Kinmonth, A. L., Sandercock, P., Spiegelhalter,

D., & Tyrer, P. (2000) Framework for design and evaluation of complex interventions to improve health. BMJ, 321, 694–696.

Carrington, P., Collings, G. H., Benson, H., Robinson, H., Wood, L., Lehrer, P. M., ...

Cole, J. W. (1980). The use of meditation-relaxation techniques for the management of stress in a working population. Journal of Occupational Medicine , 22 , 221-231.

155

References

Carroll, C., Patterson, M., Wood, S., Booth, A., Rick, J. & Balain, S. (2007). A conceptual framework for implementation fidelity. Implementation Science, 2 .

Carver, C. S. (2004). Self-regulation of action and affect. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. VOsh

(Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory and applications (pp. 13-39).

New York: Guilford Press.

Cary, M., & Jagdish, D. (1999). Cognitive-behavioral and systematic desensitization procedures in reducing stress and anger in caregivers for the disabled. International

Journal of Stress Management , 6 (2), 75-87.

Castro, F. G., Barrera, M., & Martinez, C.R. (2004). The Cultural Adaptation of

Prevention Interventions: Resolving Tensions Between Fidelity and Fit. Prevention

Science, 5, 41-45.

Cepoiu, M., McCusker, J., Cole, M. G., Sewitch, M., Belzile, E., & Ciampi, A. (2008).

Recognition of Depression by Non-psychiatric Physicians - A Systematic

Literature Review and Meta-analysis. J Gen Inter Med,23 (1), 25-36.

Chandola, T., Brunner, E., & Marmot, M. (2006). Chronic stress at work and the metabolic syndrome: Prospective study. British Medical Journal , 332 , 521A-524A.

Chevalier, P. (2009). Non-inferioriteitsstudies: het nut, de beperkingen en de valkuilen.

[Non-inferiority trials: the use, limitations and the pitfalls]. Minvera: Tijdschrift voor

Evidence-based Medicine, 8( 6), 88.

Collins, C., Hewson, D. L., Munger, R., & Wade, T. (2010). Evolving models of behavioral health integration in primary care. New York, NY: Millbank Memorial Fund.

Collins, E., Katona, C., & Orrell, M. W. (1997). Management of depression in the elderly by general practitioners: Referral for psychological treatments. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 445-448.

Coppens, E., Scheerder, G., Van Audenhove, Ch. (2011). De evaluatie van het Vlaams

Actieplan Suïcidepreventie [The evaluation of the Flemish Action Plan for Suicide

Prevention].

Leuven: LUCAS, KU Leuven.

Corrigan, P. W., Larson, J. E., & Rüsch, N. (2009). Self-stigma and the "why try" effect: impact on life goals and evidence-based practices. World Psychiatry, 8 , 75-81.

Corso, P. S. & Lutzker, J. R. (2006). The need for economic analysis in research on child maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30, 727-738.

156

References

Christensen, H. & Hickie, I.B. (2010). Using e-health applications to deliver new mental health services. MJA, 192 , S53-S56.

Cousineau, T. M., Green, T. C., Corsini, E., Seibring, A., Showstack, M. T., Applegarth, L.,

... Perloe, M. (2008). Online psychoeducational support for infertile women: A randomized controlled trial. Human Reproduction , 23 , 554-566.

Crawford, J.R. & Henry, J. D. (2003). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS):

Normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 42 , 111-131.

Cuijpers, P., Muñoz, R. F., Clarke, G. N., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (2009). Psychoeducational treatment and prevention of depression: The “coping with depression” course thirty years later. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 449-458.

Dalgleish, T., Spinks, H., Yiend, J., & Kuyken, W. (2001). Autobiographical Memory Style in Seasonal Affective Disorder and Its Relationship to Future Symptom Remission.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110 (2), 335-340.

Dane, A. V. & Schneider, B. H. (1998). Program Integrity In Primary And Early Secondary

Prevention: Are Implementation Effects Out Of Control? Clinical Psychology

Review, 18 , 23-45. de Anda, D. (1998). The evaluation of a stress management program for middle school adolescents. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 15 (1), 73-85. de Beurs, E., Van Dyck, R., Marquenie, L. A., Lange, A., & Blonk, R. W. B. (2001) De

DASS: een vragenlijst voor het meten van depressie, angst en stress. [The DASS: a questionnaire for measuring depression, anxiety and stress]. Gedragstherapie, 34,

35–53.

Deckro, G. R., Ballinger, K. M., Hoyt, M., Wilcher, M., Dusek, J., Myers, P.,

... Benson, H. (2002). The evaluation of a mind/body intervention to reduce psychological distress and perceived stress in college students. Journal of

American College Health , 50 , 281-287.

De Lepeleire, J. (2011). Zorggebruik voor psychische stoornissen in België. Reflecties vanuit de huisartsgeneeskunde [Care usage for mental disorders in Belgium.

Reflections from general medical practice]. In R.Bruffaerts, A. Bonnewyn, & K.

Demyttenaere (red), Kan geestelijke gezondheid worden gemeten? Psychische stoornissen bij de Belgische bevolking [Can mental health be measured? Mental

157

References disorders in the Belgian population]. Leuven: Acco.

Des Jarlais, D. C., Lyles, C., Crepaz, N., & the Trend Group (2004). Improving the reporting quality of nonrandomized evaluations of behavioral and public health interventions: The TREND statement. American Journal of Public Health, 94, 361-

366.

Devilly, G. J. (2005). ClinTools Software for Windows (Version 4) [Computer Software].

La Habra, CA: Psytek.

Donker, T., Straten, A. van, Marks, I. M., & Cuijpers, P. (2009). A brief web-based screening questionnaire for common mental disorders: Development and validation . Journal of Medical Internet Research, 11 (3), e19-e35.

Dumas, J. E., Lynch, A. M., Laughlin, J. E., Smith, E. P., & Prinz, R. J. (2001) Promoting intervention fidelity. conceptual issues, methods, and preliminary results from the

EARLYALLIANCE prevention trial. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 20, 38–

47.

Dusenbury, L., Brannigan, R., Falco, M., & Hansen, W. B. (2003) A review of research on fidelity of implementation: implications for drug abuse prevention in school settings. Health Education Research, 18 , 237–256.

Ehrenberg, R. G., Brewer, D. J., Gamoran, A., & Wilms, J. D. (2001). Class size and student achievement. Psychological Science in the Public Interest , 2 (1), 1-30.

Eisses, A., Kluiter, H., Jongenelis, K., Pot, A., Beekman, A. & Ormel, J. (2005). Care staff training in detection of depression in residential homes for the elderly. British

Journal of Psychiatry, 186, 404-409.

Emmen, M. J., Meijer, S. A., & Verhaak, P. F. M. (2008). Positie van de eerstelijnspsycholoog in de geestelijke gezondheidszorg [Position of the primary care psychologist in mental healthcare]. Tijdschrift voor

Gezondheidswetenschappen , 86 (3), 142-149.

Eriksen, H. R., Ihlebaek, C., Mikkelsen, A., Grønningsæter, H., Sandal, G. M., & Ursin, H.

(2002). Improving subjective health at the worksite: A randomized controlled trial of stress management training, physical exercise and an integrated health programme.

Occupational Medicine , 52 , 383-391.

Esch, T., Fricchione, G. L., & Stefano, G. B. (2003). The therapeutic use of the relaxation response in stress-related diseases. Medical Science Monitors , 9 (2), RA23-RA24.

158

References

Evans, J., Williams, J. M. G., O’Loughlin, S., & Howells, K. (1992). Autobiographical memory and problem-solving strategies of parasuicide patients. Psychological

Medicine, 22, 399-405. doi:10.1017/S0033291700030348.

Evans-Lacko, S., London, J., Japhet, S., Rusch, N., Flach, C., Corker, E. et al. (2012). Mass social contact interventions and their effect on mental health related stigma and intended discrimination. Bmc Public Health, 12 .

Everaert, S., Scheerder, G., De Coster, I. & Van Audenhove C. (2007). Getrapte zorg voor personen met depressie in de Centra voor Geestelijke Gezondheidszorg.

[Stepped care for people with depression in the centers for ambulatory mental healthcare] . Leuven: LUCAS, KU Leuven.

Farrell, J. & Goebert, D. (2008). Collaboration between psychiatrists and clergy in recognizing and treating serious mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 59 , 437-440.

Fetterman, D. M. (1996). Empowerment Evaluation: An introduction to Theory and

Practice. In D. Fetterman, S. Kaftarian, & A. Wandersman (Eds).

Empowerment

Evaluation: Knowledge and Tools for Self-assessment and Accountability.

(pp. 3-46).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

First, M. B. (2005). Clinical Utility: A Prerequisite for the Adoption of a Dimensional

Approach in DSM. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114 (4), 560-564.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach.

New York: Psychology Press.

Fischler, G., Kendall, P., & Vye, C. (1982). Qualitative scoring procedure for interpersonal cognitive problem-solving (ICPS) measures.

Minnesota: University of

Minnesota.

Fuller, J., Edwards, J., Procter, N., & Moss, J. (2000). How definition of mental health problems can influence help seeking in rural and remote communities. Aust. J.

Rural Health, 8 , 148-153.

Gibbs, B. R., & Rude, S. S. (2004). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as depression vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 511-526.

Gladstone, T. R. G., & Beardslee, W. R. (2009). The prevention of depression in children and adolescents: A review. La Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie , 54 , 212-221.

Glasgow, R. E., Lichtenstein, E., Marcus, A. C. (2003). Why Don't We See More

Translation of Health Promotion Research to Practice? Rethinking the Efficacy-to-

159

References

Effectiveness Transition. American Journal of Public Health, 93 (8), 1261-1267.

Glasgow, R. E., Marcus, A. C., Bull, S. S., & Wilson, K. M. (2004) Disseminating effective cancer screening interventions. Cancer, 101, 1239–1250.

Goddart, L., Dritschel, B., & Burton, A. (1996). Role of autobiographical memory in social problem solving and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105 (4), 609-616.

Goldberg, D. & Huxley, P. (1992) Common Mental Disorders. A Bio-social Model.

London:Routledge.

Goldney, R. D. & Fisher, L.J. (2008). Have broad-based community and professional education programs influenced mental health literacy and treatment seeking of those with major depression and suicidal ideation? Suicide and Life-Threatening

Behavior, 37 , 308-321.

Goldney, R. D., Fisher, L. J., Dal Grande, E. & Taylor, A. W. (2005). Changes in mental health literacy about depression: South Australia, 1998 to 2004. MJA, 183 , 134-137.

Gray, B. (1989) Collaborating. Finding Common Ground for Multiparty Problems.

Jossey-

Bass Publishers, San Francisco.

Gray (2009). Doing Research in the Real World. 2nd edition.

London: Sage Publications

Ltd.

Gresham, F. M., Gansle, K. A., & Noell, G.H. (1993). Treatment Integrity in Applied

Behavior Analysis with Children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 257-263.

Griffith, J. W., Kleim, B., Sumner, J. A., & Ehlers, A. (2012). The factor structure of the

Autobiographical Memory Test in recent trauma survivors. Psychological

Assessment, 24 , 640-646.

Griffith, J. W., Sumner, J. A., Debeer, E., Raes, F., Hermans, D., Mineka, S., Zinbarg, R. E.,

& Craske, M. G. (2009). An Item Response Theory/Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Autobiographical Memory Test. Memory, 17, 609-623.

Griffiths, K. M., Farrer, L., & Christensen, H. (2010). The efficacy of internet interventions for depression and anxiety disorders: a review of randomised controlled trials. MJA, 192, S4-S11.

Haaga, D. A. F. (2000). Introduction to the special section on stepped care models in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology , 68 , 547-548.

Hamilton, M (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56-62.

160

References

Hampel, P., Meier, M., & Kümmel, U. (2008). School-based stress management training for adolescents: Longitudinal results from an experimental study. Journal of

Youth & Adolescence , 37 , 1009-1024.

Harvey, A. G., Bryant, R. A., & Dang, S. T. (1998). Autobiographical memory in acute stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 500-506.

Hasson, H. (2010). Systematic evaluation of implementation fidelity of complex interventions in health and social care. Implementation Science, 5 .

Henderson, P. & Thomas, D. N. (2013). Skills in Neighbourhood Work.

New York, NY:

Routledge.

Henderson, C. & Thornicroft, G. (2009). Stigma and discrimination in mental illness:

Time to Change. The Lancet, 373(9679) , 1928-1930.

Hermans, D., Vandromme, H., Debeer, E., Raes, F., Demyttenaere, K., Brunfaut, E., &

Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts diagnostic status in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 668-677.

Heron, J. Crane, C. Gunnell, D., Lewis, G., Evans, J., & Williams, J. M. G. (2012). 40,000 memories in young teenagers: Psychometric properties of the Autobiographical

Memory Test in a UK cohort study. Memory, 20 (3), 300-320.

Hickie I. B., Davenport, T. A., Luscombe,G. M., Moore, M., Griffiths, K. M., &

Christensen H. (2010). Practitioner-supported delivery of internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy: evaluation of the feasibility of conducting a cluser randomised trial. MJA, 192, S31-S35.

Higgins, J. P. T., & Green, S. (Eds.). (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions . Chichester, England: John Wiley.

Hipwell, A. E., Reynolds, S., & Pitts Crick, E. (2004). Cognitive vulnerability to postnatal depressive symptomatology. Journal of Repreductive and Infant Psychology, 22 (3),

211-227.

Hirokawa, K., Akihiro Y., & Yo, M. (2002). An examination of the effects of stress management training for Japanese college students of social work. International

Journal of Stress Management , 9 (2), 113-123.

Hodges, B., Inch, C. & Silver, I. (2001). Improving the psychiatric knowledge, skills and attitudes of primary care physicians, 1950-2000: a review. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 158, 1579-1586.

161

References

Hoge, M. A., Morris, J. A., Daniels, A. S., Stuart, G. W., Huey, L. Y., & Adams, N. (2007).

An action plan for behavioral health workforce development.

Washington, DC:

Department of Health and Human Services.

Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sanchez-Meca, J., Marin-Martinez, F., & Botella, J. (2006).

Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychological

Methods , 11 , 193-206.

ISW Limits (2006). Stressbeheersing [Stress Control] . Leuven: ISW Limits.

ISW Limits (2009). Kleur je Leven. Algemene voorstelling [Colour your Life General

Introduction] . Leuven: ISW Limits.

Jacobson, N.S. & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 59, 12-19.

Jane-Llopis, E., Hosman, C., & Saxena, S. (2004). Next generation of preventive interventions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,

43, 5-6.

Johansson, R., Ekbladh, S., Hebert, A., Lindström, M., Möller, S., Petitt, E. ... Andersson,

G. (2012). Psychodynamic Guided Self-Help for Adult Depression through the

Internet: A Randomised Controlled Trial. Plos One, 7 (5), e38021.

Jones, L. V. (2004). Enhancing psychosocial competence among Black women in college. Social Work , 49 , 75-84.

Jones, M. C., & Johnston, D. W. (2000). Evaluating the impact of a worksite stress management programme for distressed student nurses: A randomised controlled trial. Psychology & Health , 15 , 689-706.

Jood, K., Redfors, P., Rosengren, A., Blomstrand, C., & Jern, C. (2009). Self-perceived psychological stress and ischemic stroke: A case-control study. BMC Medicine , 7 , 53.

Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H. & Griffiths, K. M. (2005a). The impact of beyond blue: the

National depression initiative on the Australian public’s recognition of depression and beliefs about treatments. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 39, 248-254.

Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H. & Griffiths, K. M. (2005b). Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for mental disorders. Changes in Australia over 8 years. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40, 764-767.

162

References

Jorm, A. F., Korten A. E., Jacomb, P. A., Christensen, H., Rodgers, B. & Pollitt, P. (1997).

“Mental health literacy”: a survey of the public’s ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. MJA, 166,

182-186.

Kazdin, A. E. & Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting Psychotherapy Research and Practice to

Reduce the Burden of Mental Illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 21-

37.

Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Chatterji, S., Lee, S., Ormel, J. et al. (2009).

The global burden of mental disorders: an update from the WHO World Mental

Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemiol.Psichiatr.Soc., 18, 23-33.

Kessler, R. C., Demler, O., Frank, R. G., Olfson, M., Pincus, H. A., Walter, E. E., Wang, P., et al. (2005). Prevalence and Treatment of Mental Disorders, 1990 to 2003. The New

England Journal of Medicine, 352 (24), 2515-2523.

Kessler, R. C. & Wang, P. S. (2008). The descriptive epidemiology of commonly occurring mental disorders in the United States. Annu.Rev.Public Health, 29, 115-129.

Keyes, C. L. M. (2007). Promoting and Protecting Mental Health as Flourishing. A

Complementary Strategy for Improving National Mental Health. American

Psychologist, 62 (2), 95-108.

King, A. (1990). Enhancing peer interaction and learning in the classroom through reciprocal questioning. American Educational Research Journal , 27 , 664-687.

Kirby, E. D., Williams, V. P., Hocking, M. C., Lane, J. D., & Williams, R. B. (2006).

Psychosocial benefits of three formats of a standardized behavioral stress management program. Psychosomatic Medicine , 68 , 816-823.

Kleim, B. & Ehler, A. (2008). Reduced Autobiographical Memory Specificity Predicts

Depression and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder After Recent Trauma. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76 (2), 231-242.

Klinkman, M.S. (1997). Competing demands in psychosocial care. A model for the identification and treatment of depressive disorders in primary care. Gen Hosp

Psychiatry, 19, 98-111.

Kirkpatrick, D. L. (1975) Evaluation training Programs.

Wisconsin, Madison.

Kiselica, M. S., Baker, S. B., Thomas, R. N., & Reedy, S. (1994). Effects of stress

163

References inoculation training on anxiety, stress, and academic performance among adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology , 41 , 335-342.

Kok, G., Schaalma, H., Ruiter, R. A. C., & Van Empelen, P. (2004) Intervention mapping: a protocol for applying health psychology theory to prevention programmes.

Journal of Health Psychology, 9 , 85–98.

KU Leuven, UGent, VUB, & KHK (2007). Proposal for funding : ”Policy Research Centre for Welfare, Public Health and Family. Leuven: Author. http://steunpuntwvg.be/swvg/_docs/Multiannual%20program.pdf

Lamberts, H., Oskam, S. K., Hoffmans-Okkers, J. M. (1994). Episodegegevens uit het

Transitieproject op diskette. De gebruiksmogelijkheden van TRANS [Episodic data from the Transition project on diskette. Uses for TRANS]. Huisarts en Wetenschap,

37 (4), 421-426.

Lasalvia, A., Zoppei, S., Van Bortel, T., Bonetto, C., Cristofalo, D., Wahlbeck, K., ... &

Thornicroft, G. (2012). Global Patterns of experienced and anticipated discrimination reported by people with major depressive disorder: a cross- sectional survey. The Lancet, 9860, 55-62.

Lauber, C., Nordt, C., & Rössler, W. (2005). Recommendations of mental health professionals and the general population on how to treat mental disorders. Soc

Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 40, 835-843.

Lauber, C., Nordt, C. & Rössler, W. (2006). Attitudes and mental illness: consumers and the general public are on one side of the medal, mental health professionals on the other . Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 114, 145-146.

Laverack, G. & Wallerstein, N. (2001) Measuring community empowerment: a fresh look at organizational domains. Health Promotion International, 16, 179–185.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal and Coping.

New York, NY:

Springer.

Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures and pains of distinct self construals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1122-1134.

Levy, S. R., Baldyga, W., Jurkowski, J.M. (2003). Developing Community Health

Promotion Interventions: Selecting Partners and Fostering Collaboration . Health

Promotion Practice, 4 , 314-322.

164

References

Lin, E. H. B., Simon, G. E., Katzelnick, D. J., & Pearson, S. D. (2001). Does physician education on improve treatment in primary depression management care? Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16, 614-619.

Littrell, J. (1998). Is the reexperience of painful emotion therapeutic? Clinical Psychology

Review, 18 (1), 71-102.

Lovibond, S. H. & Lovibond, P. F. (1995) Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress

Scales.

The Psychology Foundation of Australia, Sydney, Australia.

Lund, C., Breen, A., Flisher, A.J., Kakuma, R., Corrigall, J., Joska, J. A., Swartz, L., & Patel,

V. (2010). Poverty and common mental disorders in low and middle income countries: a systematic review. Social Science & Medicine, 71, 517-528.

Luyten, P. & Blatt, S. J. (2007). Looking Back Towards the Future: Is It Time to Change the DSM Approach to Psychiatric Disorders? The Case of Depression. Psychiatry,

70 (2), 85-99.

Macaulay, A. P., Gronewold, E., Griffin, K. W., Williams, C., & Samoulis, J. (2005)

Evaluation of an Online Implementation and Enrichment Program for Providers of a

Drug Prevention Program for Adolescents.

Paper presented at the American Public

Health Association 133rd Annual Meeting & Exposition, Philadelphia, PA, December

2005.

Mackinger, H. F., Loschin, G. G., & Leibetseder, M. M. (2000). Prediction of Postnatal

Affective Changes by Autobiographical Memories. European Psychologist, 5 (1), 52-

61.

Madsen, S. M., Mirza, M. R., Holm, S., Hilsted, K. L., Kampmann, K., & Riis, P. (2002).

Attitudes towards clinical research amongst participants and nonparticipants.

Journal of Internal Medicine, 251 , 156-168.

Maercker , A. & Zoellner, T. (2004). The Janus Face of Self-Perceived Growth: Towards a

Two-Component Model of Posttraumatic Growth. Psychological Inquiry, 15 (1), 41-

48.

Martin, A., Sanderson, K., Cocker, F., Hons, B.A. (2009). Meta-analysis of the effects of health promotion intervention in the workplace on depression and anxiety symptoms. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment & Health, 35 (1), 7-18.

Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality.

New York, NY: Harper.

Mathers, C. D. & Loncar, D. (2006). Projections of Global Mortality and Burden of

165

References

Disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS MEdicine, 3 (11), e442.

Matud, M. P. (2004). Gender differences in stress and coping styles.

Personality and

Individual Differences , 37 , 1401-1415.

McCrae, N., Murray, J., Banerjee, S., Huxley, P., Bhugra, D., Tylee, A. & MacDonald, A.

(2005). “They’re all depressed, aren’t they?” A qualitative study of social care workers and depression in older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 9, 508-516.

McGuire, W. J. (1985). Attitudes and attitude change. in Lindsay, G. & Aronson, E. (Eds),

The Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 233-235). New York, NY: Random House.

McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., Pitman, R. K. (1995).Autobiographical memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 33 (6), 619-630.

McNally, R. J., Litz, B. T., Prassas, A., Shin, L. M., & Weathers, F. W. (1994). Emotional priming of Autobiographical Memory in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder. Cognition &

Emotion, 8 (4), 351-367.

Meichenbaum, D. (1985) Stress Inoculation Training.

Pergamon, New York.

Mercer, S. L., DeVinney, B. J., Fine, L. J., Green, L. W., & Dougherty (2007). Study Designs for Effectiveness and Translation Research. Indentifying Trade-offs. Am J Prev Med,

33 (2), 139-154.

Merry, S. N. (2007). Prevention and early intervention for depression in young people—

A practical possibility? Current Opinion in Psychiatry , 20 , 325-329.

Merry, S. N., McDowell, H. H., Hetrick, S. E., Bir, J. J., & Muller, N. (2004). Psychological and/or educational interventions for the prevention of depression in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews , 2, CD003380.

Merry, S. N., & Spence, S. H. (2007). Attempting to prevent depression in youth: A systematic review of the evidence. Early Intervention in Psychiatry , 1 , 128-137.

Metaforum (2010). Het toenemend gebruik van psychofarmaca. [The increasing use of psychiatric medication] . Leuven: KU Leuven.

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

28 , 487-495. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6.

Mihalic, S. (2002). Blueprints for Violence Prevention Violence Initiative: Summary of training and implementation (Final Process Evaluation Report) . Boulder, CO:

166

References

University of Colorado at Boulder, Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence.

Mihalopoulos, C. Vos, T., Pirkis? J., & Carter, R. (2012). The Population Cost- effectiveness of Interventions Designed to Prevent Childhood Depression.

Pediatricts, 129 (3), e723-730.

Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 49 , 377-412.

Minkler, M. & Wallerstein, N. (eds) (2003). Community Based Participatory Research in Health.

Jossey-Bass, San Francisco.

Minkler, M., Vásquez, V. B., Warner, J. R., Steussey, H., & Facente, S. (2006) Sowing the seeds for sustainable change: a community-based participatory research partnership for health promotion in Indiana, USA and its aftermath. Health

Promotion International, 21, 293–300.

Mischoulon, D., McColl-Vuolo, R., Howarth, S., Lagomasino, I. T., Alpert, J. E.,

Nierenberg, A. A., ... Fava, M. (2001). Management of major depression in the primary care setting. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 103-107.

Mitchell, A. M., Vaze, A., & Rao, S.(2009). Clinical diagnosis of depression in primary care: a meta-analysis. The Lancet, 374, 609-619.

Moffit, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A. Kokaua. J., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G. & Poulton, R.

(2010). How common are mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence reates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment.

Psychological Medicine, 40 , 899-909.

Moher, D., Hopewell, S., Schulz, K. F., Montori, V., Gøtzsche, P. C., Deveraux, P. J.,...

Altman, D. G. (2010). CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Journal of Clinical

Epidemiology , 63 , e1-e37.

Molden, D. C., Lee, A. Y., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Motivations for promotion and prevention. In J. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.) Handbook of motivation science (pp. 169-

187). New York: Guilford Press.

Morgan, A. & Jorm, A. (2007). Awareness of beyondbleu: the national depression initiative in Australian young people. Australasian Psychiatry, 15 (4), 329-333.

Mojtabai, R. (2007). Americans’ attitudes toward mental health treatment seeking:

167

References

1990-2003. Psychiatric Services, 58, 642–651.

Mulder, N. & Kroon, H. (2009). Assertive Community Treatment: bemoeizorg voor patiënten met complexe problemen [Assertive Community Treatment: assertive outreach for patients with complex problems] . Culemborg: Centraal Boekhuis.

Muñoz, R. F., Cuijpers, P., Smit, F., Barrera, A. Z., & Leykin, Y. (2010). Prevention of

Major Depression. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol., 6, 181-212.

Munz, D. C., Kohler, J. M., & Greenberg, C. I. (2001). Effectiveness of a comprehensive worksite stress management program: Combining organizational and individual interventions. International Journal of Stress Management , 8 (1), 49-62.

Murray C. J. L. & Lopez A. D. (Eds.) (1996). The global burden of disease: A comprehensive assessment of mortality and disability from diseases, injuries and risk factors in 1990 and projected to 2020.

Cambridge (Massachusetts): Harvard

University Press.

Natl. Res. Counc. & Inst. Med. 2009. Preventing Mental, Emotional, and Behavioral

Disorders Among Young People: Progress and Possibilities.

Washington, DC: Natl.

Acad. Press.

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (2010). OECD Health

Ministerial Meeting. Health System Priorities in the Aftermath of the Crisis.

Paris:

OECD.

Neil, A. L., & Christensen, H. (2009). Efficacy and effectiveness of school-based prevention and early intervention programs for anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review ,

29 , 208-215.

Neshat-Doost, H. T., Dalgleish, T., Yule, W., Kalantari, M., Ahmadi, S. J., Dyregrov, A., &

Jobson, L. (2013). Enhancing autobiographical memory specificity through cognitive training: An intervention for depression translated from basic science.

Clinical Psychological Science, 1 (1), 84-92.

Oakley, A., Strange, V., Bonell, C., Allen, C. B., Stephenson, J., & RIPPLE Study Team

(2006) Process evaluation in randomised controlled trials of complex interventions.

BMJ, 332, 413–416.

Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine (2011). The Oxford 2011 Levels of Evidence.

www.cemb.net/index.aspx?0=5513, Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine.

Palinkas, L. A., Aarons, G. A., Horwitz, S., Chamberlain, P., Hurlburt, M., & Landsver, J.

168

References

(2011) Mixed method designs in implementation. Administration and Policy in

Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research, 38, 44–53.

Peeters, F., Wessel, I., Merckelbach, H., & Boon-Vermeeren, M. (2002).

Autobiographical memory specificity and the course of major depressive disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 43 (5), 344-350.

Platt, J. J., & Spivack, G. (1975). Manual for the Means Ends Problem-Solving

procedure (MEPS): A measure of interpersonal problem-solving skill.

Philadelphia:

Department of Mental Health Sciences, Hahnemann Medical College and Hospital.

Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2001). Effective problem solving in suicide attempters depends on specific autobiographical recall. Suicide and Life-

Threatening Behavior, 31 (4), 386-396.

Prince, M., Patel, V., Saxena, S., Maj, M., Maselko, J., Philips, M. R., & Rahman, A.

(2007). No health without mental health. The Lancet, 370 , 859-877.

Prochaska, J. & DiClemente, C. (1985). Common processes of change in smoking, weight control and psychological distress. In S. SHiffman & T. Wills (Eds.). Coping and

Substance Use: A Conceptual Framework (pp. 345-363). New York: Academic

Press.

Pyne, J. M., Smith, J., Fortney, J., Zhang, M., Williams, D. K., Rost, K. (2003). Cost- effectiveness of a primary care intervention for depressed females. Journal of

Affective Disorders, 74, 23-32.

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Beyers, W., Brunfaut, E., & Eelen, P. (2006).

Reduced autobiographical memory specificity and rumination in predicting the course of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 699-704. doi:10.1037/

0021-843X.115.4.699.

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Demyttenaere, K., Sabbe, B., Pieters, G., et al.

(2005). Reduced specificity of autobiographical memories: a mediator between rumination and ineffective social problem-solving in major depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 87 , 331-335.

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., & Eelen, P. (2007). A sentence completion procedure as an alternative to the Autobiographical Memory Test for assessing overgeneral memory in non-clinical populations. Memory, 15 , 495-507

Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Griffith, J. W., & Hermans, D. (2013). A non-randomized

169

References controlled trial investigating Memory Specificity Training (MeST) in depressed inpatients. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Raes, F., Sienaert, P., Demyttenaere, K., Peuskens, J., Williams, M., & Hermans, D.

(2008). Overgeneral memory predicts stability of short-term outcome of ECT for depression. Journal of ECT, 24, 81-83. doi:10.1097/YCT.0b013e31814da995.

Raes, F., Williams, J. M. G., & Hermans, D. (2009). Reducing cognitive vulnerability to depression: a preliminary evaluation of MEmory Specificity Training (MEST) in inpatients with depressive complaints. Journal of Behavior Therapy and

Experimental Psychiatry, 40 , 24-38.

Rabin, B. A., Glasgow, R. E., Kerner, J. F., Klump, M. P., & Brownson, R. C. (2010)

Dissemination and implementation research on community-based cancer prevention: a systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 38, 443–

56.

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401.

Rahe, R. H., Taylor, C. B., Tolles, R. L., Newhall, L. M., Veach, T. L., & Bryson, S. (2002).

A novel stress and coping workplace program reduces illness and healthcare utilization. Psychosomatic Medicine , 64 , 278-286.

Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and

Data Analysis methods (2 nd

. Ed.). London: Sage Publications.

Raue, P. J., Schulberg, H. C., Moonseong, H., Klimstra, S., & Bruce, M. L. (2009). Patients’

Depression Treatment Preferences and Initiation, Adherence, and Outcome: A

Randomized Primary Care Study. Psychiatr Serv, 60 (3), 337-343.

Rawal, A. & Rice, F. (2012). Examining Overgeneral Autobiographical Memory as a Risk

Factor for Adolescent Depression. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 51 (5), 518-527.

Regional Office for Europe of the World Health Organization (2010). Policies and

Practices for mentalhealth in Europe. Meeting the challenges.

Kopenhagen: World

Health Organization.

Reynders, A., Scheerder, G., Molenberghs, G., & Van Audenhove, C. (2011) Suïcide in

Vlaanderen en Nederland. Een verklaring vanuit sociaal cognitieve factoren en

170

References hulpzoekend gedrag [Suicide in Flanders and The Netherlands. An explanation using social cognitive factors and help seeking behaviour] . Leuven: LUCAS, KU Leuven.

Richardson, K. M. & Rothstein, H. R. (2008). Effects of Occupational Stress Management

Intervention Programs: A Meta-Analysis . Journal of Occupational Health Psychology,

13 (1), 69-93.

Riper, H., Andersson, G., Christensen, H., Cuijpers, P., Lange, A., & Eysenbach, G. (2010).

Theme Issue on E-mental Health: A Growing Field in Internet Research. J Med

Internet Res, 12 (5), e74.

Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of Innovation.

New York, NY: The Free Press.

Rose, S. & van der Laan, M.J. (2008). Why Match? Investigating Matched Case-Control

Study Designs with Causal Effect Estimation. Int J Biostat., 5 (1), 1.

Rosenberg, M. S. (2005). The file-drawer problem revisited: A general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution , 59 , 464-468.

Rosengren, A., Hawken, S., Ôunpuu, K. S., Sliwa, K., Zubaid, M., Almahmeed, W., ...

Yusuf, S. (2004). Association of psychosocial risk factors with risk of acute myocardial infarction in 11119 cases and 13648 controls from 52 countries (the

INTERHEART study): A case-control study. Lancet , 364 , 953-962.

Rothman, K. & Greenland, S. (1998). Modern Epidemiology.

Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott,

Williams and Wilkins.

Rowe, M. M. (2000). Skills training in the Long-Term Management of Stress and

Occupational Burnout. Current Psychology, 19 (3), 215-228.

Rowe, M. M. (2006). Four-year Longitudinal Study of Behavioral Changes in Coping With

Stress. Am J Health Behav, 30 (6), 602-612.

Ruggiero, K. J., Resnick, H.S., Acierno, R., Carpenter, M. J., Kilpatrick, D. G., Coffey, S. F.

... Galea, S. (2006). Internet-Based Intervention for Mental Health and Substance

Use Problems in Disaster-Affected Populations: A Pilot Feasibility Study. Behavior

Therapy, 37, 190-205.

Rychetnik, L., Frommer, M., Hawe, P., & Shiell, A. (2002) Criteria for evaluating evidence on public health interventions. Journal of Epidemiology & Community

Health, 56 , 119–127.

Rychetnik, L., Hawe, P., Waters, E., Barratt, A., & Frommer, M. (2004) A glossary for

171

References evidence based public health. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 58,

538–545.

SARWGG (2012). Visienota. Integrale zorg en ondersteuning in Vlaanderen [Vision

Statement. Integral care and support in Flanders] . Brussels: Author.

Saunders, J.B., Aasland, O.G., Babor, T.F., de la Puente, J.R., & Grant, M. (1993).

Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Screening Test (AUDIT). WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption II. Addiction, 88, 791-804.

Scheerder, G. De Coster, I. & Van Audenhove, C. (2008). Pharmacists' Role in

Depression Care: A Survey of Attitudes, Current Practices, and Barriers.

Psychiatric Services, 59 , 1155-1161.

Scheerder, G., De Coster, I., & Van Audenhove, C. (2009). Community pharmacists' attitude toward depression: A pilot study. Research in Social and Administrative

Pharmacy, 5, 242-252.

Schönefeld, S., Ehlers, A., Böllinghaus, I., & Rief, W. (2007). Overgeneral memory and suppression of trauma memories in post-traumatic stress disorder. Memory, 15 (3),

339-352.

Sefton, T. (2000). Getting Less for More: Economic Evaluation in the Social Welfare

Field.

CASEpaper, 44. Centre for Analysis of Social Exclusion, London School

Economics and Political Science, London, UK.

Shadish, W., Cook, T., & Campbell, D. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs.

Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.

Shen, J., Yang, H., Cao, H., & Warfield, C. (2008) The fidelity-adaptation relationship in non- evidence-based programs and its implication for program evaluation.

Evaluation, 14, 467–481.

Shimazu, A., Kawakami, N., Irimajiri, H., Sakamoto, M., & Amano, S. (2005). Effects of web-based psychoeducation on self-efficacy, problem solving behavior, stress responses and job satisfaction among workers: A controlled clinical trial. Journal of

Occupational Health , 47 , 405-413.

Shimazu, A., Okada, Y., Sakamoto, M., & Miura, M. (2003). Effects of stress management program for teachers in Japan: A pilot study. Journal of Occupational

Health , 45 , 202-208.

172

References

Smith, A. P., Wadsworth, E. J. K., Shaw, C., Stansfeld, S., Bhui, K., & Dhillon, K. (2005).

Ethnicity, work characteristics, stress and health (Research Report No. 308).

Sudbury, England: HSE Books.

Spinhoven, P., Bockting, C. L. H., Schene, A. H., Koeter, M. W. J., Wekking, E. M., &

Williams, J. M. G. (2006). Autobiographical Memory in the Euthymic Phase of

Recurrent Depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115 (3), 590-600.

Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W. (1999). Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA, 282, 1737-1744.

Steidtmann, D., Manber, R., Arnow, B. A., Klein, D. N., Markowitz, J. C., Rothbaum, B.O.,

... Kocsis, J. H. (2012). Patient treatment preference as a predictor of response and attrition in treatment for chronic depression. Depression and Anxiety 00, 1-10.

Stein, D. J., Phillips, K. A., Bolton, D., Fulford, K. W. M., Sadler, J. Z., & Kendler, K. S.

(2010). What is a mental/psychiatric disorder? From DSM-IV to DSM-V.

Psychological Medicine, 40 , 1759-1765.

Steinhardt, M., & Dolbier, C. (2008). Evaluation of a resilience intervention to enhance coping strategies and protective factors and decrease symptomatology. Journal of

American College Health , 56 , 445-453.

Stice, E., Shaw, H., Bohon, C. Marti, C. N. & Rohde, P. (2009). A Meta-Analytic

Review of Depression Prevention Programs for Children and Adolescents: Factors

That Predict Magnitude of Intervention Effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77 (3), 486-503.

Stice, E., Shaw, H., & Marti, C. N. (2007). A meta-analytic review of eating disorder prevention programs: Encouraging findings. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology , 3 ,

233-257.

Stricker, G. (2000). The relationship between efficacy and effectiveness. Prevention &

Treatment, 3 (1).

Sumner, J. A., Griffith, J. W., & Mineka, S. (2010). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as a predictor of the course of depression: a meta-analysis. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 48, 614-625. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.03.013.

Sumner, J. A., Griffith, J. W., Mineka, S., Rekart, K. N., Zinbarg, R. E., & Craske, M. G.

(2011). Overgeneral autobiographical memory and chronic interpersonal stress as predictors of the course of depression in adolescents. Cognition & Emotion, 25 (1),

173

References

183-192.

Tiemeier, H., de Vries, W.J., van het Loo, M., Kahan, J.P., Klazinga, N., Grol, R., & Rigter,

H. (2002). Guideline adherence rates and interprofessional variation in a vignette study of depression. Qual Saf Healthcare, 11, 214-218.

Toroyan, T., Oakley, A., Laing, G., Roberts, L., Mugford, M., & Turner, J. (2004) The impact of day care on socially disadvantaged families: an example of the use of process evaluation within a randomized controlled trial. Child: Care Health &

Development, 30 , 691–698.

Uher, R. (2012). The Conceptual Evolution of DSM-5. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 42 (1), 143-145.

Van Daele, T., Van Audenhove, C., Hermans, D., Van den Bergh, O., Van den Broucke, S.

(2013). Empowerment implementation: Enhancing fidelity and adaptation in a psychoeducational intervention. Health Promotion International, advance online publication.

Van Daele, T., Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2012). Stress prevention through psychoeducation: a meta-analytic review. Health Education &

Behavior, 39(4), 474-485.

Van den Broucke, S. (2001). Preventie in de geestelijke gezondheidszorg. Strategieën en methoden. [Prevention in mental healthcare. Strategies and methods] . Welzijnsgids

- Gezondheidszorg, Geestelijke gezondheidszorg, 41, Bro. 1-Bro. 20. van der Klink, J. J. L., Blonk, R. W. B., Schene, A. H., & van Dijk, F. J. H. (2001). The benefits of interventions for workrelated stress. American Journal of Public Health ,

91 , 270-276. van der Lem, R., van der Wee, N. J. A., van Veen, T., & Zitman, F. G. (2012). Efficacy versus Effectiveness: A Direct Comparison of the Outcome of Treatment for Mild to

Moderate Depression in Randomized Controlled Trials and Daily Practice.

Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 81, 226-234. van der Velde, J., Williamson, D. L., & Ogilvie, L. D. (2009) Participatory action research: practical strategies for actively engaging and maintaining participation in immigrant and refugee communities. Qualitative Health Research, 19, 1293–1302. van Minnen, A., Wessel, I., & Verhaak, C. (2005). The relationship between

174

References autobiographical memory specificity and depressed mood following a stressful life event: A prospective study. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 405-415. van Os, T. W., van den Brink, R. H., Jenner, J. A., van der Meer, K., Tiemens, B. G., &

Ormel, J. (2002). Effects on depression pharmacotherapy of a Dutch general practitioner training program. Journal of Affective Disorders, 71, 105-111. van Praag, H. M. (2004). Can stress cause depression? Progress in Neuro-

Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry , 28 , 891-907.

Van Regenmortel, T. (2009). Empowerment als uitdagend kader voor sociale inclusie en moderne zorg [Empowerment as a challenging framework for social inclusion and modern care]. Journal of Social Intervention: Theory and Practice, 18( 4), 22-42. van Rijsoort, S. N., Vervaeke, G., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1997). De Penn State Worry

Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: Eerste resultaten in een normale Nederlandstalige populatie. [The Penn State Wory Questionnaire and the

Worry Domains Questionnaire: First results in a normal Dutch population].

Gedragstherapie, 30, 121-128.

van Straten, A., Tiemens, B., Hakkaart, L., Nolen, W. A., & Donker, M. C. H. (2006).

Stepped care vs. matched care for mood and anxiety disorders: a randomized trial in routine practice. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 113, 468-476. van ‘t Veer-Tazelaar, P. J., van Marwijk, H. W. J., van Oppen, P., van Hout, H. P. J., van der Horst, H. E., Cuijpers, P., ... Beekman, A.T. (2009). Stepped-Care Prevention of

Anxiety and Depression in Late Life A Randomized Controlled Trial. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 66, 297-304. van Vreeswijk, M. F., de Wilde, E. J. (2004). Autobiographical memory specificity, psychopathology, depressed mood and the use of the Autobiographical Memory

Test: a meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 731-743.

Vermette, H., Pinals, D. & Appelbaum, P. (2005). Mental health training for law enforcement professionals . Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and Law,

33, 42-46.

Viera, J. A., & Garrett, J. M. (2005). Understanding Interobserver Agreement: The Kappa

Statistic. Family Medicine, 37 (5), 360-363.

Vitale, M. R. & Romance, N. R. (2005). A Multi-phase Model for Scaling Up a Research-

Validated Instructional Intervention: Implications for Leadership of Systemic

175

References

Educational Reform. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American

Education Research Association, Montreal, Canada, April 2005.

Wallerstein, N. B. & Duran, B. (2006). Using Community-Based Participatory Research to

Address Health Disparities. Health Promotion Practice, 7, 312-323.

Walter, H., Gouze, K. & Lim, K. (2006). Teachers’ beliefs about mental health needs of inner city elementary schools. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 61-68.

Wandersman, A., Snell-Johns, J., Lentz, B. E., Fetterman, D. M., Keener, D. C., Livet, M. et al. (2005) The principals of empowerment evaluation. In Fetterman, D. and

Wandersman, A. (eds), Empowerment Evaluation: Principles in Practice.

The

Guilford Press, New York, pp. 27–41.

Wang, J. (2005). Work stress as a risk factor for major depressive episode(s).

Psychological Medicine , 35 , 865-871.

Watson, A., Corrigan, P., & Ottati, V. (2004). Police officers’ attitudes toward and decisions about persons with mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 55, 49-53.

Weissberg, R. P. (1990) Fidelity and adaptation: combining the best of both perspectives. In Tolan, P., Keys, C., Chertok, F. and Jason, L. (eds), Researching

Community Psychology: Issues of Theory and Methods (pp. 186-189) .

American

Psychological Association, Washington, DC.

Werkgroep Eerstelijnsgezondheidszorg (2010). Eindrapport conferentie eerstelijnsgezondheidszorg [Final report primary care convention].

Brussels: Author.

White, J. (2000). Treating anxiety and stress: a group psychoeducational approach using brief CBT. Chichester: Wiley.

White, J., & Keenan, M. (1990). ‘Stress Control’: a pilot study of large group therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26, 133-141.

White, J., Keenan, M., & Brooks, N. (1992). Stress Control’: a controlled comparative investigation of large group therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: process of change. Clinical Psychology an Psychotherapy, 20, 97-114.

Wilhelm, S., McNally, R. J. , Baer, L., & Florin, I. (1997). Autobiographical memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 21-31.

Williams, J. M. G. (2008). Overgeneral autobiographical memory predicts diagnostic status in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 668e677.

176

References

Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., et al.

(2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological

Bulletin, 133, 122-148. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122.

Williams, J. M. G., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95 , 144-149.

Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., & Healy, H. (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory & Cognition,

24 (1), 116-125.

WHO International Consortiumin Psychiatric Epidemiology (2000). Cross-national comparisons of the prevalences and correlates of mental disorders . Bulletin of the

World Health Organization, 78, 413-426.

Williams, J. W., Noël, P. H., Cordes, J.A., Ramirez, G., & Pignone, M. (2002). Is this patient clinically depressed? Journal of the American Medical Association, 287 (9),

1160-1170.

World Health Organization (1986). Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion.

Geneva:

World Health Organization.

World Health Organization (2001). Strengthening mental health promotion.

Fact Sheet

No. 220, Geneva.

World Health Organization (2005). Mental health: facing the challenges, building solutions. Report from the WHO European Ministerial Conference.

Copenhagen,

Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe.

World Health Organization (2008). ICD-10: International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (10th Rev. ed.). New York, NY: World Health

Organization.

World Health Organization (2010). Vaccine-preventable diseases: monitoring system.

2010 global summary. New York, NY: World Health Organization.

World Health Organization (2011). Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2010 . Geneva: World Health Organization.

World Health Organization (2012). Action plan for implementation of the European

Strategy for the Prevention and Control of Noncommunicable Diseases 2012-2016.

Copenhagen, Denmark: WHO Regional Office for Europe.

Xu, W., Zhao, Y., Guo, L., Yanhong, G., & Gao, W. (2009). Job Stress and Coronary Heart

177

References

Disease: A Case-control Study using a Chinese Population. Journal of Occupational

Health, 51, 107-113.

Zimmerman, M. A. (1995) Psychological empowerment: issues and illustrations.

American Journal of Community Psychology, 23, 581–599.

Zimmerman, M. A. (2000) Empowerment theory: psychological, organizational, and community levels of analysis. In Rappaport, J. and Seldman, E. (eds), Handbook of

Community Psychology.

Plenum, New York.

Zimmerman, M. A. & Rappaport, J. (1988) Citizen participation, perceived control and psychological empowerment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 725–

750

178

Dankwoord

Ziezo, je bent er geraakt: het einde van dit doctoraat, meerbepaald het dankwoord! Een woordje van dank op het einde plaatsen leek me iets logischer dan in het begin, want alleen al het feit dat je tot hier bent geraakt verdient een welgemeende dankjewel. Tenzij je al mijn harde werk gewoon genegeerd hebt en uit nieuwsgierigheid meteen tot hier hebt gebladerd. Ik neem het je niet kwalijk, maar ik kan je wel aanraden om toch eens terug te kijken naar de voorgaande hoofdstukken: interessant onderzoek, relevante resultaten en toch ook wel een groot deel van mijn tijdsinvestering de afgelopen vier jaar.

Maar, genoeg gezeverd en nu even serieus. Ik mag dan wel degene zijn die

‘het proefschrift heeft aangeboden’, het is zeker niet enkel en alleen mijn verdienste. Doorheen de jaren heb ik met heel veel mensen contact gehad die elk op hun eigen manier hebben bijgedragen aan dit doctoraat. Ze allemaal bedanken is een huzarenstukje en je loopt natuurlijk altijd het risico om iemand te vergeten, maar ik zou toch graag een poging wagen.

Ten eerste natuurlijk mijn promotor, Omer. Vier jaar geleden introduceerde je me bij het steunpunt, gaf je me de mogelijkheid om te doctoreren en nam je daarbij ook de rol op van promotor. We waren beiden misschien enigszins naïef om na het relatieve succes van mijn masterproef de stresscursus nu eens te gaan implementeren en valideren in de wijde wereld (of althans toch in

Vlaanderen). Het bleek uiteindelijk een hele beproeving, met onverwachte ontwikkelingen en veel bijsturen. Ik kon er echter altijd op rekenen dat je me met de nodige rust en kalmte hierdoor zou leiden. Bij het schrijven van verschillende artikels en het doctoraat stond je verder ook altijd klaar met nuttige en constructieve feedback. Al zal ik vooral de bemerking “ opvallend goed geschreven qua taal, beter dan gewoonlijk :) ”, niet snel vergeten. Voor dit

179

Dankwoord alles wil ik je heel oprecht van harte bedanken!

Ik kon verder ook nog rekenen op twee copromotoren die me elk vanuit hun eigen expertise met raad en daad hebben bijgestaan. Dirk, bijna zes jaar geleden werd mijn eerste keuze voor een masterproefonderwerp door puur toeval, tot twee maal toe uitgeloot. Al na onze eerste contacten bleek dat ik niet alleen het geluk had om een masterproef te mogen schrijven op een onderwerp dat me echt aansprak, maar dat het ook wel heel goed klikte met de promotor daarvan. Doorheen de jaren heb ik altijd op je kunnen rekenen voor snelle commentaren, je oprechte interesse en een grenzeloos en aanstekelijk enthousiasme. Ook aan jou daarom: bedankt! Chantal, LUCAS was in eerste instantie onbekend terrein: niet alleen was het een eindje verwijderd van het vertrouwde PSI, ook beleidsrelevant onderzoek was mij nog vrij onbekend. Doorheen de jaren heb je me stap voor stap geïntroduceerd in deze wereld en was je er steeds om me op de do’s en don’ts te wijzen. Jouw ervaring, bijsturingen en tips hebben een essentiële rol gespeeld in het tot stand komen van dit doctoraat.

Naast met mijn promotorenteam was er ook veel ondersteuning uit diverse andere hoeken. Ten eerste zijn er de co-auteurs van onze publicaties en de leden van mijn begeleidingscommissie. Bedankt Filip, om altijd vol enthousiasme en ongelofelijk snel te reageren op mijn vragen en om geregeld te polsen hoe het met de vooruitgang van mijn doctoraat stond. Jamie, al beperkt onze samenwerking zich slechts tot het afgelopen jaar, ik heb in die periode je analytische skills en je input bij het schrijven heel erg geapprecieerd.

Deb, bedankt voor de verschillende keren dat we hebben samengezeten rond het opzetten van de interventies, het waren telkens heel verrijkende ervaringen. Een dankjewel is zeker ook gepast voor de je hulp die je hebt geboden in het bijsturen van het onderzoek, toen dat wat moeilijker liep.

Stephan, ik kwam destijds naar jou met een idee om een artikel over onze implementatieperikelen te schrijven. Ik ben er nog altijd van onder de indruk

180

Dankwoord hoe we onder jouw impuls erin geslaagd zijn om op basis daarvan een volledig theoretisch framework te construeren. Hoewel onze contacten meestal via mail verliepen, vond ik de bezoekjes aan Louvain-La-Neuve ook heel verrijkend.

Daarnaast kon ik ook beroep doen op de nationale en internationale expertise van mijn juryleden. Jim, I would like to thank you sincerely. You might not be fully aware of it, but you have been a major influence on me for the past six years. Ever since I heard about the Stress Control course and your vision on mental health(care) I was really fascinated by its potential and I wanted to get involved. In the end, both my master’s thesis and my doctoral dissertation are largely inspired by your work. Furthermore, I really enjoyed our contacts so far and I hope we can keep in touch! Heleen, ook jou wil ik bedanken om opnieuw een tekst van mij door te nemen en hier kritisch over te reflecteren. Daarnaast zal ik zeker ook onthouden hoe je me geïntroduceerd hebt in de wondere wereld van e-mental health en de groep Europese onderzoekers die hiermee bezig is. Professor Claes wil ik ten slotte bedanken om ook de rol als jurylid van mijn doctoraat te willen opnemen.

De volgende groep mensen die ik wil bedanken, is een meer omvangrijke: mijn collega’s. Het Steunpunt Welzijn, Volksgezondheid en Gezin was officieel mijn werkgever. Valérie, als coördinator stond je heel dicht bij het dagelijks reilen en zeilen van mijn onderzoek. Ik wil je bedanken voor het luisterend oor, de talloze keren dat ik je bureau ben binnengestormd met de een of andere bezorgdheid. Je zorgde voor het nodige overzicht, maar ook voor een ontspannende babbel bij een kop koffie of tijdens een lunch in de Timory en in diverse andere etablissementen. LUCAS was de onderzoeksgroep waar ik meestal mijn dagen spendeerde. Jullie zijn ondertussen met een hele bende en de laatste tijd zijn er ook heel wat nieuwe gezichten, maar ik wil jullie allemaal bedanken om af en toe te polsen hoe het onderzoek of het doctoraat gaat en de vele ontspannende momenten. Sophie, Evelien, Johanna, Anja, Iris, Veerle,

Jasper, Eva, Kirsten, Koen, Jeroen, Evy, Inge, Jessie, Aline, Nele, Stephanie, Joke,

181

Dankwoord

Liza, Dirk, Marieke, Monia, Kevin, Manuela en Lut: allemaal hartelijk bedankt!

Ook bedankt aan de collega’s die er voor een groot deel van mijn traject bij waren, maar ondertussen elders vertoeven: Alexandre, Ann, Else, Kristien,

Maartje, Nele en Bert. Ten slotte, een specifieke dankjewel aan mijn huidig bureaugenoot Annelies voor haar steun. Aan jou wil ik ook mijn oprechte excuses aanbieden voor mijn vele ostentatieve gezucht de afgelopen maanden tijdens het finaliseren van dit doctoraat. Zeker ook bedankt aan mijn oud bureaugenoot Bram, voor de jaren die we samen al doorgesparteld hebben en waarbij ping pong tijdens de middagpauze werd afgewisseld met hitte- en koudegolven, met het maken van constructies met duplo en met het bezoek van wespen, een ‘inbreker’ en een verdwaalde koolmees. Natuurlijk was er ook nog de Onderzoekseenheid voor Gezondheidspsychologie. De meeste onder jullie kennen me daar vooral als de persoon die pas opduikt tijdens retraites, recepties of etentjes. Verder was de jaarlijkse International Dinner Party telkens een schot in de roos, waarvoor dank, Angela! Jullie hielpen me met de nodige ontspanning, maar het was voor mij ook iedere keer weer een verrijkende confrontatie met basisonderzoek en het reilen en zeilen van de academische wereld. Ook jullie allemaal bedankt: Kai, Katleen, Erik, Elena,

Marleen, Rena, Hassan, Ali, Joanna, Meike, Sibylle, Martien, Elke, Stéphanie,

Marta, Katja en Ruth. Een extra dankjewel aan Thomas, Steven en Ann: al van bij het hele begin, op de eerste retraite in de Ardennen, hebben jullie visie en de ervaringen die jullie deelden mij een beeld helpen vormen van wat de mogelijkheden zijn als onderzoeker en hoe je die zo optimaal mogelijk kan benutten.

Omdat mijn onderzoek in nauwe samenwerking werd opgezet met de praktijk, kan ik ook de verschillende lokale partners niet dankbaar genoeg zijn.

Velen van jullie hebben je vrijwillig geëngageerd en bijkomend ingezet voor het project. Sommige onder jullie gingen zelfs zo ver om deelnemers aan te manen de vragenlijsten in te dienen ‘want het is toch wel voor Tom zijn doctoraat,

182

Dankwoord hoor!’ . Een welgemeende dankjewel daarom aan CGG Vagga, Provincie

Antwerpen, het toenmalig LOGO Antwerpen, Stad Antwerpen, CGG LITP,

OCMW Genk, de gemeentebesturen van As, Zutendaal, Opglabbeek en

Diepenbeek, de werkgroep Genk Preventief Gezond, CGG Largo, het toenmalig

LOGO OIVD, de Provincie West-Vlaanderen, het Cultuurcentrum Ieper en alle andere personen en organisaties die zich op de een of andere manier voor het

STAP-project hebben ingezet.

Onderzoek is natuurlijk niet alles. Daarom ook bedankt aan iedereen uit mijn omgeving en de mensen bij het VVKP en het LAPP, in het bijzonder Jana, voor de nodige afwisseling en ontspanning. Verder wil ik ook Sigrid bedanken voor de wekelijkse broodjes in de Kruidtuin, een ideaal moment om even te ventileren over de problemen met publicaties, maar ook om stil te staan bij hoe leuk doctoreren soms wel niet kon zijn. Daarnaast zijn er natuurlijk ook nog

Steven, Pieter en Ben. De middagslunches aan ‘de tempel’, onze mannenweekends, de after work drinks en zo veel meer, het zijn telkens hoogtepunten.

Daarnaast is er natuurlijk ook mijn familie. Een speciaal woordje van dank voor tante Karien en nonkel Martin die al sinds mijn eerste jaren hier aan de universiteit er een gewoonte van hebben gemaakt om af en toe uit Zuienkerke langs te komen, vooral in de examens, om me even uit de studiesfeer te halen en voor wat broodnodige ontspanning te bezorgen. Ook bedankt aan mijn broer Dries, voor de avonden hier in Leuven, maar ook voor de gezellige tijd die we samen geregeld hebben in Varsenare. Mama en papa ik heb zoveel aan jullie te danken: doorheen de jaren hebben jullie me steeds alle kansen gegeven en hebben jullie me steeds op alle mogelijke manieren ondersteund.

Zonder jullie was dit nooit gelukt. Het mag dan misschien wel ‘mijn’ doctoraat zijn, maar jullie verdienste hierin is niet te onderschatten.

Als allerlaatste wil ik ook nog Ann-Sofie bedanken. Mijn verloofde op het moment dat ik dit schrijf, mijn vrouw wanneer ik dit doctoraat verdedig. Na al

183

Dankwoord die tijd zijn we zo op elkaar ingespeeld dat ik mijn leven zonder jou eigenlijk niet kan voorstellen. Je hebt samen met mij het hele traject meegemaakt, zowel de frequente ups als de occasionele downs. Je bent er altijd voor mij geweest. Samen met jou tijd doorbrengen is pure ontspanning en brengt me altijd tot rust. Laat ons eerlijk zijn: had iedereen iemand zoals jou, kreeg ik

‘mijn’ stresscursussen zelfs aan de straatstenen niet kwijt. Ik hart je hard!

184

Curriculum vitae

Tom Van Daele obtained a Master of Science in Clinical and Health Psychology, with a specialization in children and adolescents at the KU Leuven. During that time he took an internship in a local organization for health consultation

(LOGO) and completed a master thesis on the effectiveness of a stress control intervention. These experiences sparked his interest for research in primary mental healthcare and in the field of (mental) health prevention and promotion. More information can be found on www.vandaeletom.be.

Publication List

Articles in internationally reviewed scientific journals

Van Daele, T.

, Van Audenhove, C., Hermans, D., Van den Bergh, O., & Van

den Broucke, S. (2012). Empowerment implementation: Enhancing fidelity and adaptation in a psychoeducational intervention. Health

Promotion International.

Advance online publication.

doi: 10.1093/heapro/das070

Van Daele, T.

, Van den Bergh, O., Van Audenhove, C., Raes, F., & Hermans,

D. (2013). Reduced Memory Specificity Predicts the Acquisition of

Problem Solving Skills in Psychoeducation . Journal of Behavior Therapy

and Experimental Psychiatry, 44, 135-140.

doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.12.005

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O.

(2012). Stress reduction through psychoeducation: a meta-analytic

review. Health Education & Behavior, 39, 474-485.

doi: 10.1177/1090198111419202

185

Curriculum vitae

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van

den Bergh, O. (2010). Preventie van stress, angst en depressie door

psycho-educatie: een overzicht van interventies. Psychologie &

Gezondheid, 38, 224-235. doi: 10.1007/s12483-010-0390-5

Articles in other scientific journals

Van Daele, T.

(2012). Computergestuurde zelfhulp een meerwaarde voor

hulpzoekende adolescenten met depressieve symptomen? [Does

computerized self help over advantages for help seeking adolescents with

depressive symptoms?]. Minerva: Tijdschrift voor Evidence-based Medicine,

11 (10), 121-122.

Meeting abstracts, presented at international conferences and symposia, published or not published in proceedings or journals

Van Daele, T.

, Van Audenhove, C., Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., Van den

Bergh, O. (2013). Detecting depression in chronic ill patients by home nurses:

Effects of a minimal intervention.

Paper presented at the 27th Annual

Conference of the European Health Psychology Society. Bordeaux, France,

16-20 July 2013.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van

den Bergh, O. (2012). Effectiveness of a Six Session Stress Reduction

Program for Groups.

Poster presented at the 120th Annual Convention of

the American Psychological Association. Orlando, FL, USA, 2-5 August 2012.

Van Daele, T.

, Van Audenhove, C., Hermans, D., Van den Bergh, O., & Van den

Broucke, S. (2012). Towards a Participatory Approach To Implement a

Psychoeducational Intervention.

Poster presented at the 120th Annual

Convention of the American Psychological Association. Orlando, FL, USA, 2-5

August 2012.

186

Curriculum vitae

Van Daele, T.

, Vansteenwegen, D., Luts, A., Hermans, D., & Van den Bergh, O.

(2012). Online intervention for depressive symptoms in chronic illness. Work

in progress. Poster presented at the First Meeting of the European Society

for Research on Internet Interventions. Lüneburg, Germany, 30-31 May

2012.

Van Daele, T.

, Van den Bergh, O., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2012). Happiness

coach: Preliminary results of an online intervention to increase mental

fitness.

Poster presented at the First Meeting of the European Society for

Research on Internet Interventions. Lüneburg, Germany, 30-31 May 2012.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2011).

Stress prevention through psychoeducation: a meta-analytic review.

Paper

presented at the 25th European Health Psychology Conference. Crete,

Greece, 20-24 September 2011.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2011).

Impact research of a psychoeducational course for stress management.

Poster presented at the 25th European Health Psychology Conference.

Crete, Greece, 20-24 September 2011.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., Van den Bergh, O. (2011).

Stress prevention through psychoeducation: A meta-analytic review.

International Conference of the European Network of Mental Health

Services. Ulm, Germany, June 2011.

Hermans, D., Vuerstaeck, S., Van Daele, T.

, & Raes, F. (2008).

Autobiographical memory specifity and the effects of a psycho-

educational programme for stress and anxiety.

Paper presented at the 6th

Special Interest Meeting on Autobiographical Memory and

Psychopathology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, January 2008.

Hermans, D., Ruys, K., Vuerstaeck, S., Van Daele, T.

, & Raes, F. (2007).

Autobiographical memory specificity and the effects of a psycho-educational

programme for stress and anxiety.

Annual Convention of the Association for

187

Curriculum vitae

Advancement of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies (AABCT). Philadelphia,

PA, USA, November 2007.

Meeting abstracts, presented at local conferences and symposia, published or not published in proceedings or journals

Van Daele, T.

, Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van

den Bergh, O. (2012). Effectiviteit van een stressreductieprogramma in

groepsverband.

Paper presented at the Zesde Vlaams Geestelijk

Gezondheidscongres ‘Macht en kracht. Zorgrelaties in verandering’.

Antwerpen, Belgium, 18-19 September 2012.

Van Daele, T.

, Van den Bergh, O., Hermans, D., & Van Audenhove, C. (2011).

Stress in Vlaanderen: perspectieven van psycho-educatie als

eerstelijnsinterventie.

Paper presented at the Steunpunt WVG studiedag

‘Wel en Wee in Vlaanderen’. Leuven, Belgium, 28 October 2011.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2011).

What is the participant profile in psychoeducational group interventions for

stress?

. Poster presented at the Steunpunt WVG studiedag ‘Wel en Wee in

Vlaanderen’. Leuven, Belgium, 28 October 2011.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2011).

STAP, Stress AanPakken.

Poster presented at the Steunpunt WVG studiedag

‘Wel en Wee in Vlaanderen’. Leuven, Belgium, 28 October 2011.

Van Daele, T.

, Van den Bergh, O., Hermans, D., & Van Audenhove, C. (2010).

Proces-evaluatie van een proefimplementatie. STAP als praktijkvoorbeeld.

Paper presented at the Steunpunt WVG Studiedag ‘In-zicht’. Leuven,

Belgium, 2 December 2010.

Van Daele, T.

, Hermans, D., Van Audenhove, C., & Van den Bergh, O. (2010).

STAP, STress AanPakken. Een systematische meta-analytische review van

preventieve psycho-educatie bij stress.

Poster presented at the Steunpunt

WVG Studiedag ‘In-zicht’. Leuven, Belgium, 2 December 2010.

188

Download