EJP Commentary Dunlop Personality: Limit One per Customer David G. Serfass, Nicolas A. Brown, Ashley Bell Jones, Daniel E. Lopez-Chavez, & Ryne A. Sherman Florida Atlantic University Authors’ Note Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to Ryne A. Sherman, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, 777 Glades Road, Boca Raton, FL 33431. Email: rsherm13@fau.edu. EJP Commentary Dunlop Abstract Contextualized theories of personality claim that people possess different personalities depending on their current context (i.e., situation). These approaches confuse behavior, which is context specific, with personality, which is not. Dunlop’s (this issue) extension of contextualized personality to include goals and life narratives similarly obfuscates this issue. While it may be of interest to examine momentary behavior in relation to context specific goals and narratives, there is only one personality per person. The scientific and practical utility of personality is its ability to predict behavior across a variety of situations. EJP Commentary Dunlop Psychologists have made numerous attempts to contextualize personality. As noted by Dunlop (this issue), this often involves having a person complete a trait inventory for each of several roles (e.g., Donahue & Harary, 1998; Roberts & Donahue, 1994; Sheldon & Elliot, 2000; Wood & Roberts, 2006). Other attempts to contextualize personality suggest that personality consists of some unknown (or unlimited) number of interactions between the person and the situations he or she encounters (e.g., Mischel, 2009; Shoda, Mischel, & Wright, 1994; Wright & Mischel, 1987). An even more recently developed view suggests that personality is the entire density distribution of one’s behavior across the many situations one encounters (Fleeson, 2001; Fleeson & Jayawickreme, in press). Unfortunately, all contextualized conceptions of personality obfuscate the issue of personality because they equate personality with behavior. That is, someone who behaves differently in different roles is said to have a different personality for each role. Someone who behaves differently in different situations is said to have different personalities for each situation (e.g., Mischel & Shoda, 1995), or, in an alternative view, the entire set of behaviors across all situations is said to be explained by a single trait (Fleeson & Jayawickreme, in press). There are two proximal and one distal problem with these interpretations of personality. The first proximal problem is that these conceptualizations are empirically indefensible. The data overwhelmingly show that personality trait measures robustly predict typical behavior across a variety of roles (Donahue & Harary, 1998; Roberts & Donahue, 1994; Wood & Roberts, 2006) and situations (Ching et al., 2014; Church et al., 2013; Fleeson, 2007; Fleeson & Gallagher, 2009; Judge, Simon, Hurst, & Kelley, 2014; Sherman, Rauthmann, Brown, Serfass, & Jones, in press). Second, by inextricably linking personality and behavior components of the “personality EJP Commentary Dunlop triad” (Funder, 2006), contextualized conceptions of personality either (a) make personality disappear altogether by calling it momentary behavior or (b) make behavior disappear altogether by suggesting that people have a personality for every role or situation. However, the root of the problem with contextualized personality theories lies not with contextualization per se. Indeed, and as was noted many years ago (Allport, 1937; Murray, 1938), behavior is clearly contextualized (Ching et al., 2014; Church et al., 2013; Donahue & Harary, 1998; Fleeson, 2001, 2007; Fournier, Moskowitz, & Zuroff, 2008, 2009; Funder & Colvin, 1991; Mischel & Shoda, 1995; Roberts & Donahue, 1994; Sherman et al., in press; Wood & Roberts, 2006; Zayas & Shoda, 2009). Instead the distal problem with contextualized personality is that it confuses personality on the outside with personality on the inside (Hogan, 2007; Hogan, Hogan, & Roberts, 1996). Personality on the outside is the sum of your behavior across all of your situations (Buss & Craik, 1983; Hogan, 1983; Wood, Gardner, & Harms, 2015). It is the ‘you’ that everyone else knows (i.e., reputation; Hogan, 1983) and that you can learn about yourself by watching others’ reactions to you (Bollich, Johannet, Vazire, 2011; Cooley, 1902; Hogan, 1983; Hogan & Blickle, 2013). Personality on the inside (i.e., motives, values, preferences), is the ‘you’ that you know (i.e., identity) and take with you wherever you go. Personality on the outside (i.e., behavior) is caused by the joint influence of personality on the inside and situations (Fleeson, 2007; Sherman et al., in press). Such an understanding makes two points. First, it provides a parsimonious explanation for behavioral consistency: you always bring your own motives, values, preferences with you wherever you go. Second, your behavior at a specific point in time is partially dependent upon your current situation. EJP Commentary Dunlop Recent work has identified a sensible set of situation characteristics that are useful for predicting momentary behavior (Rauthmann et al., 2014; Sherman, Nave, & Funder, 2010; Sherman et al., in press; Wagerman & Funder, 2009). Moreover, situation characteristics subsume the litany of haphazardly used terms to describe contexts including rules, norms, expectations, roles, agendas, and affordances. For example, the three roles used by Donahue and Harary (1998) were work, romantic partner, and friend. Roles are situation classes and generally less useful than characteristics for behavioral prediction (Rauthmann, Sherman, & Funder, in press). Fortunately, situation characteristics from the DIAMONDS (Rauthmann et al., 2014) framework can be mapped onto these roles. Work roles are characterized by high Duty (i.e., a job needs to be done), romantic roles are characterized by high Mating (i.e., a potential romantic partner is present), and friendship roles are characterized by Sociality (i.e., close personal relationships are present or have the potential to develop). Dunlop’s (this issue) take on contextualized personality incorporates McAdams’ (1995) three levels of personality – traits, goals, and life narratives – into the contextualization, distinguishing it from other contextualized views that focus exclusively on traits. Unfortunately, this broader conceptualization of personality is still a contextualized one and, as such, suffers from the same problems that plague other contextualized views. Situation characteristics indicate goal affordances (Neuberg, Kenrick, & Schaller, 2010; Yang, Read, & Miller, 2006), so of course goals are contextualized. Likewise, situation characteristics undoubtedly shape momentary self-conceptions (i.e., narratives). However, the fact that people have different goals or narratives in different situations is not evidence of contextualized motives or identities any more than variability in behavior is evidence of contextualized personality. Indeed, people have EJP Commentary Dunlop consistent goals and narratives across contexts (Dunlop, Walker, & Weins, 2013), pointing to individual differences in underlying motivational systems and identities. In sum, while it may be of interest to examine momentary behavior in relation to context specific goals and narratives, the scientific and practical utility of personality is its ability to predict behavior across a variety of situations. As such, contextualized approaches to personality – the notion that we have different personalities for each situation – effectively make personality meaningless. EJP Commentary Dunlop References Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A Psychological Interpretation. New York: Holt. Buss, D. M., & Craik, K. H. (1983). The act frequency approach to personality. Psychological Review, 90(2), 105. Bollich, K. L., Johannet, P. M., & Vazire, S. (2011). In search of our true selves: feedback as a path to self-knowledge. Frontiers in Psychology. 2:312. Ching, C. M., Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., Rayes, J. A. S., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., Takaoka, S., …Ortiz, F. A. (2014). The manifestation of traits in every behavior and affect: A fiveculture study. Journal of Research in Personality, 48, 1-16. Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., Ching, C. M., Zhang, H., Shen, J., Arias, R. M., … Alvarez, J. M. (2013). Within-individual variability in self-concepts and personality states: Applying density distribution and situation-behavior approaches across cultures. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 922-935. Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons. Donahue, E. M., & Harary, K. (1998). The patterned inconsistency of traits: Mapping the differential effects of social roles on self-perceptions of the Big Five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 610-619. Dunlop, W. L. (this issue). Contextualized personality, beyond personality traits. European Journal of Personality. EJP Commentary Dunlop Dunlop, W. L., Walker, L. J., & Wiens, T. K. (2013). What do we know when we know a person across contexts? The differing relationship between self-concept differentiation and adjustment at the three levels of personality. Journal of Personality, 81, 376-389. Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 1011-1027. Fleeson, W. (2007). Situation based contingencies underlying trait content manifestation in behavior. Journal of Personality, 75(4), 825-862. Fleeson, W., & Gallagher, P. (2009). The implications of Big Five standing for the distribution of trait manifestation of behavior: Fifteen experience-sampling studies and a metaanalysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1097-1114. Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (in press). Whole trait theory. Journal of Research in Personality. Fournier, M. A., Moskowitz, D. S., & Zuroff, D. C. (2008). Integrating dispositions, signatures, and the interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 531545. Fournier, M. A., Moskowitz, D. S., & Zuroff, D. C. (2009). The interpersonal signature. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(2), 155-162. Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 197-221. EJP Commentary Dunlop Funder, D. C. (2006). Towards a resolution of the personality triad: Persons, situations, and behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 21-34. Funder, D. C., & Colvin, C. R. (1991). Explorations in behavioral consistency: properties of persons, situations, and behaviors. Journal of personality and social psychology, 60(5), 773. Hogan, R. (1983). A socioanalytic theory of personality. In M. M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1982 (Vol. 29, pp. 55-89). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Hogan, R., Hogan, J., & Roberts, B. W. (1996). Personality measurement and employment decisions: questions and answers. American psychologist, 51(5), 469. Hogan, R., & Blickle, G. (2013). Socioanalytic theory. In N. D. Christianesen & R. P. Tett (Eds.), Handbook of Personality at Work (pp. 53-70). New York: Routledge. Hogan, R. (2007). Personality and the Fate of Organizations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Judge, T. A., Simon, L. S., Hurst, C., & Kelley, K. (2014). What I experienced yesterday is who I am today: Relationship of work motivations and behaviors to within-individual variation in the five-factor model of personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99(2), 199-221. McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? Journal of Personality, 63, 365-396. EJP Commentary Dunlop Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246-268. Mischel, W. (2009). From personality and assessment (1968) to personality science (2009). Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 282-290. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in Personality: A Clinical and Experimental Study of Fifty Men of College Age. New York: Oxford University Press. Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2010). Evolutionary social psychology. In S. T. Fiske, D. Gilbert, and G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 761-796). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Rauthmann, J. F., Sherman, R. A., & Funder, D. C. (in press). Principles of situation research: Towards a better understanding of psychological situations. European Journal of Personality. Rauthmann, J. F., Gallardo-Pujol, D., Guillaume, E. M., Todd, E., Nave, C. S., Sherman, R. A., Ziegler, M., Jones, A. B., & Funder, D. C. (2014). The situational eight DIAMONDS: A taxonomy of major dimensions of situation characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 677-718. Roberts, B. W., & Donahue, E. M. (1994). One personality, multiple selves: Integrating personality and social roles. Journal of Personality, 62(2), 199-218. EJP Commentary Dunlop Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Personal goals in social roles: Divergences and convergences across roles and levels of analysis. Journal of Personality, 68(1), 51-84. Sherman, R. A., Nave, C. S., & Funder, D. C. (2010). Situational similarity and personality predict behavioral consistency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(2), 330-343. Sherman, R. A., Rauthmann, J. F., Brown, N. A., Serfass, D. G., & Jones, A. B. (in press). The independent effects of personality and situations on real-time expressions of behavior and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Wright, J. C. (1994). Intra-individual stability in the organization and patterning of behavior: Incorporating psychological situations into the idiographic analysis of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 674-687. Wagerman, S. A., & Funder, D. C. (2008). Situations. In P. J. Corr and G. Matthews (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Personality (pp. 27-42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wright, J. C., & Mischel, W. (1987). A conditional approach to dispositional constructs: the local predictability of social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(6), 1159. Wood, D., Gardner, M. H., & Harms, P. D. (2015). How functionalist and process approaches to behavior can explain trait covariation. Psychological Review, 122(1), 84-111. EJP Commentary Dunlop Wood, D., & Roberts, B. W. (2006). Cross-sectional and longitudinal tets of the personality and role identity structural model. Journal of Personality, 74(3), 779-809. Yang, Y., Read, S. J., & Miller, L. (2006). A taxonomy of situations from Chinese idioms: Goal processes as a central organization principle. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 750778. Zayas, V., & Shoda, Y. (2009). Three decades after the personality paradox: Understanding situations. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 280-281.