Foreign Policy Attitudes As Networks Joshua D. Kertzer* and Kathleen E. Powers† Last revised: August 8, 2014 ABSTRACT: What’s systemic about foreign policy belief systems? We introduce political scientists to a new — networked — paradigm for political attitudes, and develop a new experimental design that disentangles the directional nature of attitude constraint while leveraging tools from network analysis. We find that established models of foreign policy attitudes make unrealistically strict assumptions about the directionality and uniformity of attitude structure. Specific policy attitudes like the Iraq war play more central roles than our existing theories give them credit for, and the topology of attitude networks varies substantially with individual characteristics like political sophistication. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS: This research was supported by the Behavioral Decision Making initiative at Ohio State, and benefited from presentations at Ohio State, the 2013 NYU Experiments Conference, and the 2013 MPSA and APSA meetings. A special thanks to Robin Baidya, Jan BoxSteffensmeier, Greg Caldeira, Chris Gelpi, Brian Greenhill, Matt Hitt, George Ibrahim, Luke Keele, Samara Klar, Vlad Kogan, Neil Malhotra, Kathleen McGraw, Joanne Miller, William Minozzi, Irfan Nooruddin, Dave Peterson, Brian Rathbun, Jason Reifler, Jonathan Renshon, Molly Roberts, Chris Skovron, Ben Valentino, and Ben Wagner for their helpful assistance and feedback on previous versions of this project. Authors are listed in alphabetical order. * Assistant Professor of Government, Harvard University. 1737 Cambridge St, Cambridge, MA 02138. Email: jkertzer@gov.harvard.edu. Web: http:/people.fas.harvard.edu/~jkertzer/. † PhD Candidate, Department of Political Science, The Ohio State University. 2140 Derby Hall, Columbus, OH 43210. Email: powers.276@osu.edu. Web: www.kepowers.com. Introduction How do foreign policy attitudes work together in a system? Ever since Converse (1964) first asked whether the American public was “innocent of ideology,” public opinion scholars have searched for signs of constraint in the public’s beliefs, in both domestic (Peffley and Hurwitz, 1985; Feldman, 2003) and foreign policy (Wittkopf, 1986; Hurwitz and Peffley, 1987) domains. By pointing to structural patterns that characterize how attitudes and beliefs relate to one another as part of a broader system — what psychologists call interattitudinal structure (Judd and Krosnick, 1989; Lavine, Thomsen, and Gonzales, 1997; Dinauer and Fink, 2005) — political scientists have demonstrated that attitudes are indeed better organized “than a bowl of cornflakes” (McGuire, 1989, 50), and that the public perhaps deserves more credit than originally thought. Against the Almond-Lippmann consensus, the past thirty years have produced a plethora of models examining the many different ways in which the public’s foreign policy attitudes are connected to one another (e.g. Holsti, 1979; Rathbun, 2007; Liberman, 2013a). These investigations provide considerable insight into the various component relationships of Americans’ foreign policy belief systems — demonstrating piece by piece how certain attitudes predict one another. Past work highlights individual relationships between attitudes that are theoretically and logically linked, yet they do so at the expense of researching the holistic properties that make belief systems systemic in the first place. We seek to build upon these foundations, while developing a theoretical framework for understanding systems of foreign policy attitudes at a higher level of analysis. In this paper, we introduce political scientists to a new — networked — paradigm for political attitudes and a method for investigating them. We begin by reviewing the foreign policy attitude literature, highlighting the structural assumptions and implicit reductionism that underlie existing work. Next, we develop a novel experimental design — a directed multigraph experiment — that dis- 1 entangles the directional nature of attitude constraint while leveraging tools from network analysis to study belief systems systemically. These tools have been used elsewhere to yield insights about interpersonal (Sokhey and Djupe, 2011) and international (Hafner-Burton, Kahler, and Montgomery, 2009) networks, but as far as we are aware, have not been applied to the study of interattitudinal structure. As a “plausibility probe” for our theoretical framework, we present our experimental results, suggesting that studying foreign policy attitudes as networks leads to three important findings. First, we find evidence that belief systems are often driven from the bottom-up rather than the top-down: specific policy attitudes play more of a role, and core values less of one, than existing work would lead us to expect. Second, although IR scholars often model the public as a monolithic entity, we find that the topology of attitude networks varies dramatically across individuals. Third, we see that political knowledge and interest play major roles in shaping the structure of foreign policy attitude networks: knowledge is critical in shaping the degree to which attitudes are structured, while levels of interest in foreign policy determine whether more specific or abstract attitudes are the most central elements of belief systems. Finally, we conclude by reviewing implications for IR scholars, political psychologists, and public opinion scholars more broadly. Three things we know about foreign policy attitudes Ever since Converse (1964) claimed that many Americans were innocent of ideology, political scientists have searched for signs of “constraint”, or functional interdependence, among political attitudes.1 Although these questions implicate political attitudes more generally, we focus here specifically on foreign policy attitudes, both for reasons of tractability, and because they were traditionally 1 While political scientists invoke “constraint” in a variety of ways, sometimes referring to response (in)stability over time (Zaller and Feldman, 1992), and other times to simple intercorrelations between issue attitudes (Nie, Verba, and Petrocik, 1979), we specifically use constraint here to describe “functional interdependence” among attitudes (Converse, 1964, 3). We follow Eagly and Chaiken (2007, 583) in defining attitudes as “an individual’s propensity to evaluate a particular entity with some degree of favorability or unfavorability.” 2 believed to be far less structured than their domestic counterparts (Holsti, 2004). The result is a rich and robust research program, which has taught us at least three things about how foreign policy attitudes are structured. First, foreign policy attitudes “go together”: evaluations of one foreign policy issue are often related to attitudes towards other foreign policy issues. We know from Wittkopf’s (1990, 33) early factor analyses that the same Americans who, for example, favored arms control agreements with the Soviet Union tended to also support cultural exchanges with them. Similarly, those who prioritize human rights support a stronger United Nations (Rathbun, 2007), and citizens heavily concerned about defending US national security during the Cold War were likely to call for the containment of communism (Chittick, Billingsley, and Travis, 1995). These examples demonstrate that foreign policy attitudes are not entirely unorganized; instead, evidence suggests that preferences cluster in predictable ways. Holding one type of foreign policy attitude makes people more likely to hold another, related, position. Second, specific foreign policy attitudes depend not only upon each other, but also on more general dispositions and values: they have abstract antecedents. In some cases scholars arrive at the antecedents inductively by estimating an exploratory factor analysis and naming the ensuing dimensions — as in Wittkopf’s (1990) cooperative and militant internationalism. Others specify a particular abstract attitude and use regression models to demonstrate that the variable of interest predicts foreign policy attitudes. Moral values, for example, are related to foreign policy stances: individuals who value authority are more likely to favor the war in Iraq, while those who value equality support cooperation with the United Nations (Kertzer et al., 2014). Drawing from an array of potentially influential dispositions, others show that beliefs about retributive justice (Liberman, 2013b) and human nature (Brewer and Steenbergen, 2002), as well as commitments to hierarchy or community (Rathbun, 2007) predict a host of foreign policy attitudes. Hurwitz and Peffley (1987) employ structural equation modeling to go further, theoretically mapping a series of relationships 3 from the most general core values to foreign policy “postures” and specific policy attitudes (defense spending, nuclear disarmament, etc.). Third, we know that these connections — both between policy attitudes and to more abstract dispositions and values — are not coincidental. They are believed to tell us something about how people form and update their political attitudes. Models of social cognition suggest not only that how we respond to new information in the world depends heavily on information already in our heads, but also that we form associational links between two ideas when we consider them simultaneously (Judd and Krosnick, 1989; Lodge and Taber, 2013). In other words, attitudes are neither formed nor updated in a vacuum. We expect that individuals rely on existing ideas to shape their attitudes toward global politics, and that related attitudes will interact with one another. Hurwitz and Peffley (1987) make this argument explicitly. According to their model, people already carry ideas about whether or not warfare is moral, which informs their level of militarism. When they determine their position on a specific policy item, they are doing so with those beliefs in the background shaping and informing their expressed opinion. A key implication of previous work, then, is that attitudes are systemic: the various component parts form a whole. We know for example that Wittkopf’s (1990) cooperative internationalism is associated with a desire to strengthen the United Nations (Holsti and Rosenau, 1990), and that a moral commitment to avoid harm predicts cooperative internationalism (Kertzer et al., 2014). We can thus think of the three attitudes as embedded within a broader system — a network in which attitudes have various, multidirectional connections to one another. Attitudes as networks Previous work thus suggests a level of complexity in the structure of foreign policy attitudes that should encourage us to think about attitudes at a higher level of analysis. While past work tells us much about specific relationships between particular attitudes, we know little about the system itself. 4 We suggest that one way to study belief systems as systems is to explicitly conceptualize attitudes as networks and leverage tools from network analysis. This enables us to make claims about systemlevel dynamics, such as how densely connected foreign policy belief systems are, whether abstract attitudes shape more specific positions in the top-down process described by Hurwitz and Peffley’s (1987) structural equation model, and which types of attitudes are most likely to spark change in other attitudes situated throughout the network. In this respect, although our methods differ, our interests here are similar to those of operational code scholars, who study leaders’ belief systems in a fashion that values the whole as much as its parts (Holsti, 1970; Schafer and Walker, 2006; Renshon, 2009). A leader’s operational code — their “philosophical and instrumental beliefs about the nature and use of power in the international system” — is composed of 10 distinct components (Renshon, 2009, 650). While knowing a leader’s stance on one component is instructive, it does not provide sufficient insight into their decisionmaking process and offers limited scholarly value on its own. Upon understanding how each of the 10 components combine and interact as part of a system, though, scholars can explain the leader’s approach to foreign policy events. Individual attitudes and bivariate relationships between them are useful objects of study, too, but we argue that as in operational code analysis, their dynamic interactions within the larger network of attitudes enable more powerful predictions. Public opinion scholars have done much to connect the foreign policy public opinion literature with psychological research on social cognition, which explores how social information is stored in memory (Fiske and Taylor, 1984, for a review in political science, see McGraw, 2000). We build on this work here, arguing that one underappreciated implication of social cognition research is that attitudes are best conceptualized as networks. Inspired by early investigations into the organization of human memory (Collins and Quillian, 1969; Loftus and Collins, 1975), psychologists have long understood attitudes as “networks of associated cognitions” (Monroe and Read, 2008, 734), whether through associative (Bower, 1981; Anderson, 1983) or connectionist (Feldman and Ballard, 1982; 5 Smith, 2009) models. Network models of attitudes envision attitude objects (Democrats, gun control, the war in Afghanistan) to be stored as a system of nodes connected to one another through associational links, the precise structure of which has implications for how attitudes are accessed and formed (Lodge and McGraw, 1995; Lavine, Thomsen, and Gonzales, 1997). Just as models of semantic memory demonstrate that links form between concepts brought to mind simultaneously (such as pets and dogs), network models of attitudes posit that links emerge or are strengthened when multiple attitudes are thought about once (Loftus and Collins, 1975). Indeed, assuming that attitudes are linked to one another necessarily means that “the representational structure becomes a network” (Judd and Krosnick, 1989, 109). Like Sinclair (2012, 14) argues in reference to the influence of social networks on political behavior, little work in political science has explicitly modeled attitudes as networks, but it has been nevertheless suggestive of the paradigm. Whenever we talk about “spreading activation” in the context of priming effects (Valentino, 1999), the role of memory-based processing in theories of survey responses (Zaller, 1992), or schemas in political belief systems (Conover and Feldman, 1984), we are implicitly studying the consequences of networked attitude structure. Despite invoking networked metaphors in our work, we have not fully embraced the implications of treating attitudes as networks, both for our theories of how attitudes interact within a system and the methods we use to test them. Treating attitudes as networks enables us to move beyond investigations of component parts to assess the system as whole, without relying on restrictive assumptions about the nature of the structure. Thus, rather than asking whether one attitude shapes another, or whether specific policy positions go together, we can ask whether attitudes at different level of abstraction — values, orientations, or policy positions2 — stimulate more change in the system as a whole, whether the system is tightly organized (implying close associations among the elements) or relatively sparse (implying that people do not readily connect attitudes in the pro2 We draw here from the language of Hurwitz and Peffley 1987, who specify three levels of abstraction in their hierarchical model of foreign policy attitude structure. 6 posed network, even those otherwise found to be correlated), and explore, rather than assume, the directionality of attitude constraint. Most research testing the impact of one or more attitudes (the IV(s)) on another attitude (the DV) assumes that attitudes are shaped from the top-down, such that core values like authority or ethnocentrism predominantly shape attitudes towards the Iraq War via militant internationalism or directly, but not the other way around. Yet we know that even a leader’s operational code, rooted in her deep philosophical and instrumental beliefs about the world, is subject to change with shifting world events: John Foster Dulles’ code changed in response to WWII (Holsti, 1970), as did Lyndon B. Johnson’s following Operation Rolling Thunder in Vietnam (Walker and Schafer, 2000). In the domain of public opinion, the link between values and partisanship changes with one’s social network (Lupton, Singh, and Thornton, 2014). Research from cognitive and social psychologists also argues that the formation and direction of links between attitudes is not necessarily organized by abstract schemas like ideology, but may instead be based on alternative considerations including other more concrete positions (Lavine, Thomsen, and Gonzales, 1997). Thus, both political scientists and psychologists raise the possibility of bottom-up processes of attitude constraint — a notion that we can test directly within the network, where directed ties (which flow unidirectionally from one attitude element to another) indicate whether the stimulation of one ‘type’ of attitude element leads to change in another. If the stimulation of core values leads to widespread changes in the system of attitudes, top-down processes are at work. If it is instead the case that an individual’s attitude toward the Iraq War can stimulate change, we learn that a network of attitudes as a whole can change in response to shifting world events. Similarly, while most factor analytic models treat militant and cooperative internationalism as independent dimensions that do not interact directly with the policy positions they organize (Wittkopf, 1986), studying the system allows us to adjudicate this claim. We can determine whether an individual’s position on one dimension stimulates changes in the other or on attitudes throughout 7 the system; if so, they are more central sources of constraint than current research suggests. Previous work also suggests an important degree of uniformity in belief systems across members of the public. While individual attitudes vary, the manner in which they are connected—the underlying structure—remains the same (e.g. Wittkopf, 1990; Rathbun, 2007). In Hurwitz and Peffley’s (1987) model, for example, individuals can be more or less militaristic, but it is always the case that militarism shapes attitudes towards defense spending. In contrast, we argue that properties of the system of attitudes will vary across subsets of the population. In a representational network in which objects exist cognitively as nodes, links are formed and strengthened over time as individuals “[come] to believe that one object implies, favors, contradicts, or opposes the other object” (Judd and Krosnick, 1989, 109), or generally through a mental process in which two or more elements are simultaneously represented. Individuals must have formed an association between two attitude objects in order for one to influence the other in the system (Lavine, Thomsen, and Gonzales, 1997). Given the diversity of experiences and encounters with political information among the public, the elements that are central to an attitude network and the direction of constraint between nodes should vary with individual-level characteristics. We predict that political sophistication will shape systemic properties of attitude networks. With tools from network analysis we can assess whether how densely foreign policy attitudes are connected in individuals with high and low levels of political sophistication. Much of the public opinion literature has sought to show that foreign policy elites and the mass public structure their foreign policy views in similar ways (e.g. Holsti and Rosenau, 1988; Chittick, Billingsley, and Travis, 1995, but see Oldendick and Bardes, 1981), but our networked paradigm implies otherwise: individuals with relatively high levels of political knowledge will more readily connect abstract and specific attitudes (Zaller, 1992). Further, expanding upon Judd and Krosnick’s (1989) finding that political attitudes are more consistent among people interested in politics, we expect that those who are most interested in foreign policy will exhibit higher levels of interdependence and connectedness in their 8 network of foreign policy attitudes. To summarize, thinking about attitudes as networks suggests a new understanding of foreign policy attitude structure that differs from previous work in three substantive ways. First, we have shifted the conversation away from isolated relationships toward a holistic discussion of how the system of attitudes operates. Second, while public opinion scholars have largely sought to exonerate the public by showing that constraint flows from the top-down (from abstract values to concrete policy positions), we expect that it can also flow from the bottom-up: if attitudes are networks of elements connected to one another in various ways, then attitudes at lower levels of abstraction (e.g. specific policy positions) may stimulate more change in the system than we often assume. Finally, we may be overestimating the uniformity of attitude structure: if the particular pattern of connections varies with individual experiences, then the topology of attitude networks should vary with individual characteristics. Methods and materials Observational data, network analysis, and directionality Thinking about attitudes as networks also raises two important methodological implications, the first highlighting the importance of causal inference, and the second the benefits of incorporating network analysis tools. First, if a single attitude can impact multiple others throughout the system simultaneously, we need to employ research designs that give us leverage on this directional question: which attitudes stimulate change, and which are stimulated? Although our existing models have made considerable contributions towards understanding how people think about world politics, political scientists have traditionally approached the structure of foreign policy attitudes as a measurement problem to be resolved by a structural equation modeling approach, in which the structure of the system is largely a product of prior theoretical assumptions, rather than some- 9 thing to be tested empirically.3 Because structural equation modeling on static observational data is poorly suited to testing causal claims (Imai et al., 2011), this literature thus tends to adopt the directional language of causal inference without employing any of the methodological techniques conducive to carrying it out.4 One potential solution is to forgo claims of directionality altogether, but whenever we estimate a regression model where we treat one attitude as a dependent variable, and another as an exogenous independent variable, we are inherently making a directional claim about how constraint operates. Another is to follow the advice of Judd et al. (1991), who call for an exploration of the “dynamic implications” of attitude structure (p. 193) and Peffley and Hurwitz (1993), who close their panel study by noting that they remain “unable to say much about the exact reasoning processes that give rise to top-down... patterns of attitude constraint, a task better left to future, experimental studies” (p. 83). We opt for the latter, employing an experimental design that lets us tackle this directional question head-on. Second, if attitudes truly are networks, they should be studied as such. Although network analysis is increasingly popular in both political science and psychology, to our knowledge no one has yet applied these tools to the study of political attitudes. Given that network analysis is used to study everything from connections between organizations to ties between neurons (Butts, 2009), there is nothing to preclude its use here, particularly when our theories are already suggestive of a network structure – and influential works in public opinion have even gone so far as to invoke network properties like degree centrality as ways of thinking about the structure of political belief systems (Luskin, 1987, 859). We therefore develop an experiment that we analyze using methods from network analysis, allowing us to derive attitude structure from the patterns of relationships that emerge in the data rather than simply imposing them ex-ante. As we note above, our intent here is to suggest a different paradigm for the study of belief systems, which requires far more than a single experiment 3 For this reason, there has been a spirited debate in the public opinion literature about the specific criteria used to establish the topology of foreign policy beliefs (e.g. Kegley 1986; Chittick, Billingsley, and Travis 1995). 4 The notable exceptions include uses of panel data (e.g. Peffley and Hurwitz 1993; Murray 1992; Goren 2005), but these studies stand out in the foreign policy public opinion literature precisely because their dynamic setup is so rare. 10 to test (Lakatos, 1970); in this sense, the empirical tests we outline below should be understood as a “plausibility probe” showing what sorts of insights political scientists gain by studying attitudes at a higher level of analysis. Study sample We conducted an online experiment in the winter of 2013 to investigate the systemic properties of dynamic attitude networks. The sample consisted of 1,307 participants recruited using Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk.5 Participants, 59.6% of whom identified as male, and 78.4% as white/Caucasian, ranged in age from 18-75 (median: 28). Samples from Mechanical Turk are increasingly widely used in the social sciences (e.g. Arceneaux, 2012; Grimmer, Messing, and Westwood, 2012; Huber, Hill, and Lenz, 2012) because of the extent to which they can replicate classic experiments on more diverse samples than traditionally employed in political psychological research (Buhrmester, Kwang, and Gosling, 2011; Berinsky, Huber, and Lenz, 2012). This sample is especially valuable for our purposes both because the length of our experimental instrumentation and complexity of our experimental design would render the study infeasible while embedded in a national survey, but also because of the nature of the theories being tested. First, since most studies presume that attitudes are structured similarly across individuals, we should not require a nationally representative sample to find evidence in favor of their tenets; Druckman and Kam’s (2011, 46) simulations make this point clear. Second, since conceptualizing attitudes as network assumes that attitude structure varies considerably based on individual characteristics, we do not expect there to be a single model of attitude structure that applies to the population as a whole. 5 Following the advice of Berinsky, Huber, and Lenz (2012), participants were paid $1.00 in exchange for their participation. In order to test our hypotheses in an appropriate sample of American adults, we required participants to be at least 18 years of age, US citizens, and have completed at least 50 human intelligence tasks with a 94% approval rate on previous work. 11 Measures, design, and instrumentation As Sinclair (2012, 27-28) notes, there are at least two ways to experimentally study network structure. One is to directly manipulate the network by altering its structure; the other is to “stimulate some part of the network directly and then observe to what extent these manipulations spill over into other parts of the network.” We adopt this latter strategy, employing an experimental design where we stimulate one foreign policy attitude and trace the network structure by measuring how these effects spill over onto other attitudes. Network models of memory in psychology are often tested using simulation methods, which allow for networks with a relatively large number of nodes (Monroe and Read, 2008). However, in order to maintain the tractability and statistical power of the experimental design, the foreign policy attitude network we test here is far smaller, consisting of 11 nodes at three levels of abstraction: four core values (authority/respect, harm/care, ingroup/loyalty, and fairness/reciprocity), three foreign policy orientations (militant internationalism, cooperative internationalism, and isolationism), and four specific policy attitudes (towards the war in Iraq, the NATO-led intervention in Libya, the potential for intervention in Syria, and renewing the Kyoto protocol). For the sake of feasibility, the elements of the network tested here thus represent a theoretically-motivated sample of the potentially massive number of relevant nodes — an often necessary step in the analysis of large network populations. The four core values of authority/respect, harm/care, ingroup/loyalty, and fairness/reciprocity were chosen for three reasons. First, they are central in the contemporary psychological literature on moral values (Haidt and Graham, 2007). Whereas fairness/reciprocity and harm/care — reflecting concerns about equality and helping others in need, respectively — are “individualizing foundations” important for both liberals and conservatives, authority/respect and ingroup/loyalty – the former emphasizing tradition and deference to authority figures and the latter focusing on remaining loyal to one’s fellow group members — are “binding foundations” fundamental only to 12 political conservatives (Graham, Haidt, and Nosek, 2009). Including both types ensures variation in what values individuals are likely to rely on. Second, two of the foundations (authority/respect and fairness/reciprocity) relate closely to what Rathbun (2007) terms hierarchy and community, the values used in his model of the foundation of foreign policy views. Third, recent survey research corroborates our expectations that these moral foundations are elements of foreign policy attitude networks, demonstrating that each is associated with both militant and cooperative internationalism, and also that they predict stances on specific policy issues (Kertzer et al., 2014). Militant internationalism (MI) and cooperative internationalism (CI) — referring to an individual’s support for the idea that the US military should play an active role in international affairs, or that the US should work with other countries to solve global problems, respectively — are selected as the two general foreign policy postures or orientations primarily because of the popularity of Wittkopf’s (1990) model, the independent structure of which is typically inferred from factor analysis rather than experimentally investigated. The third orientation, isolationism, is included in order to assess general preferences for disengagement independent of MI and CI (Holsti, 1979). Finally, we include four specific policy attitudes: positions on the Iraq War and the intervention in Libya, as well as attitudes about potential U.S. military involvement in Syria and the U.S. role in establishing a new treaty to replace the Kyoto protocol. These policy items are selected because they vary in two key respects. First, attitudes toward the Iraq war are likely to be widely connected due to their long-term salience, allowing us to determine the extent to which individuals extrapolate from one battleground to another when forming their views. Second, they represent a range of policies that are positively associated with MI (Iraq, Syria), CI (Kyoto), or both (Libya), allowing us to test whether stimulating one follows the logical patterns of constraint proposed in previous research (Kertzer et al., 2014). Following terminology from graph theory, we test networks of foreign policy attitudes with what we call a directed multigraph experiment, because it allows us to simultaneously adjudicate the 13 direction of constraint between multiple attitude nodes as well as make claims about system-level properties such as the density of the network.6 In the experiment, we employ a persuasive-message approach (Dinauer and Fink, 2005), in which, after answering a series of demographic questions, participants are exposed to one of eleven stimulus paragraphs designed to stimulate their position on the node being manipulated, or a control message unrelated to foreign policy attitudes. This approach has the effect of activating the targeted attitude object in the participant’s mind, together with both the positive associations offered in the message and any negative associations that they already have. Where associational links exist, after one attitude is stimulated, attitudes toward linked nodes should be open to influence while unconnected attitude objects will not be implicated. Just as Judd et al. (1991) show that priming one attitude with survey questions produces more polarized responses to linked attitudes through spreading activation, we expect that attitudes towards linked nodes will differ across treatment conditions — changing indirectly in response to the treatment. Importantly, while we target a message at one attitude, we are interested in these indirect, or “downstream” effects of our stimulus paragraphs, rather than on whether the message changed the targeted attitude itself. Indeed, we have reason to predict that we would find very little change on the explicitly targeted position: Blankenship, Wegener, and Murray (2012) show that changing an attitude with a direct message is challenging — particularly where it concerns controversial policy positions — as participants guard against the obvious effort. They find evidence that an “indirect route” (a message targeted at a different part of the attitude network), is “more effective in changing the policy attitudes” (Blankenship, Wegener, and Murray, 2012, 609). If a message successfully leads to attitude shifts, we expect to see evidence through changes in linked attitudes. Thus, this design enables us to get at the process through which some attitudes shape others and identify the directions in which this constraint occurs. The stimulus materials – presented in full in the appendix §2.1 – were similar in length, compo6 In graph theory, attitude constraint implies a directed graph; given distinct attitudes A and B, if A constrains B, we we draw a directed edge (A, B). If both (A, B) and (B, A) exist, we have a multigraph. 14 sition, and the inclusion of an expert quote identifying reasons for an individual to have a positive attitude toward the element of the network being manipulated; for example, authority/respect is emphasized because “authority figures represent the wisdom of their position and can help people have better lives by protecting them.” These short messages were created to only activate the relevant node in the network, deliberately avoiding language that might directly reference important components of any of the other elements.7 As a result, the manipulation ensured that only the element of interest was directly stimulated, such that changes in participants’ position on other nodes in the network, relative to participants who received the control message, could be attributed the association within the network. Our experimental approach thus differs dramatically from most investigations into attitude structure in political science, which infers structure by searching for correlations in individuals’ responses to survey items. We contend that our approach has three advantages. First, as we note above, to suggest that one attitude determines an individual’s position on another posits a dynamic, directed process, and experiments are better suited to determining whether A constrains B or B constrains A within the network. Second, it ties into Converse’s (1964) original understanding of dynamic attitude constraint: the idea that “when new information changes the status of one ideaelement in a belief system, by postulate some other change must occur as well.” (p. 208) Our experimental design mimics this process by targeting new information at each node in the network, and measuring what changes occur with respect to the other nodes. Third, by mapping changes across the network rather than from one attitude to another, we are able to investigate how the system operates holistically. We first exposed participants to the persuasive message. Next, we measured each participant’s position on the eleven elements of our proposed attitude network. Questions were presented to par7 Because they relate to salient foreign policy issues, some of the messages may be more likely to trigger affective reactions while others may be read as more cognitively-laden. Consistent with the psychology literature on automatic attitude activation, where both affect and cognition are implicated in spreading activation (Fazio, 2001), we expect that either process will facilitate change in associated attitudes. 15 ticipants in nine blocks in random order to avoid potentially confounding order effects. To measure participants’ endorsement of the abstract moral values, we relied on the twenty-four items pertaining to these four values that are included on the full version of the moral foundations questionnaire developed by Haidt and Graham (2007). In these questions, participants record how relevant particular considerations are to their judgments of right and wrong on a 5-point scale ranging from “not at all relevant” to “extremely relevant” and the extent to which they agree with statements about the importance of each value on a 6-point scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” These two types of value questions are each presented in a separate block, with items measuring all of the four values randomized within each block. While widely used scales exist for the purposes of measuring militant and cooperative internationalism (Wittkopf, 1986), many of their items measure these orientations by gauging support for specific policy items such as Cold War containment or the importance of intervening in humanitarian crises. Because determining whether attitudes at one level of abstraction spark more widespread change than others requires keeping general orientations and specific policy attitudes distinct, we chose subsets of the standard MI and CI items for use in this study. These questions tap participants’ general ideas about the use of force in global politics (MI) and about the need for the U.S. to cooperate with other nations and organizations to solve transnational problems (CI). Participants recorded the extent to which they agreed with each of the items on a scale from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 7 (“strongly agree”). Isolationism is measured using a four item scale that taps the extent to which individuals think that the U.S. would be better served by focusing on national issues as opposed to becoming involved in the world. The final four blocks of questions measured attitudes toward the 2003 U.S. intervention in Iraq, the 2011 airstrikes conducted by NATO in Libya, the potential for U.S. military involvement in the Syrian conflict, and the possibility of a new agreement to replace the expired Kyoto Protocol. Items included in these blocks were drawn from questions used on a variety of public opinion polls conducted by organizations such as ABC News and the Pew Research 16 Center. Responses were provided on 4-7 point scales, with higher scores reflecting more support for the policy option.8 Finally, participants reported their political ideology and party identification, indicated their personal interest in foreign policy issues, and completed a 9-item scale measuring political knowledge. The scale included four questions recommended by Delli Carpini and Keeter (1993), together with five additional questions, three of which specifically tested knowledge on international political issues.9 Creating the directed multigraphs The conventional method of presenting the results would be to simply analyze the magnitude and statistical significance of each of the treatments on each of the dependent variables in sequence. However, this sequential approach is problematic for two reasons. First, with eleven treatments and eleven dependent variables, the results are too complex to clearly convey, and the sequential approach thus obscures more than it reveals. Second, and perhaps more importantly, our quantities of interest are not individual treatment effects, but the overall configuration; our hypotheses concern the network-level structure of foreign policy attitudes, rather than whether one particular attitude influences another. Thus, we present our results in a different manner, analyzing the experimental data using tools from network analysis, which to our knowledge is one of the project’s novel contributions. In our setup, each attitude object (authority, the Iraq war, etc.) constitutes a node or vertex in a network. If manipulating one node causes a change in another node above a certain threshold, we deem the two nodes to be connected to one another (or, in network terminology, to share an edge). For example, if stimulating authority causes a change in attitudes towards the Iraq war, we draw an 8 Measurement scales for each item are presented in full in appendix §2.2; scale reliabilities in appendix §3. The additional questions were: “Who is the Speaker of the U.S. House of Representatives?”; “Who is the current majority leader of the U.S. Senate?”; “Who is the Prime Minister of the United Kingdom?”; “What country is led by Hamid Karzai?” and “Which five countries are permanent members of the U.N. Security Council?” 9 17 edge from authority to Iraq. Because the experimental design lets us calculate treatment effects bidirectionally — that is, we can tell both whether manipulating node A causes a change in node B, and whether manipulating node B causes a change in node A — the network is a directed graph. By using network analytic techniques to analyze the experimental results, we can not only display the network graphically, but also use simple statistical techniques to analyze the topology of the network, thereby highlighting the “big picture” of the results even while comparing across subgroups. This approach has four steps: (i) calculate the treatment effects, (ii) “thin” or “threshold” the results by retaining only the largest treatment effects, which are then entered into an adjacency matrix, (iii) plot the adjacency matrix as a network, and (iv) employ permutation and randomization tests to calculate the probability that the observed network topologies are due to chance. We discuss each step in turn. The first step to generating any network is to define its edges. Whereas some types of relations are easily dichotomized and thus have fairly straightforward criteria for what constitutes an edge – for example, whether senators i and j cosponsored a bill in a given time period (Fowler, 2006) – others are more ambiguous (Butts, 2009). The conventional approach would be to estimate each of our treatment effects using t-tests, drawing an edge from node i to node j if i’s treatment effect on j is statistically significant according to threshold α. The difficulty with this method, however, is that the sheer number of hypothesis tests produces an obvious multiple comparisons problem, for which even relatively powerful empirical Bayes techniques like local fdr will produce overly sparse networks. Instead, we propose two solutions. First, we could simply abandon thresholding altogether (Thomas and Blitzstein, 2011), drawing edges between all nodes and weighting them according to the magnitude of each respective treatment effect. To facilitate the use of simpler binary network statistics, however, we instead adopt a thresholding approach, in which we threshold edges based upon the distribution of potential effect sizes in the data, rather than by making reference to 18 a broader population from which our sample is assumed to be drawn.10 We first calculate all of the treatment effects all at once, both for the full sample of participants, and for a series of subsamples dividing the participants along two measures of political sophistication.11 We represent our treatment effects with t-statistics, since they capture both the difference in means between the treatment and control groups and also their sample sizes and standard deviations, but a variety of other statistics could be used, ranging from a simple difference in means to estimates from regression models or matching estimators that also control for pretreatment covariates. In order to produce the analyses reported below, we take the absolute value of the test statistics and threshold them by declaring the top 10% of effects to merit consideration and discarding the remaining 90%; in sensitivity analyses in the Appendix we show that our results are robust to the selection of different cut points. The effects that meet this 10% threshold are then used to produce an 11 × 11 adjacency matrix that depicts the relationships between the 11 nodes in a given sample, where entry (i, j) refers to the presence of an edge from treatment i to dependent variable j. Since the matrix is binarized and thresholded, those treatment effects that meet the 10% test are represented with a 1, and those that fail to meet the test are represented with a 0.12 We can then plot this adjacency matrix, and calculate a variety of network statistics to characterize the topology of the network. Moreover, we can then engage in network inference to produce uncertainty estimates around these network statistics, comparing our observed networks with randomly generated networks of similar size and density, to calculate the probability that features of our network are due solely to chance. To evaluate the likelihood that the distributions of edges amongst core values, orientations, and policy attitudes are simply due to chance, we perform a series of permutation tests where we ran10 In using the observed data to determine whether a particular effect merits consideration, our analyses are similar in spirit to other forms of local inference (Efron, 2010; Keele, McConnaughy, and White, 2012). 11 By calculating the distribution of treatment effects for all of our analyses at once, it prevents the analyses from being ad-hoc. 12 If no thresholding was employed, a weighted adjacency matrix would be used, in which each entry in the matrix would depict the magnitude of a treatment effect, rescaled to be bounded by 0 and 1. 19 domly generate 10000 networks of an identical size and density as our observed network, to test the probability that we would observe a given network statistic (the number of edges connected to a node or category of nodes) due simply to network size and density alone (Butts, 2008).13 Results We present our results in two stages. First, we analyze the attitude network across our full sample. Second, because we expect heterogeneous treatment effects, we generate separate attitude networks for different subgroups to show how foreign policy attitude structure varies as a function of political knowledge and interest in foreign policy. Throughout, we assess the topologies of the observed networks with reference to whether the results support the assumptions implicit in extant models of attitude structure, and what we learn by “zooming out” to the system level. The full sample We begin by plotting the 11-node network for the full sample, in Figure 1.14 Core values are depicted with circles, foreign policy orientations with diamonds, and specific policy attitudes with triangles. The arrows indicate the direction of the attitude constraint, and nodes are scaled by their degree centrality, which measures each nodes’ connectedness to other nodes in the sample: the larger the node, the more important a role it plays in the network.15 Additionally, emphasizing the importance of directional hypotheses in models assuming that the most abstract attitudes shape more concrete positions (from the top-down), it is also useful to differentiate between outdegree centrality — that is, how many nodes a particular attitude influences — versus indegree centrality — that is, how many nodes influence a particular attitude, which we discuss further with reference 13 See appendix §5 for permutation tests to evaluate network density. All network plots and statistics are generated using the statnet suite of packages in R. (Handcock et al., 2008) 15 We estimated additional measures of centrality, such as Eigenvector centrality, but because of the small number of nodes in the network, many of these measures failed to converge. As Butts (2008, 22) notes, however, degree centrality is highly correlated “with most other measures of centrality, making it a powerful summary index,” and thus a satisfactory measure of a node’s connectedness in attitude networks. 14 20 Figure 1: Foreign policy attitude network: full sample Auth Fair Syria Ingroup Iraq Libya Iso Harm MI CI Kyoto Note: Arrows depict the thresholded treatment effects, and thus, patterns of attitude constraint; nodes are scaled by their degree centrality, so larger nodes are connected to a higher number of other attitudes. Core values are represented as circles, foreign policy orientations as diamonds, and policy attitudes as triangles. Thus, core values play a smaller role in the results from the full sample — and specific policy attitudes play a larger role — than typical models of foreign policy attitude structure would suggest. 21 to the specific networks. An observable implication of top-down influence is that values will have only outgoing connections (as abstract values shape orientations and policy positions below them) and thus will have higher out-degree centrality, but the fact that more specific attitudes do not shape values should produce very low levels of in-degree centrality. Policy positions should display the opposite pattern if these assumptions are correct, with higher levels of in-degree centrality implying that they are constrained by higher-order concepts, and an absence of out-degree ties that would otherwise indicate that policy attitudes can stimulate change across elements of a belief system. Contrary to top-down assumptions about attitude constraint, values do not play a large role in shaping less abstract orientations or policy attitudes: in fact, fairness/reciprocity, ingroup/loyalty, and harm/care are isolates, disconnected from other nodes in the network. In other words, there is a general absence of links between moral values and foreign policy attitudes. Indeed, permutation tests demonstrate that moral values play significantly less of a role (p = 0.024), and policy positions play significantly more of a role (p = 0.046), as measured by degree centrality, than would be expected by chance.16 Constraint is evident in multiple directions, both from the bottom-up — positions on the Iraq war shape both isolationism and authority — and within the same level of specificity, as policy attitudes shape one another. Attitudes toward the war in Iraq are most central in the full network, with more overall and outgoing connections than what we would expect by chance (p = 0.012 and p = 0.001, respectively). This corroborates our prediction that, consistent with operational code analyses and psychological theories of how networks in memory are organized, highly salient foreign policy events can link to other activated attitudes, and thus come to influence more general ideas about how foreign policy should be conducted. Figure 1 thus offers an initial indication of the benefits of a network approach, displaying patterns of stimulation flowing in directions that are inconsistent with our current understanding of attitude structure. At the same 16 See the Table 1 for a complete summary of the permuted analyses. The statistics show the number of total edges (degree centrality), outgoing edges (outdegree centrality), and incoming edges (indegree centrality) by node type. They demonstrate whether specific node types (values, orientations, or policy positions) display greater levels of centrality than we would see in a random network of equal size and density. 22 time, however, with psychological research on interattudinal structure as a foundation (Judd and Krosnick, 1989), we emphasize that properties of attitude networks are likely to vary considerably with individual-level characteristics like political sophistication, such that the relatively sparse attitude network derived from average treatment effects across the full sample might belie considerable treatment heterogeneity when the sample is partitioned into subgroups – a hypothesis we explore below.17 Subgroup analyses Political sophistication Political sophistication offers an opportunity to investigate the appropriateness of “one-size fits all” models of attitude structure. Although much of the foreign policy attitude literature has sought to show that both foreign policy experts and the general public structure their foreign policy attitudes similarly (e.g. Holsti and Rosenau, 1988; Rathbun, 2007), political knowledge has been found to affect both people’s likelihood of receiving political information (Zaller, 1992), and how they process the information they do receive (Fiske, Lau, and Smith, 1990). Given that the formation of associational links is affected by both the reception and processing of information, we expect that participants with different levels of political sophistication will exhibit distinct patterns of constraint at the system level. Since conclusions about political sophistication depend on how it is measured (Luskin, 1987), we look at it in two different ways: first, as political knowledge (Delli Carpini and Keeter, 1993), and second as interest in foreign policy issues (Krosnick, 1990). Figure 2 displays the attitude networks by three levels of political knowledge. First, network density statistics that measure how connected each network is as a whole reveal a curvilinear effect, in that moderately knowledgeable participants have more densely connected foreign policy attitude networks than both their high and low-knowledge counterparts – the middle group’s network is 1.63 17 We also explore gender- and ideology-based structural differences in Appendix §5. 23 Table 1: Permuted Network Statistics Degree Centrality Observed p-value Out-degree Centrality In-degree Centrality Observed Observed p-value p-value Full Network Values 1 [2,8] 0.024 0 [0,5] 0.057 1 [0,5] 0.261 Orientations 3 [1,7] 0.531 1 [0,4] 0.450 2 [0,4] 0.549 Policies 8 [2,8] 0.046 5 [0,5] 0.038 3 [0,5] 0.419 Low Interest Values 11 [6,15] 0.503 5 [2,9] 0.560 6 [2,9] 0.444 Orientations 4 [4,12] 0.054 2 [1,7] 0.180 2 [1,7] 0.185 Policies 17 [6,16] 0.010 9 [2,9] 0.039 8 [2,9] 0.109 Medium Interest Values 4 [3,10] 0.221 2 [1,6] 0.380 2 [1,6] 0.367 Orientations 4 [1,8] 0.526 1 [0,5] 0.294 3 [0,5] 0.395 Policies 6 [3,10] 0.578 4 [1,6] 0.343 2 [1,6] 0.387 High Interest Values 0 [4,13] 0.000 0 [1,8] 0.004 0 [1,7] 0.004 Orientations 13 [3,10] 0.002 6 [1,6] 0.064 7 [1,6] 0.016 Policies 9 [5,13] 0.505 5 [1,8] 0.441 4 [1,8] 0.564 Low Knowledge Values 6 [4,12] 0.255 2 [1,7] 0.172 4 [1,7] 0.609 Orientations 7 [2,10] 0.378 5 [0,6] 0.138 2 [0,6] 0.400 Policies 9 [4,12] 0.392 4 [1,7] 0.611 5 [1,7] 0.357 Medium Knowledge Values 10 [9,19] 0.106 6 [3,11] 0.415 4 [3,11] 0.103 Orientations 13 [6,15] 0.196 5 [2,9] 0.578 8 [2,9] 0.104 Policies 13 [9,19] 0.452 7 [3,11] 0.582 6 [3,11] 0.416 High Knowledge Values 9 [5, 15] 0.445 5 [2,8] 0.599 4 [2,8] 0.413 Orientations 11 [3,12] 0.081 5 [1,7] 0.295 6 [1,7] 0.132 Policies 10 [5,15] 0.559 5 [2,8] 0.592 5 [2,8] 0.603 Note: table shows the observed network total, in-degree, or out-degree centrality — the number of observed connections — for a given node type [along with 95% CIs derived from 10000 permuted networks], and the corresponding p-values. 24 Figure 2: Foreign policy attitude network by political knowledge (a) Low-knowledge (b) Moderate-knowledge Kyoto MI Kyoto Iso Ingroup Syria Fair Iraq Fair Libya Auth Iraq CI Harm Iso Libya Auth Syria Harm Ingroup CI MI (c) High-knowledge Iso Fair Syria CI Iraq Ingroup Harm Auth MI Kyoto Libya Note: Arrows depict the thresholded treatment effects, and thus, patterns of attitude constraint; nodes are scaled by their degree centrality. Core values are represented as circles, foreign policy orientations as diamonds, and policy attitudes as triangles. Consistent with Zaller (1992), knowledge displays a curvilinear effect here, as the medium-knowledge network has a greater density than either the low-knowledge or high-knowledge counterparts. 25 times as dense as those low in political knowledge, and 1.2 times the density of the high knowledge network. This is consistent with Zaller’s (1992) conclusion that the most sophisticated individuals have stable attitudes that are more resistant to change, and that the least sophisticated lack the ability to form and store connections between abstract and specific concepts like values to specific attitudes. Instead, it is the moderate sophisticates, who hold multiple considerations but have the ability to connect concepts, that are most open to indirect attitude change via the stimulus messages. Second, each of these three subgroup networks are more densely connected than the aggregated network in Figure 1, likely due to the fact that aggregating attitude structure for an entire sample of participants masks important treatment heterogeneity. Finally, of the three levels of political knowledge, only the network of the most knowledgeable participants includes a role for orientations that is greater than what a random assignment of edges would predict (p = 0.081). Thus, as Converse would predict, it appears that only the most sophisticated individuals engage the abstract views about foreign policy so familiar to academics when forming their attitudes. Figure 3 operationalizes political sophistication differently by looking at the extent to which participants expressed interest in foreign policy issues, since political interest is often held to be both a cause and consequence of political sophistication (Fiske, Lau, and Smith, 1990). The results show strikingly different patterns compared to political knowledge: with political knowledge, the moderate knowledge network was the most dense, whereas with foreign policy interest, we see that individuals who expressed a moderate amount of interest in foreign policy issues have attitude networks that are less dense than both those who reported low or high levels of interest. Those least interested in foreign policy have attitude networks that are driven primarily by specific policy positions, most evidently those regarding the war in Iraq — policy attitudes play a larger role in the low-interest attitude network than we would expect due to chance (p = 0.010). Further analysis reveals that the policy-based connections are primarily out-going, demonstrating more precisely that the direction of influence flows from specific policy attitudes to other elements of attitude networks 26 Figure 3: Foreign policy attitude network by interest in foreign policy issues (a) Low-interest (b) Medium-interest CI Harm Libya Syria Libya Ingroup MI MI Auth Iraq Syria Iraq Kyoto Fair Iso Kyoto Auth Harm Iso Ingroup CI Fair (c) High-interest Harm Iraq Kyoto CI Auth Iso MI Fair Libya Ingroup Syria Note: Arrows depict the thresholded treatment effects, and thus, patterns of attitude constraint; nodes are scaled by their degree centrality. Core values are represented as circles, foreign policy orientations as diamonds, and policy attitudes as triangles. Here, we see the inverse curvilinear effect to that from Figure 2, in that the medium-interest network displays less constraint than its low-interest and high-interest counterparts, with specific policy events playing a greater role influencing attitudes throughout the network for low-interest participants, and the more abstract foreign policy orientations playing a greater role influencing other attitudes for high-interest participants. 27 for the uninterested public. Those least interested in foreign policy issues therefore have patterns of associations that could lead to widespread changes in the network of attitudes based on a single event in U.S. foreign policy — the chief concern of Almond, Lippmann, and the other postwar realists. Thus, when the uninterested engage with a foreign policy event, they are more likely to base their broader ideas about how the US should conduct itself internationally on that attitude, as they come to believe that it implicates other pre-existing but newly associated attitude objects. Multiple nodes in the network update in response to the attitude. In contrast, general orientations play a greater role in the networks of those most interested in international affairs, similar to what we found in the high knowledge subgroup (p = 0.002). Moreover, values play no role at all in the foreign policy networks of the most interested segment of our sample, which is telling given the large number of edges in this network (p = 0.000). Consistent with Judd and Krosnick’s (1989) findings that attitudes are more reliably consistent among the group of individuals who ascribe importance to the political attitudes being investigated, those participants who report high levels of interest in foreign policy seem to closely connect their general views on how foreign policy should be conducted (orientations) to specific issues, but leave out the values that may not be obviously associated with the rest. The high interest network also exhibits the patterns most closely consistent with top-down directionality assumptions, given that constraint flows from orientations to policy items, but bottom-up effects such as the influence of Libya on both MI and Isolationism, bidrectional edges indicating within-level influences between CI and Isolationism, and the nonexistent role of core values suggests that this assuming effects in only one direction misses much of the story. The results of the two political sophistication analyses thus suggest two findings. First, both general political knowledge and interest in foreign policy affect how foreign policy attitudes are structured, as well as the degree of structure itself — something that we can only understand by turning to the properties of the system. The importance of sophistication thus suggests that, in con- 28 trast to many existing models of foreign policy attitude structure (e.g., Holsti and Rosenau, 1988; Rathbun, 2014), expertise matters. Second, regardless of how one operationalizes political sophistication, we detect violations of the top-down patterns of constraint assumed by past work, finding signs of bottom-up patterns of influence, bidirectional edges across levels, and constraint among attitudes within levels of abstraction. Conclusion Political scientists have long sought evidence of structure and constraint in foreign policy attitudes, producing a variety of models based on divergent sets of assumptions about how these attitudes are organized. In this paper, we build on these important contributions by proposing a new way of studying political attitudes: networks. We argued that interattitudinal structure is best conceptualized as a flexible network of nodes, interacting with one another as part of a system, and developed a novel experimental design that illuminates the properties of the whole system rather than relations between its component parts while studying attitudes as network data. Our intent here is thus to explore a different ontology for studying belief systems. The results from our experimental “plausibility probe” suggest four key conclusions. First, conceptualizing attitudes as networks offers a promising theoretical framework. With our design, we were able to visualize interactions within a broader system of attitudes, directly observing which types of attitudes stimulated more change throughout the system. We bring together findings from across the foreign policy attitudes literature to determine how various relationships reported elsewhere work within an attitude network. Additionally, the networked paradigm allows us to study system-level properties like network density — which revealed important differences in the attitude structures of political sophisticates at different tiers — that would have been difficult to uncover in a traditional framework. The experiment thus provides insight into holistic properties of attitude networks. 29 Second, Converse’s (1964) pessimistic assessment about the paucity of top-down constraint in the American public seems to be more accurate than revisionist accounts have suggested: while regression and SEM-based research designs assume that values and abstract dispositions will be most influential in ultimately determining policy positions, we find that values generally play less and policy positions more of a role in the system of attitudes than we would expect by chance. Given the extent to which IR scholars have long complained about the moralistic nature of public opinion — Hans Morgenthau claimed that “intoxication with moral abstractions” was “one of the great sources of weakness and failure in American foreign policy” (Morgenthau, 1951, 4) — it is noteworthy that we find more evidence of specific policy issues constraining moral values than of moral values constraining specific policy issues. The outsize role played by bottom-up constraint suggests that the public is more likely to tie leaders’ hands in response to specific policy issues – particularly involving salient events – rather than abstract principles per se. Third, when it comes to attitude structure, one size does not fit all: the oft-dramatic differences revealed in subgroup analyses clearly demonstrate that there is no single attitude structure that can be readily applied to all members of the population at large. Instead, we find sensible patterns of divergence based on political sophistication. This ultimately suggests that it is misleading to speak of such a thing as “the” structure of foreign policy attitudes — but it also suggests that the IR literature, which frequently views the public as a monolithic unit responding to international events in tandem (Fearon, 1994), can benefit from taking this heterogeneity into account. Finally, we show that political knowledge and levels of interest in foreign policy are particularly important in understanding how attitude networks are organized. Those with moderate levels of knowledge have more densely connected attitude networks than their high and low-knowledge counterparts, and interest in foreign policy determines whether the direction of constraint flows mainly from more specific or abstract attitude objects in the network. The central role of expertise in our findings thus raises questions about whether it is realistic to expect the mass public to 30 organize their foreign policy attitudes in the same manner as do foreign policy elites. Importantly, the results also have implications far beyond the study of foreign policy attitudes. Although political scientists are well aware of the danger of order effects in survey responses, the network patterns of interdependence detected here suggest that order effects may be even more problematic than many political scientists may realize, as activating one attitude can affect evaluations of other attitudes across levels of abstraction. Moreover, directional debates in attitude structure are relatively common in political science: do policy preferences cause party ID, or does party ID cause policy preferences? (Jackson, 1975) Does racial prejudice cause attitudes towards specific policies like welfare and busing? (Sears, 1988) Do evaluations of political parties stem from economic assessments, or do economic perceptions drive political support? (Evans and Andersen, 2006) Traditionally, these hypotheses have been tested either using path analysis of cross-sectional data (whereupon assumptions about the direction of causality is governed entirely by theoretical assumptions), and panel surveys, which offer more leverage on causal inference. The directed multigraph experimental design fielded here offers another way to test theories of directional attitude interdependence and offers yet another path for matching our methods to our theories. Finally, the current investigation suggests four fruitful avenues for future research. First, although we investigate a number of individual differences in the subgroup analyses, the analyses are not multivariate. Given the current experimental design and sample size, we are only able to control for one variable at a time, but developing a factorial design will facilitate multivariate analyses that may provide additional insight into the structure of foreign policy attitude networks across individuals. Second, although we employ a thresholding approach when drawing edges between nodes, based on the distribution of effects in the sample, future work could examine not just the presence of a significant treatment effect, but its substantive impact as well, treating the experimental results as a weighted network. Third, the between-subjects component of our design limits our ability to investigate changes in an individual’s unique attitude network. Developing a within-subjects version 31 of a directed multigraph experiment will allow us to more clearly discern causal patterns of attitude constraint. Finally, the current investigation explored whether individual-level attributes affect the topology of attitude structure, but future research should examine how particular attitude-level attributes (strength, extremity, level of abstraction, and so on) structure the topology of foreign policy belief systems. References Anderson, John R. 1983. The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Arceneaux, Kevin. 2012. “Cognitive Biases and the Strength of Political Arguments.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (2): 271-285. Berinsky, Adam J., Gregory A. Huber, and Gabriel S. Lenz. 2012. “Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk.” Political Analysis 20 (3): 351-368. Blankenship, Kevin L, Duane T Wegener, and Renee A Murray. 2012. “Circumventing Resistance: Using Values to Indirectly Change Attitudes.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 103 (4): 606. Bower, Gordon H. 1981. “Mood and Memory.” American Psychologist 36 (2): 129-148. Brewer, Paul R., and Marco R. Steenbergen. 2002. “All Against All: How Beliefs about Human Nature Shape Foreign Policy Opinions.” Political Psychology 23 (1): 39-58. Buhrmester, Michael, Tracy Kwang, and Samuel D. Gosling. 2011. “Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A New Source of Inexpensive, Yet High-Quality, Data?” Perspectives on Psychological Science 6 (1): 3-5. Butts, Carter T. 2008. “Social Network Analysis: A Methodological Introduction.” Asian Journal of Social Psychology 11: 13-41. Butts, Carter T. 2009. “Revisiting the Foundations of Network Analysis.” Science 325 (July 24 2009): 414-416. 32 Chittick, William O., Keith R. Billingsley, and Rick Travis. 1995. “A Three-Dimensional Model of American Foreign Policy Beliefs.” International Studies Quarterly 39 (3): 313-331. Collins, Allan M., and M. Ross Quillian. 1969. “Retrieval Time from Semantic Memory.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8 (2): 240-247. Conover, Pamela J., and Stanley Feldman. 1984. “How people organize the political world: a schematic model.” American Journal of Political Science 28 (1): 95-126. Converse, Philip E. 1964. “The nature and origin of belief systems in mass publics.” In Ideology and Discontent, ed. David E. Apter. New York: Free Press. Delli Carpini, Michael X., and Scott Keeter. 1993. “Measuring Political Knowledge: Putting First Things First.” American Journal of Political Science 37 (4): 1179-1206. Dinauer, Leslie D., and Edward L. Fink. 2005. “Interattitude Structure and Attitude Dynamics: A Comparison of the Hierarchical and Galileo Spatial-Linkage Models.” Human Communication Research 31 (1): 1-32. Druckman, James N., and Cindy D. Kam. 2011. “Students as Experimental Participants.” In Cambridge Handbook of Political Science, ed. James N. Druckman, Donald P. Green, James H. Kuklinski, and Arthur Lupia. Cambridge University Press. Eagly, Alice H., and Shelly Chaiken. 2007. “The Advantages of an Inclusive Definition of Attitude.” Social Cognition 25 (5): 582-602. Efron, Bradley. 2010. Large-Scale Inference: Empirical Bayes Methods for Estimation, Testing, and Prediction. Cambridge University Press. Evans, Geoffrey, and Robert Andersen. 2006. “The Political Conditioning of Economic Perceptions.” Journal of Politics 68 (1): 194-207. Fazio, Russell H. 2001. “On the Automatic Activation of Associated Evaluations: An Overview.” Cognition & Emotion 15 (2): 115–141. Fearon, James D. 1994. “Domestic Political Audiences and the Escalation of International Disputes.” 33 American Political Science Review 88 (3): 577-592. Feldman, J.A., and D.H. Ballard. 1982. “Connectionist Models and Their Properties.” Cognitive Science 6 (3): 205-254. Feldman, Stanley. 2003. “Values, Ideology, and the Structure of Political Attitudes.” In Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, ed. David O. Sears, Leonie Huddy, and Robert Jervis. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fiske, Susan T., Richard R. Lau, and Richard A. Smith. 1990. “On the varieties and utilities of political expertise.” Social Cognition 8 (1): 31-48. Fiske, Susan T., and Shelley E. Taylor. 1984. Social Cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fowler, James H. 2006. “Legislative cosponsorship networks in the US House and Senate.” Social Networks 28: 454-465. Goren, Paul. 2005. “Party Identification and Core Political Values.” American Journal of Political Science 49 (4): 881-896. Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, and Brian A. Nosek. 2009. “Liberals and Conservatives Rely on Different Sets of Moral Foundations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 (5): 10291046. Grimmer, Justin, Solomon Messing, and Sean J. Westwood. 2012. “How Words and Money Cultivate a Personal Vote: The Effect of Legislator Credit Claiming on Constituent Credit Allocation.” American Political Science Review 106 (4): 703-719. Hafner-Burton, Emilie M., Miles Kahler, and Alexander H. Montgomery. 2009. “Network Analysis for International Relations.” International Organization 63: 559-592. Haidt, Jonathan, and Jesse Graham. 2007. “When Morality Opposes Justice: Conservatives Have Moral Intuitions that Liberals may not Recognize.” Social Justice Research 20 (1): 98-116. Handcock, Mark S., David R. Hunter, Carter T. Butts, Steven M. Goodreau, and Martina Morris. 2008. “statnet: Software Tools for the Representation, Visualization, Analysis and Simulation of 34 Network Data.” Journal of Statistical Software 24 (1): 1-11. Holsti, Ole. 1970. “The ‘Operational Code’ Approach to the Study of Political Leaders: John Foster Dulles’ Philosophical and Instrumental Beliefs.” Canadian Journal of Political Science 3 (1): 123– 157. Holsti, Ole R. 1979. “The Three-Headed Eagle: The United States and System Change.” International Studies Quarterly 23 (3): 339-359. Holsti, Ole R. 2004. Public Opinion and American Foreign Policy. Revised edition ed. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Holsti, Ole R., and James N. Rosenau. 1988. “The Domestic and Foreign Policy Beliefs of American Leaders.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 32 (2): 248-294. Holsti, Ole R, and James N Rosenau. 1990. “The structure of foreign policy attitudes among American leaders.” The Journal of Politics 52 (01): 94–125. Huber, Gregory A., Seth J. Hill, and Gabriel S. Lenz. 2012. “Sources of Bias in Retrospective Decision Making: Experimental Evidence on Voters’ Limitations in Controlling Incumbents.” American Political Science Review 106 (4): 720-741. Hurwitz, Jon, and Mark Peffley. 1987. “How Are Foreign Policy Attitudes Structured?” American Political Science Review 81 (4): 1099-1120. Imai, Kosuke, Luke Keele, Dustin H. Tingley, and Teppei Yamamoto. 2011. “Unpacking the Black Box of Causality: Learning about Causal Mechanisms from Experimental and Observational Studies.” American Political Science Review 105 (November 2011): 765-789. Jackson, John E. 1975. “Issues, Party Choices, and Presidential Votes.” American Journal of Political Science 19 (May): 161-185. Judd, Charles M., and Jon A. Krosnick. 1989. “The structural bases of consistency among political attitudes: Effects of political expertise and attitude importance.” In Attitude structure and function, ed. Anthony R. Pratkanis, Steven J. Breckler, and Anthony G. Greenwald. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 35 Judd, Charles M., Roger A. Drake, James W. Downing, and Jon A. Krosnick. 1991. “Some Dynamic Properties of Attitude Structures: Context-Induced Response Facilitation and Polarization.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60 (2): 193-202. Keele, Luke, Corrine McConnaughy, and Ismail White. 2012. “Strengthening the Experimenter’s Toolbox: Statistical Estimation of Internal Validity.” American Journal of Political Science 56 (2): 484-499. Kegley, Charles W., Jr. 1986. “Assumptions and Dilemmas in the Study of American’s Foreign Policy Beliefs: A Caveat.” International Studies Quarterly 30 (4): 447-471. Kertzer, Joshua D., Kathleen Powers, Brian C. Rathbun, and Ravi Iyer. 2014. “Do moral values shape foreign policy preferences?” Unpublished manuscript. Krosnick, Jon A. 1990. “Government Policy and Citizen Passion: A Study of Issue Publics in Contemporary America.” Political Behavior 12 (1): 59-92. Lakatos, Imre. 1970. “Falsification and the Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes.” In Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge, ed. Imre Lakatos and Alan Musgrave. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lavine, Howard, Cynthia J. Thomsen, and Marti Hope Gonzales. 1997. “The Development of Interattitudinal Consistency: The Shared-Consequences Model.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 (4): 735-749. Liberman, Peter. 2013a. “Retributive Support for International Punishment and Torture.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 57 (2): 285-306. Liberman, Peter. 2013b. “Retributive support for international punishment and torture.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 57 (2): 285–306. Lodge, Milton, and Charles S Taber. 2013. The Rationalizing Voter. Cambridge University Press. Lodge, Milton and Kathleen M. McGraw, eds. 1995. Political Judgment: Structure and Process. University of Michigan Press. 36 Loftus, Elizabeth, and Allan M. Collins. 1975. “A Spreading Activation Theory of Semantic Processing.” Psychological Review 82 (6): 407-428. Lupton, Robert N, Shane P Singh, and Judd R Thornton. 2014. “The Moderating Impact of Social Networks on the Relationships Among Core Values, Partisanship, and Candidate Evaluations.” Political Psychology. Luskin, Robert C. 1987. “Measuring Political Sophistication.” American Journal of Political Science 31 (November 1987): 856-899. McGraw, Kathleen M. 2000. “Contributions of the Cognitive Approach to Political Psychology.” Political Psychology 21 (4): 805-832. McGuire, William J. 1989. “The Structure of Individual Attitudes and Attitude Systems.” In Attitude structure and function, ed. Anthony R. Pratkanis, Steven J. Breckler, and Anthony G. Greenwald. Erlbaum. Monroe, Brian M., and Stephen J. Read. 2008. “A General Connectionist Model of Attitude Structure and Change: The ACS (Attitudes as Constraint Satisfaction) Model.” Psychological Review 115 (3): 733-759. Morgenthau, Hans J. 1951. In Defense of the National Interest. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Murray, Shoon. 1992. “Turning an Elite Cross-Sectional Survey into a Panel Study while Protecting Anonymity.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 36 (3): 586-595. Nie, Norman H, Sidney Verba, and John R. Petrocik. 1979. The Changing American Voter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Oldendick, Robert, and Barbara Ann Bardes. 1981. “Belief Structures and Foreign Policy: Comparing Dimensions of Elite and Mass Opinions.” Social Science Quarterly 62 (3): 432-442. Peffley, Mark A., and Jon Hurwitz. 1985. “A Hierarchical Model of Attitude Constraint.” American Journal of Political Science 29 (Nov.): 871-890. Peffley, Mark, and Jon Hurwitz. 1993. “Models of Attitude Constraint in Foreign Affairs.” Political 37 Behavior 15 (1): 61-90. Rathbun, Brian C. 2007. “Hierarchy and Community at Home and Abroad: Evidence of a Common Structure of Domestic and Foreign Policy Beliefs in American Elites.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 51 (3): 379-407. Rathbun, Brian C. 2014. “Wedges and Widgets: The Ideological Origins of Trade Attitudes.” Foreign Policy Analysis Forthcoming. Renshon, Jonathan. 2009. “When Public Statements Reveal Private Beliefs: Assessing Operational Codes at a Distance.” Political Psychology 30 (4): 649–661. Schafer, Mark, and Stephen G Walker. 2006. “Democratic Leaders and the Democratic Peace: The Operational Codes of Tony Blair and Bill Clinton.” International Studies Quarterly 50 (3): 561– 583. Sears, David O. 1988. “Symbolic Racism.” In Eliminating Racism: Profiles in Controversy, ed. Phyllis A. Katz and Dalmas A. Taylor. Plenum Press. Sinclair, Betsy. 2012. The Social Citizen: Peer Networks and Political Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Smith, Eliot R. 2009. “Distributed Connectionist Models in Social Psychology.” Social and Personality Psychology Compass 3 (1): 64-76. Sokhey, Anand E., and Paul A. Djupe. 2011. “Interpersonal Networks and Democratic Politics.” PS: Political Science & Politics 44 (1): 55-59. Thomas, A. C., and J. K. Blitzstein. 2011. “Valued Ties Tell Fewer Lies: Why Not To Dichotomize Network Edges With Thresholds.” ArXiv e-prints (January). Valentino, Nicholas A. 1999. “Crime News and the Priming of Racial Attitudes During Evaluations of the President.” Public Opinion Quarterly 63 (3): 293-320. Walker, Stephen G, and Mark Schafer. 2000. “The Political Universe of Lyndon B. Johnson and his Advisors: Diagnostic and Strategic Propensities in their Operational Codes.” Political Psychology 38 21 (3): 529–543. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1986. “On the Foreign Policy Beliefs of the American People: A Critique and Some Evidence.” International Studies Quarterly 30 (4): 425-445. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1990. Faces of internationalism: Public Opinion and American foreign policy. Duke University Press. Zaller, John R. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Public Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Zaller, John R., and Stanley Feldman. 1992. “A Simple Theory of the Survey Response: Answering Questions versus Revealing Preferences.” American Journal of Political Science 36 (3): 579-616. 39 Foreign Policy Attitudes as Networks Supplementary Appendix Last revised: August 6, 2014 Contents 1 The network of foreign policy attitude research, 1970-2013 Table 1: Attitude structure models, 1970-2013 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1.1 Latent position cluster model of citation patterns . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Figure 1: The citation network of foreign policy attitude structure research, 1970-2013 . . . 2 Materials and Instrumentation 2.1 Text of Persuasive Messages . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.1.1 Values . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.1.2 Orientations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.1.3 Policy Positions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2 Instrumentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.1 Values: Moral Foundations Questionnaire 2.2.2 Orientations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2.2.3 Policy Positions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 4 5 6 6 6 7 8 10 10 12 13 3 Scale Reliabilities 14 4 Thresholding and sensitivity analyses 15 5 Supplementary Subgroup Analyses 5.1 Gender . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5.2 Political Ideology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17 17 20 6 Additional permutation tests Figure 7: Permutation tests of network density . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20 22 References 23 1 1 The network of foreign policy attitude research, 1970-2013 One of our claims in the manuscript is that existing research on foreign policy attitudes has focused primarily on testing only one type of relationship at a time — either the clusters of policy positions uncovered through factor analysis, for example, or the specific predispositions thought to constrain them, without looking holistically at the dynamic properties — the systemic features — of belief systems. Another way of making this point comes from looking at the conversations that have taken place within the literature itself. We conducted a search of the literature on foreign policy attitude structure published from 1970 to 2013. To compile the list, we used Google Scholar’s search engine to find articles using the search terms “foreign policy attitudes,” “public opinion and foreign policy,” and “foreign policy attitude structure,” as well as searching for works citing each included reference, to compile what we believe to be a fairly comprehensive list of existing work on the structure of foreign policy attitudes.1 We then categorized each of the 70 pieces into one of two camps (the results of which are listed in Table 1), based on the underlying assumptions about patterns of attitude constraint in the of the models they put forward. A piece is listed in the “horizontal” constraint category — to borrow terminology from Holsti 1992 — if it assumes and looks for clusters of related policy attitudes along one or several dimensions (typically recovered through factor analysis). We classify a piece as “vertical” if it assumes that underlying predispositions or values determine individual policy preferences or other more specific orientations toward foreign policy — these pieces look for evidence of top-down constraint, often with regression-based or structural equation modeling approaches. Differentiating between these two approaches not only informs our understanding of the assumptions that we investigate in our directed multigraph experiment, but also allows us to assess our claim that much of this research occurs in parallel and thus misses the opportunity to study the system of attitudes as a whole. Thus, once the pieces were categorized, we examined their citation patterns. If there is little overlap between the citation networks of pieces that employ horizontal or vertical models, this offers further support for our claim that the insights from each are not being combined holistically — a problem that we seek to rectify in our article. 1 A piece of research is included in the literature review if it meets several criteria. First, it must have been published in 1970 or later. Second, the piece of research must be published in an academic journal, as a book, or as part of an edited volume, thus excluding conference papers. Third, the piece is included only if it makes a specific argument about the structural basis of foreign policy attitudes at the individual level, positing certain dimensions or attitudes that shape the foreign policy beliefs of the elites or the public. This criterion means that the list excludes reviews of previously published work (e.g. Holsti, 1992; Russett, Hartley and Murray, 1994), those investigating the influence of personality and other individual differences on foreign policy attitudes (e.g. Federico, Golec and Dial, 2005), models of attitude structure that focus solely or primarily on domestic politics (e.g. Nie and Andersen, 1974; Peffley and Hurwitz, 1985), and the robust body of work that looks at the aggregate longitudinal dynamics of the public’s foreign policy ‘mood’ (e.g. Caspary, 1970; Page and Shapiro, 2010). 2 Table 1: Foreign policy attitude structure model pieces, 1970-2013 Citation H Bardes and Oldendick (1978) ✓ ✓ Bartels (1994) Bennett (1974) ✓ ✓ Binning (2007) Bjereld and Ekengren (1999) V ✓ Citation H V Jenkins-Smith, Mitchell and Herron (2004) ✓ Liberman (2006) ✓ Liberman (2007) ✓ Liberman (2013) ✓ Maggiotto and Wittkopf (1981) ✓ Brewer and Steenbergen (2002) ✓ Mandelbaum and Schneider (1978) ✓ Brewer et al. (2004) ✓ Mandelbaum and Schneider (1979) ✓ Brody and Verba (1972) ✓ Mayton, Peters and Owens (1999) ✓ Chanley (1999) ✓ Modigliani (1972) Chittick and Billingsley (1989) ✓ Murray (1996) Chittick, Billingsley and Travis (1990) ✓ Murray, Cowden and Russett (1999) ✓ Chittick, Billingsley and Travis (1995) ✓ Oldendick and Bardes (1981) ✓ ✓ ✓ Cohrs and Moschner (2002) ✓ Oldendick and Bardes (1982) ✓ Cohrs et al. (2005) ✓ Patchen (1970) ✓ Cohrs et al. (2007) ✓ Peffley and Hurwitz (1992) ✓ Fite, Genest and Wilcox (1990) ✓ Peffley and Hurwitz (1993) ✓ Rathbun (2007) ✓ Herrmann and Keller (2004) ✓ ✓ Herron and Jenkins-Smith (2002) Reifler, Scotto and Clarke (2011) ✓ Hinckley (1988) ✓ Richman (1996) ✓ Hinckley (1992) ✓ Richman, Malone and Nolle (1997) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1976) ✓ Rosati and Creed (1997) ✓ Holsti (1979) ✓ Rosati, Link and Creed (1998) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1979) ✓ Rosenau and Holsti (1983) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1981) ✓ Russett and Hanson (1975) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1984) ✓ Russett et al. (1990) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1986) ✓ Sulfaro (1996) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1988) ✓ Vail and Motyl (2010) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1990) ✓ Wittkopf (1981) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1993) ✓ Wittkopf and Maggiotto (1983a) ✓ Holsti and Rosenau (1996) ✓ Wittkopf and Maggiotto (1983b) ✓ Holsti (2004) ✓ Wittkopf (1986) ✓ Hurwitz and Peffley (1987a) ✓ Wittkopf (1987) ✓ Hurwitz and Peffley (1987b) ✓ Wittkopf (1990) ✓ Hurwitz and Peffley (1990) ✓ Wittkopf (1994) ✓ Hurwitz, Peffley and Seligson (1993) ✓ Ziegler (1987) ✓ Note: H and V refer to horizontal and vertical models of attitude structure, respectively. 3 1.1 Latent position cluster model of citation patterns As Table 1 shows, of the 70 pieces, 47 adopt the tenets of the horizontal model, and 23 contain vertical models. The predominance of horizontal models in Table 1, however, belies the growing popularity of vertical models over time: while 27 of the 29 pieces published before 1990 adhere to the assumptions of horizontal models, a full 12 of the 15 works published after 2000 employ vertical models. Yet although horizontal and vertical models make different assumptions about how attitudes are structured, the tension between these assumptions is rarely discussed, perhaps because proponents of each type of model are focusing more on rejecting the null hypothesis of no constraint, rather than adjudicating between the competing claims of the directionality of constraint or conducting research into the system as a whole. However, it is striking that if we map out how these 70 pieces listed in Table 1 cite one another and analyze the clustering in the citation network using a latent position cluster model (LPCM) (Handcock, Raftery and Tantrum, 2007), the LPCM can correctly classify which “camp” a piece falls into with 71.4% accuracy. Figure 1 plots the citation network in the first two dimensions of a three-dimensional Euclidean latent space; the pie chart corresponding to each article denotes the posterior probability (calculated using MCMC sampling) of the article being located in a particular cluster (red depicting horizontal models, and green depicting vertical models). Despite occasional misclassifications, the LPCM is nonetheless relatively accurate, correctly classifying 73.9% of the vertical models, and 70.2% of the horizontal models — with relative accuracy, then, the model can predict which assumptions a piece makes about how foreign policy attitudes are structured based solely on citation patterns, even without looking at the content of the pieces themselves. This supports our expectations that there is insufficient integration across the literature, and that a systemic perspective can profitably advance insights from across these parallel research traditions. 4 Figure 1: The citation network of foreign policy attitude structure research, 1970-2013 2 Cohrs.2002 Russett.1975 Vail.2010 1 Hurwitz.1987a Bardes.1978 Modigliani.1972 Binning.2007 0 Brody.1972 -1 Second principal component of Z 3 Patchen.1970 Maggiotto.1981 Wittkopf.1981 Mandelbaum.1978 Oldendick.1982 Oldendick.1981 Bennett.1974 Wittkopf.1983a Ziegler.1987 Holsti.1979b + Chittick.1989 Holsti.1979a Holsti.1977 Holsti.1981 Wittkopf.1990 Chittick.1995 Holsti.1984 Mandelbaum.1979 Rosati.1997 Rosenau.1983 Holsti.1988 Hurwitz.1987bWittkopf.1987 Peffley.1993 Chanley.1999 Cohrs.2005 Hurwitz.1990 Hurwitz.1993 Brewer.2002 Brewer.2004 Cohrs.2007 Liberman.2013 Liberman.2006 Hinckley.1992 + Holsti.1996 Herron.2002Bartels.1994 Hinckley.1988 +Holsti.1990 Rathbun.2007 Liberman.2007 Russett.1990 Murray.1999 Murray.1996 Holsti.2004.1996. Wittkopf.1986 Wittkopf.1983b Holsti.1986 Bjereld.1999 Wittkopf.1994 Jenkins.Smith.2004 Fite.1990 Peffley.1992 Herrmann.2004 Mayton.1999 Reifler.2011 Chittick.1990 Rosati.1998 Holsti.1993 Sulfaro.1996 -2 Richman.1996 Richman.1997 -2 0 2 4 First principal component of Z By examining the citation patterns between the 70 foreign policy attitude network pieces listed in Table 1, a latent position cluster model (LPCM) can successfully predict which “camp” each piece falls into with 71.4% accuracy. An arrow from node i to node j shows that article j cites article i: thus, for example Patchen (1970) is cited by Russett and Hanson (1975). The pie chart for each article represents the posterior probability (estimated using 5000 MCMC samples) that each article falls into a particular camp; here, the red represents horizontal models, and the green represents vertical models. The x and y axes represent the first two principal components in a three-dimensional Euclidean latent space as measured by the Minimum Kullback-Leibler (MKL) estimates. The LPCM is estimated using the latentnet package in R (Krivitsky and Handcock, 2008). 5 2 Materials and Instrumentation 2.1 2.1.1 Text of Persuasive Messages Values Authority/respect/hierarchy Philosophers throughout history have stressed that people should show respect for authority figures in a society. Authority figures represent the wisdom of their position and can help people have better lives by protecting them. They also act as exemplars, such that people can learn from them about how to achieve success in their own lives. Prominent psychologists state that many cultures value “virtues related to good leadership, which involve[s] magnanimity, fatherliness, and wisdom.” This emphasis has been echoed by research in anthropology and sociology, which demonstrates the importance of hierarchies and traditions to most cultures. Respecting legitimate authority figures is considered virtuous and fosters a happy and healthy environment for all members of a community. For these reasons, respecting persons in positions of authority is widely regarded as good for both individuals and society. Fairness/reciprocity/community Philosophers throughout history have stressed that people should treat all other people just as they wish to be treated themselves. More contemporary evidence suggests that using this ‘golden rule’ as a guide will produce increasing levels of success in business practices and relationships because it helps to build trust. Prominent psychologists state that “all cultures have developed virtues related to fairness.” This emphasis has been echoed by research in anthropology and sociology, which demonstrates the importance of reciprocity and justice to the cores of societies all over the world. Adherence to such virtues helps to foster happy and healthy environments for all members of a community. For these reasons, equal and just treatment of other humans is widely regarded as good for both individuals and society. Harm/care Philosophers throughout history have stressed that people should avoid doing harm to others, and seek to alleviate suffering where possible. This principle is enshrined in contemporary medical practice, where doctors vow to “first, do no harm.” Prominent psychologists further state that people in most cultures “feel approval to toward those who prevent or relieve harm.” This emphasis has been echoed by research in anthropology and sociology, which demonstrates the importance of kindness as a virtue in societies throughout the world. Adherence to such virtues helps to foster happy and 6 healthy environments for all members of a community. For these reasons, kind and compassionate, rather than harmful, treatment of other humans is widely regarded as good for both individuals and society. Ingroup/loyalty Philosophers throughout history have stressed that people should stay loyal to members of groups to which they belong. Other members of the same group can be trusted and are easy to cooperate with. Thus, it is considered virtuous to stay loyal to one’s group and to make sacrifices to protect and improve the group as a whole. Prominent psychologists state that “most cultures have constructed virtues such as loyalty, patriotism, and heroism.” This emphasis has been echoed by research in anthropology and sociology, which demonstrates the importance of group solidarity to strong societies. Adherence to such virtues fosters a happy and healthy environment for all members of a community. For these reasons, being a loyal member of one’s group is widely regarded as good for both individuals and society. 2.1.2 Orientations Militant Internationalism Many countries and international leaders construct policy in order to have a positive impact on the world. Often, this requires military force on the part of one or both states. For example, countries sometimes need to use their military to stop another country from starting a war, or to intervene when there is no other way to solve a dispute. Scholars and diplomats alike recognize the benefits of using military force under certain conditions, with one prominent leader recently stating that “to say that force may sometimes be necessary... is recognition of history,” and is often essential for maintaining order in the world. Countries should use force when they are threatened or when it will produce a positive outcome, and military interventions in history have been successful ways for countries to resolve their differences. Thus, many agree that the use of military force abroad is a good foreign policy tool. Cooperative Internationalism Many countries and international leaders construct policy in order to have a positive impact on the world. Often, this involves working diplomatically with other nations and international organizations to provide aid or produce an acceptable solution to a pressing global problem. For example, states can come together to establish guidelines for the protection of human rights or to aid the eco7 nomic development of other nations. Scholars and diplomats alike recognize the benefits of such cooperation. Regarding the provision of aid for the building of new police stations abroad to cut down on international crime, one prominent leader recently remarked that “the solution of the most important problems require international cooperation, and this project is a testimony of what international cooperation for development can do.” Many others agree that it is good when countries have positive diplomatic relations with other countries and work with them and through international institutions to solve global problems. Isolationism Many countries and international leaders construct policy in order to have the most positive impact. Often, this involves focusing primarily on domestic issues, so that the country can primarily establish their own national prosperity. For example, a state might focus on improving its own economy rather than becoming entangled in agreements with or the affairs of other countries. Scholars and diplomats alike recognize the benefits of such an emphasis on domestic affairs. One prominent leader, for example, recently stressed that the United States should focus first on “our energy, economic, and domestic policies,” as they all advance the national interest. Many others agree that it is good when countries put their domestic priorities first and avoid entanglements abroad. 2.1.3 Policy Positions The Iraq War In 2003, the United States led a coalition in an invasion of Iraq due to fears that the government possessed weapons of mass destruction. In the time that has passed since the beginning of the war, many American citizens have come to see it as a positive intervention. The occupation of Iraq and activities by the U.S. has helped to bring prosperity to an otherwise faltering nation. The country has also become increasingly more stable over time. One prominent US leader noted this recently, stating that he “agrees with the invasion of Iraq” because it “was the right thing to do” and that all Americans should “desire to see signs of hope across Iraq flourish.” With time, then, we have come to know that this intervention was crucial in stopping the potential military threat from Iraq and the various human rights abuses under Saddam Hussein. There would have been substantially more violence and instability in the state without the intervention of the United States and other coalition forces. NATO Air strikes on Libya In early 2011, armed conflict emerged in Libya between supporters of the current leader Muam8 mar Gaddafi and rebels who sought to remove his government from power. In response, several countries, including the United States, engaged in military action in support of the rebels. One prominent US leader recently stated that this involvement was necessary, because “you had a potentially significantly destabilizing event taking place in Libya.” With time, we have come to know that this intervention was crucial in putting an end to the massacre of many Libyans by oppressive government forces. It helped to bring a swifter end to the violence, and many agree that it was a good decision. There would have been many more deaths and dangerous regional instability without the involvement of other states. Intervention in Syria In early 2011, armed conflict emerged in Syria between supporters of the current government led by President Bashar al-Assad and protesters who sought to remove his government from power. The government crackdown against the protestors and ongoing civil war has resulted in some 60,000 deaths. As a consequence, many prominent analysts and world leaders have called for the US to intervene in the conflict, by directly arming rebel soldiers or even sending their own troops, despite the disapproval voiced by fellow United Nations Security Council members China and Russia. One prominent leader and analyst, stresses this when he states that “we must certainly consider direct military action” by “[using] force ourselves, or see the Assad regime murder its way to victory.” With time, it has become evident to many that an intervention will be the only way to put an end to the violence. An intervention could prevent more deaths and dangerous regional instability. Kyoto Protocol In 1997, delegates from 194 countries met in Kyoto, Japan and crafted an agreement to reduce the amount of greenhouse gas emissions throughout the world. This agreement, the Kyoto Protocol, expired at the end of 2012, and countries have not yet established a new treaty to replace it. Many analysts and leaders argue that the United States should become heavily involved in negotiating a new treaty designed to curb greenhouse gas emissions and protect the global environment. One prominent leader in the United States stresses this point when he states that “we are ready, willing, and able” to have a discussion about establishing a new climate agreement. US support for and promotion of a new treaty will be crucial to its future success. Control: Online Retail Internet retail has emerged in recent years as a prominent mode of commerce. One of the most important ways that sellers increase the number of people interested in purchasing their product 9 is by earning positive customer reviews. These are easily accessible by individuals while they are shopping via the internet, and many people report using them to determine their ultimate purchase. As a result, the importance of online retail is only expected to increase. Time Magazine recently announced that online shopping was the holiday season’s top shopping trend, and $1.25 billion was spent online on Cyber Monday alone. 2.2 Instrumentation 2.2.1 Values: Moral Foundations Questionnaire We measure participants’ moral values using four of the moral values from the Moral Foundations Questionnaire (Graham et al., 2011). As is standard with the MFQ, each moral foundation is measured with six items, the first three of which are responses to the question “When you decide whether something is right or wrong, to what extent are the following considerations relevant to your thinking?” These items have 6 point Likert responses ranging from “not at all relevant” to “extremely relevant.” The second three items for each foundation are 6 point Likert responses from “Strongly disagree” to “Strongly agree.” Items are presented to participant in two blocks – one with the first 3 items from each scale and one with the latter 3 items from each scale – with the order of items randomized within blocks. Authority/Respect(α = 0.719) 1. Whether or not someone showed a lack of respect for authority 2. Whether or not someone conformed to the traditions of society 3. Whether or not an action caused chaos or disorder 4. Respect for authority is something all children need to learn. 5. Men and women each have different roles to play in society. 6. If I were a soldier and disagreed with my commanding officer’s orders, I would obey anyway because that is my duty. Fairness/Reciprocity (α = 0.666) 1. Whether or not some people were treated differently than others 2. Whether or not someone acted unfairly 3. Whether or not someone was denied his or her rights 10 4. When the government makes laws, the number one principle should be ensuring that everyone is treated fairly. 5. Justice is the most important requirement for a society. 6. I think it’s morally wrong that rich children inherit a lot of money while poor children inherit nothing. Harm/Care (α = 0.710) 1. Whether or not someone suffered emotionally 2. Whether or not someone cared for someone weak or vulnerable 3. Whether or not someone was cruel 4. Compassion for those who are suffering is the most crucial virtue. 5. One of the worst things a person could do is hurt a defenseless animal. 6. It can never be right to kill a human being. Ingroup/Loyalty (α = 0.727) 1. Whether or not someone’s action showed love for his or her country 2. Whether or not someone did something to betray his or her group 3. Whether or not someone showed a lack of loyalty 4. I am proud of my country’s history. 5. People should be loyal to their family members, even when they have done something wrong. 6. It is more important to be a team player than to express oneself. 11 2.2.2 Orientations Each item has a seven-point Likert response scale ranging from “Strongly disagree” to ”Strongly agree”. Items for each orientation are presented together in a block, with block order randomized. Militant internationalism (α = 0.783) 1. The United States should take all steps including the use of force to prevent aggression by any expansionist power 2. Rather than simply countering our opponents’ thrusts, it is necessary to strike at the heart of an opponent’s power. 3. Going to war is unfortunate but sometimes the only solution to international problems. 4. In dealing with other nations our government should be strong and tough. Cooperative internationalism (α = 0.789) 1. The United States needs to cooperate more with the United Nations. 2. It is essential for the United States to work with other nations to solve problems such as overpopulation, hunger, and pollution. 3. In dealing with other nations our government should be understanding and flexible. 4. The best way to ensure peace is to sit down with other countries and work out our disagreements. Isolationism (α = 0.814) 1. The U.S. should mind its own business internationally and let other countries get along the best they can on their own. 2. We should not think so much in international terms but concentrate more on our own national problems. 3. The U.S. needs to play an active role in solving conflicts around the world. (Reverse-coded) 4. America’s conception of its leadership role in the world must be scaled down. 12 2.2.3 Policy Positions The Iraq War (α = 0.788) 1. All things considered, do you approve or disapprove of the U.S. decision to intervene in Iraq? (7-point scale from “Strongly disapprove” to “Strongly approve”) 2. Considering the costs to the United States versus the benefits to the United States, do you think the war in Iraq was worth fighting, or not? (5-point scale from “Not worth it” to “Worth it”) NATO Air Strikes on Libya (α = 0.764) 1. All things considered, do you approve or disapprove of the decision of the U.S. and its allies to conduct military air strikes in Libya? (7-point scale from “Strongly disapprove” to “Strongly approve”) 2. Do you think the U.S. did the right thing by using military force in Libya now, or should the US not have been involved in Libya? (4-point scale from “Definitely the wrong thing” to “Definitely the right thing”) Intervention in Syria (α = 0.742) 1. Would you favor or oppose the U.S. and its allies sending arms and military supplies to antigovernment groups in Syria? (7-point scale from “Strongly oppose” to “Strongly favor”) 2. Would you approve or disapprove of the use of U.S. forces to protect the anti-government protesters in Syria? (7-point scale from “Strongly disapprove” to “Strongly approve”) Kyoto Protocol (α = 0.827) 1. The Kyoto Protocol, the global treaty designed to curb greenhouse gas emissions, expired in 2012. Do you approve or disapprove of the United States working closely with other nations to create a new international treaty to fight global warming? (7-point scale from “Strongly disapprove” to “Strongly approve”) 2. How much do you think the U.S. government should do about global warming? (5-point scale from “Nothing” to “A great deal”) 13 3 Scale Reliabilities Table 2: Scale reliabilities Scale Cronbach’s α Fairness α = 0.666 Authority α = 0.719 Harm α = 0.710 In-group α = 0.727 Militant internationalism α = 0.783 Cooperative internationalism α = 0.789 Isolationism α = 0.814 Iraq war α = 0.788 Libya α = 0.764 Syria α = 0.742 Kyoto α = 0.827 Foreign policy interest α = 0.907 Political knowledge α = 0.684 14 4 Thresholding and sensitivity analyses A central question in network analysis is how to define the network’s edges, particularly when some types of relations are less easily dichotomized than others, and thus have more ambiguous criteria as to what constitutes an edge. Typically network analysts make two choices: either include all potential edges and simply weight them by their strength (Thomas and Blitzstein, 2011), or threshold based a particular cut point. As illustrated in Figure 2 below, in the main analyses in the text, we employ a cut point of 10%, taking the top decile of treatment effects in our experimental results (shaded in the darker grey) and using them to generate our networks. We represent our treatment effects with t-statistics, since they capture both the difference in means between the treatment and control groups and also their sample sizes and standard deviations, but a variety of other statistics could be used — the choice of which depends on the nature of the analyses.2 In order to ensure that the patterns of results we present above are not simply artifacts of this 10% cut point, we conduct a series of sensitivity analyses below probing the robustness of our two main findings (the prevalence of bottom-up constraint, and the network density’s curvilinear relationship with political knowledge and interest) to our thresholding criteria. In the sensitivity analyses, we generated six different versions of the network, defining the edging threshold as the top 1%, 5%, 10%, 15%, 20%, and top 25% of the treatment effects, respectively. As one would expect, the more restrictive the threshold, the fewer edges are included in the adjacency matrix, and the less dense the networks, while the less restrictive the threshold, the more edges are included in the adjacency matrix, and the more dense the networks. Figure 3 plots the total degree centrality, indegree centrality, and outdegree centrality scores for core values (depicted as circles) and policy attitudes (depicted as triangles) across all of the attitude networks generated at each effect threshold.3 If our networks are characterized by bottom-up constraint, we should see that policy attitudes consistently play a larger role in our networks than core values — that is, the triangles should be higher than the circles. If, however, our networks are characterized by top-down constraint, we should see that core values consistently play a larger role in our networks than core values — that is, the circles should be higher than the triangles. As the three panels of Figure 3 make clear, although the overall centrality scores for both core values and policy attitudes increase as less restrictive effect thresholds are employed, the centrality scores for policy attitudes are always higher 2 W statistics from nonparametric rank sum tests, for example, are a function of sample size, so simply thresholding the network based on the effects with the largest W statistics would be inappropriate when the effects are drawn from samples of different sizes. Similarly, if analysts were interested in employing local fdr or FDR to control the false discovery rate, the statistics in the distribution should be z statistics and p values from t-tests, respectively. 3 The analyses add across all of the different networks we generate in the piece (the network for the full sample, the liberal and conservative networks, the low, medium and high knowledge networks, etc.), but the results also hold when the subsample networks are dropped and only the full network is considered. 15 0.2 0.0 0.1 Density 0.3 0.4 Figure 2: Distribution of effect sizes -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 Effect size (t statistics) The histogram plots the distribution of effect sizes, represented here using t-statistics. The largest 10% of the treatment effects are shaded, indicating those effects for which edges are drawn in the attitude networks. Other thresholds can also be applied, but the key point is that significance is inferred not in reference to particular parametric assumptions or assumed populations, but rather in relation to the observed data itself. than for core values – and in this sense, we find strong support for bottom-up constraint regardless of the effect threshold used in the analyses. Figure 4 probes the robustness of the other main finding from the piece: the curvilinear relationship that knowledge and political interest have with attitude network density. In the main analyses in the text that use a 10% effect threshold, we find that medium knowledge respondents displayed the densest attitude networks compared to their low- and high-knowledge peers, while medium-interest respondents displayed less dense attitude networks compared to their low- and high-interest counterparts. As in the analyses reported in Figure 3, we generated six different versions of each network (using a 1%, 5%, 10%, 15%, 20% and 25% effect threshold), this time focusing on the low, medium, and high political knowledge networks and the low, medium and high political interest networks. As Figure 4(a) shows, as long as the effect threshold is greater than 1% (whereupon all three knowledge networks have an equally low density), we replicate the same findings 16 detected with a 10% threshold, with the medium knowledge network displaying a greater number of edge than either the low or the high knowledge networks. Similarly, Figure 4(b) shows that as long as the effect threshold is greater than 1%, we see an inverse-U shaped relationship between foreign policy interest and network density. Together, Figures 3 and 4 show that even as the individual edges included in these networks change as the edge threshold is tightened or relaxed, the network-level properties (the prevalence of bottom-up constraint, and the curvilinear relationships between network density and political knowledge and interest) persist, mitigating concern about the effect of measurement error. 1% 5% 10% 15% Effect threshold 20% 25% 100 80 60 Outdegree Centrality 0 20 40 100 80 60 0 0 20 40 Indegree centrality 200 150 100 50 Degree centrality 120 Figure 3: Sensitivity analyses: relative centrality of core values versus policy orientations 1% 5% 10% 15% Effect threshold 20% 25% 1% 5% 10% 15% 20% 25% Effect threshold The sensitivity analyses show robust evidence for bottom-up constraint: core values (depicted by circles) consistently have lower centrality scores than specific policy attitudes (depicted by triangles). 5 5.1 Supplementary Subgroup Analyses Gender The main text presents an analysis of the full network along with networks separated by political ideology and sophistication. However, these are not the only dimensions of individual difference upon which we expect structural variance. An individual’s gender, too, might impact the type and direction of foreign policy attitude constraint. Some previous scholarship has shown that the foreign policy attitudes of men and women occasionally diverge in limited ways (such as with regard to defense spending) but can be captured by the same dimensions (Holsti and Rosenau, 1981) or are determined by the same hierarchically constraining elements (Fite, Genest and Wilcox, 1990). 17 Figure 4: Sensitivity analyses: curvilinear effect of knowledge and interest on network density (b) Interest in politics 0.25 (a) Political knowledge 25% 0.20 0.3 25% 20% 0.15 15% 10% 0.10 Density 15% 0.2 Density 20% 10% 0.05 0.1 5% 5% 0.0 Low Medium 1% 0.00 1% High Knowledge Low Medium Foreign Policy Interest The sensitivity analyses show consistent evidence for quadratic effects of political knowledge (panel a) and interest (panel b) on network density once the effect threshold is above 1%; effect thresholds are labeled on the right-hand side of each panel. Other work in psychology, however, suggests that women and men develop and rely on different sets of values to make judgments (Gilligan and Attanucci, 1988; Jaffee and Shibley Hyde, 2009). Thus, Figure 5 disaggregates the foreign policy networks for women and men, respectively. We find that the Female network is driven largely by attitudes about the airstrikes in Libya, and more generally that policy positions serve as the most important sources of constraint. Attitudes toward Libya influence two core values as well as positions on Syria, and the nodes representing attitudes toward the war in Iraq, Syria, and the Kyoto Protocol each have outgoing connections. The permuted analyses confirm the importance of specific policy positions to the female network, demonstrating that there are more outgoing edges than we would otherwise expect (p = 0.014). The Male network, in contrast, has more evenly distributed connections — values, policies, and orientations are about equally central, suggesting that specific policy events may be more important sources of constraint for women than for men. These distinctions may help to explain why the gender gap in foreign policy attitudes seems to fluctuate based on the issue and time, with men and women coming to their states attitudes via different pathways (Holsti, 2004). Additionally, we see patterns that are only evident from a systemic perspective, and that challenge the assumptions implicit in previous work: attitudes at the same level of analysis influence each other (as with harm and authority for 18 High men, or the reciprocal connection between Libya and Syria for women), and we have already noted that constraint in the female network works primarily from the bottom up. Figure 5: Foreign policy attitude network by gender (a) Female (b) Male MI Fair Kyoto Iraq Fair Syria Ingroup Libya Auth Harm Harm Libya Iso CI Ingroup Iraq Auth Kyoto Syria Iso MI CI Note: Arrows represent the patterns of attitude constraint; nodes are scaled by their degree centrality, so larger nodes are connected to a higher number of other attitudes. Core values are represented as circles, foreign policy orientations as diamonds, and policy attitudes as triangles. Neither network conforms with patterns expected by either the horizontal or the vertical models: attitudes at the same level of abstraction influence one another (e.g. harm and authority, for men), and we see signs of bottom-up constraint (e.g. Libya and ingroup, for women). 19 5.2 Political Ideology The psychological literature on political ideology tells us that liberals and conservatives believe in fundamentally different things (e.g. they rely on different moral foundations — Graham, Haidt and Nosek 2009) for fundamentally different reasons (e.g. they maintain their belief systems in response to strikingly different social-cognitive motivations — Jost et al. 2003). In contrast to “one-size fits all” models of attitude structure, we thus expect that liberals and conservatives would organize their attitudes differently. As shown by Figure 6, liberals and conservatives maintain distinct foreign policy attitude networks, with CI central to the conservative network, and isolationism situated most centrally in the liberal network. Whereas attitudes toward the airstrikes in Libya, renewing the Kyoto Protocol, and intervening in Syria constrain CI for conservatives, they influence isolationism for liberals. One thing that the liberal and conservative networks share, then, is the prominence of bottom-up patterns of constraint going from policy positions to general orientations: for conservatives, for example, values shape fewer attitudes (p = 0.097) and policies shape more (p = 0.062) than expected by chance. The directions of the observed links, along with the existence of links between orientations such as CI and isolationism for liberals, are inconsistent with the directional assumptions from past work. Interestingly, while the structural patterns of constraint clearly differ in these networks, their overall topology is relatively similar, and Hamming distance scores suggest that these networks are the least distinct from one another of all of the subgroup networks presented here — indicating perhaps that other individual level characteristics, such as political sophistication, are more important for understanding the underlying variation in foreign policy attitude networks. 6 Additional permutation tests The main analyses employ permutation tests that examine the probability of particular node-level attributes by randomly generating 10000 networks of an identical size and density as our observed network, thereby observing the probability that, for example, core values would display a particular level of constraint due to chance alone.4 While conditioning the permutation test on observed network densities allows us to determine the probability of given node-level statistics, it does not let us make inferences about the probability of observing given densities themselves. Thus, Figure 7 displays the results from a permutation test to evaluate the density of each network, randomly generating 99000 networks of an identical size of each observed network, in which the probability of each edge forming is varied from 0.01 to 0.99 every 1000 permutations. This technique is used 4 As Butts (2008, fn. 5) notes, this type of permutation test is popular amongst network analysts, although the terminology used to describe the test often varies: Butts refers to them as Conditional Uniform Graph Tests. 20 Figure 6: Foreign policy attitude network by political ideology (a) Liberals (b) Conservatives Iraq Ingroup Iso Libya Syria Kyoto Kyoto MI Harm Iso Libya CI Fair CI Syria Iraq Fair Harm MI Auth Auth Ingroup Note: Arrows depict the thresholded treatment effects, and thus, patterns of attitude constraint; nodes are scaled by their degree centrality. Core values are represented as circles, foreign policy orientations as diamonds, and policy attitudes as triangles. Thus, we see that liberals and conservatives display different patterns of attitude constraint — for example, CI shapes isolationism for liberals, but isolationism shapes CI for conservatives — but once again, both attitude networks show considerable bottom-up patterns of constraint, as specific policy attitudes shape foreign policy orientations and core values. to test two different null hypotheses: a random null hypothesis in which each edge has an identical probability of forming, and an informative null hypothesis in which the edge probabilities are constrained to 0 according to the assumptions of the hierarchical model – that is, all constraint is top-down rather than bottom-up or within-levels. Thus, Panel (a) of Figure 7 shows that against a random null hypothesis, we are unlikely to see the observed density for our full network if the probability of any given edge forming is greater than 0.1. Similarly, Panel (b) shows that against a hierarchical null hypothesis, we are unlikely to see the observed density for our full network if the probability of any given edge forming is less than 0.1 or greater than 0.3. 21 22 Network 11. H Knowledge 10. M Knowledge 09. L Knowledge 08. H Interest 07. M Interest 06. L Interest 05. Female 04. Male 03. Conservative 02. Liberal 01. Full 11. H Knowledge 10. M Knowledge 09. L Knowledge 08. H Interest 07. M Interest 06. L Interest 05. Female 04. Male 03. Conservative 02. Liberal 01. Full 0.0 0.0 0.1 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.3 0.3 0.5 Edge probability 0.6 0.4 0.5 Edge probability 0.6 (b) Informative (hierarchical) null 0.4 (a) Random null 0.7 0.7 Figure 7: Permutation tests of network density 0.8 0.8 0.9 0.9 1.0 1.0 0.1 0.05 0.5 p value 0.1 0.05 0.5 p value the expected probability that we would see networks of the observed density for each edge probability. commensurate with top-down patterns of constraint (e.g. all edges from policy attitudes to core values, for example, are constrained at 0). These heatmaps thus plot corresponding to the predictions from a hierarchical model, in which each potential edge is drawn with the probability denoted on the x-axis only if the edge is also each potential edge is drawn with the probability denoted on the x-axis, while the bottom panel shows the results from an informative null hypothesis the probability of each edge forming varies from 0.01 to 0.99 every 1000 permutations. The top panel shows the results from a random null hypothesis in which The figures plot the results of permutation tests that compare each observed network density with 99000 randomly generated networks of identical size, in which Network References Bardes, Barbara and Robert Oldendick. 1978. “Beyond Internationalism: A Case for Multiple Dimensions in the Structure of Foreign Policy Attitudes.” Social Science Quarterly 59(3):496–508. Bartels, Larry M. 1994. “The American public’s defense spending preferences in the post-cold war era.” Public Opinion Quarterly 58(4):479–508. Bennett, Stephen Earl. 1974. “Attitude structures and foreign policy opinions.” Social Science Quarterly 55(3):732–742. Binning, Kevin R. 2007. ““It’s Us Against the World”: How Distrust in Americans versus People-InGeneral Shapes Competitive Foreign Policy Preferences.” Political Psychology 28(6):777–799. Bjereld, Ulf and Ann-Marie Ekengren. 1999. “Foreign policy dimensions: A comparison between the United States and Sweden.” International Studies Quarterly 43(3):503–518. Brewer, Paul R, Kimberly Gross, Sean Aday and Lars Willnat. 2004. “International trust and public opinion about world affairs.” American Journal of Political Science 48(1):93–109. Brewer, Paul R and Marco R Steenbergen. 2002. “All against all: How beliefs about human nature shape foreign policy opinions.” Political Psychology 23(1):39–58. Brody, Richard A and Sidney Verba. 1972. “Hawk and dove: the search for an explanation of Vietnam policy preferences.” Acta Politica 7:285–322. Butts, Carter T. 2008. “Social Network Analysis: A Methodological Introduction.” Asian Journal of Social Psychology 11:13–41. Caspary, William R. 1970. “The “mood theory”: A study of public opinion and foreign policy.” The American Political Science Review 64(2):536–547. Chanley, Virginia A. 1999. “US Public Views of International Involvement from 1964 to 1993 TimeSeries Analyses of General and Militant Internationalism.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 43(1):23– 44. Chittick, William O. and Keith R. Billingsley. 1989. “The Structure of Elite Foreign Policy Beliefs.” The Western Political Quarterly 42(2):201–224. Chittick, William O, Keith R Billingsley and Rick Travis. 1990. “Persistence and change in elite and mass attitudes toward US foreign policy.” Political psychology pp. 385–401. Chittick, William O, Keith R Billingsley and Rick Travis. 1995. “A three-dimensional model of American foreign policy beliefs.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 313–331. Cohrs, J Christopher and Barbara Moschner. 2002. “Antiwar knowledge and generalized political attitudes as determinants of attitude toward the Kosovo War.” Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology 8(2):139–155. Cohrs, J Christopher, Barbara Moschner, Jurgen Maes and Sven Kielmann. 2005. “Personal values and attitudes toward war.” Peace and Conflict 11(3):293–312. Cohrs, J Christopher, Jürgen Maes, Barbara Moschner and Sven Kielmann. 2007. “Determinants of human rights attitudes and behavior: A comparison and integration of psychological perspectives.” Political Psychology 28(4):441–469. Federico, Christopher M, Agnieszka Golec and Jessica L Dial. 2005. “The relationship between the need for closure and support for military action against Iraq: Moderating effects of national attachment.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31(5):621–632. Fite, David, Marc Genest and Clyde Wilcox. 1990. “Gender Differences in Foreign Policy Attitudes A Longitudinal Analysis.” American Politics Research 18(4):492–513. Gilligan, C. and J. Attanucci. 1988. “Two Moral Orientations: Gender Differences and Similarities.” 23 Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33:223–237. Graham, Jesse, Brian A. Nosek, Jonathan Haidt, Ravi Iyer, Spassena Koleva and Peter H. Ditto. 2011. “Mapping the Moral Domain.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101(2):366–385. Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt and Brian A. Nosek. 2009. “Liberals and Conservatives Rely on Different Sets of Moral Foundations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96(5):1029– 1046. Handcock, Mark S., Adrian E. Raftery and Jeremy M. Tantrum. 2007. “Model-based clustering for social networks.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A 170(2):301–354. Herrmann, Richard K and Jonathan W Keller. 2004. “Beliefs, values, and strategic choice: US leaders’ decisions to engage, contain, and use force in an era of globalization.” Journal of Politics 66(2):557–580. Herron, Kerry G and Hank C Jenkins-Smith. 2002. “US perceptions of nuclear security in the wake of the Cold War: Comparing public and elite belief systems.” International Studies Quarterly 46(4):451–479. Hinckley, Ronald H. 1988. “Public attitudes toward key foreign policy events.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 32(2):295–318. Hinckley, Ronald H. 1992. People, polls, and policymakers: American public opinion and national security. Free Press. Holsti, Ole R. 1979. “The three-headed eagle: The United States and system change.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 339–359. Holsti, Ole R. 1992. “Public opinion and foreign policy: Challenges to the Almond-Lippmann Consensus Mershon Series: Research programs and debates.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 439– 466. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1976. “Meaning of Vietnam: Belief Systems of American Leaders, The.” International Journal 32:452. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1979. “Vietnam, consensus, and the belief systems of American leaders.” World Politics 32(01):1–56. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1981. “The foreign policy beliefs of women in leadership positions.” The Journal of Politics 43(02):326–347. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1984. American leadership in world affairs: Vietnam and the breakdown of consensus. Allen & Unwin Boston. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1986. “Consensus Lost. Consensus Regained?: Foreign Policy Beliefs of American Leaders, 1976-1980.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 375–409. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1988. “The domestic and foreign policy beliefs of American leaders.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 32(2):248–294. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1990. “The structure of foreign policy attitudes among American leaders.” The Journal of Politics 52(01):94–125. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1993. “The structure of foreign policy beliefs among American opinion leaders-after the cold war.” Millennium-Journal of International Studies 22(2):235–278. Holsti, Ole R and James N Rosenau. 1996. “Liberals, populists, libertarians, and conservatives: The link between domestic and international affairs.” International Political Science Review 17(1):29– 54. Holsti, Ole Rudolf. 2004. Public opinion and American foreign policy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley. 1987a. “How are foreign policy attitudes structured? A hierarchical 24 model.” The American Political Science Review pp. 1099–1120. Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley. 1987b. “The means and ends of foreign policy as determinants of presidential support.” American Journal of Political Science pp. 236–258. Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Peffley. 1990. “Public images of the Soviet Union: The impact on foreign policy attitudes.” The Journal of Politics 52(01):3–28. Hurwitz, Jon, Mark Peffley and Mitchell A Seligson. 1993. “Foreign policy belief systems in comparative perspective: The United States and Costa Rica.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 245–270. Jaffee, Sara and Janet Shibley Hyde. 2009. “Gender Differences in Moral Orientation: A MetaAnalysis.” Psychological Bulletin 126:703–726. Jenkins-Smith, Hank C., Neil J. Mitchell and Kerry G. Herron. 2004. “Foreign and Domestic Policy Belief Structures in the U.S. and British Publics.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 48(3):287–309. Jost, John T., Jack Glaser, Arie W. Kruglanski and Frank Sulloway. 2003. “Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition.” Psychological Bulletin 129(3):339–375. Krivitsky, Pavel N. and Mark S. Handcock. 2008. “Fitting Position Latent Cluster Models for Social Networks with latentnet.” Journal of Statistical Software 24(5). Liberman, Peter. 2006. “An eye for an eye: Public support for war against evildoers.” International Organization 60(3):687. Liberman, Peter. 2007. “Punitiveness and US elite support for the 1991 Persian Gulf War.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 51(1):3–32. Liberman, Peter. 2013. “Retributive support for international punishment and torture.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 57(2):285–306. Maggiotto, Michael A and Eugene R Wittkopf. 1981. “American public attitudes toward foreign policy.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 601–631. Mandelbaum, M. and W. Schneider. 1979. The New Internationalisms. In Eagle Entangled: U.S. Foreign Policy in a Complex World, ed. K.A. Oye. New York: Longman. Mandelbaum, Michael and William Schneider. 1978. “The new internationalisms.” International Security pp. 81–98. Mayton, Daniel M. II, Danya J Peters and Rocky W Owens. 1999. “Values, militarism, and nonviolent predispositions.” Peace and Conflict 5(1):69–77. Modigliani, Andre. 1972. “Hawks and doves, isolationism and political distrust: An analysis of public opinion on military policy.” The American Political Science Review 66(3):960–978. Murray, Shoon Kathleen. 1996. Anchors against change: American opinion leaders’ beliefs after the cold war. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. Murray, Shoon Kathleen, Jonathan A Cowden and Bruce M Russett. 1999. “The convergence of American elites’ domestic beliefs with their foreign policy beliefs.” International Interactions 25(2):153–180. Nie, Norman H and Kristi Andersen. 1974. “Mass belief systems revisited: Political change and attitude structure.” The Journal of Politics 36(3):540–591. Oldendick, Robert and Barbara Ann Bardes. 1981. “Belief Structures and Foreign Policy: Comparing Dimensions of Elite and Mass Opinions.” Social Science Quarterly 62(3):432–442. Oldendick, Robert W and Barbara Ann Bardes. 1982. “Mass and elite foreign policy opinions.” Public Opinion Quarterly 46(3):368–382. Page, Benjamin I and Robert Y Shapiro. 2010. The rational public: Fifty years of trends in Americans’ policy preferences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Patchen, Martin. 1970. “Social class and dimensions of foreign policy attitudes.” Social Science Quar25 terly 51(4):649–667. Peffley, Mark A. and Jon Hurwitz. 1985. “A Hierarchical Model of Attitude Constraint.” American Journal of Political Science 29(4):871–890. Peffley, Mark and Jon Hurwitz. 1992. “International Events and Foreign Policy Beliefs: Public Response to Changing Soviet-U.S. Relations.” American Journal of Political Science 36(2):431–461. Peffley, Mark and Jon Hurwitz. 1993. “Models of attitude constraint in foreign affairs.” Political Behavior 15(1):61–90. Rathbun, Brian C. 2007. “Hierarchy and Community at Home and Abroad Evidence of a Common Structure of Domestic and Foreign Policy Beliefs in American Elites.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 51(3):379–407. Reifler, Jason, Thomas J Scotto and Harold D Clarke. 2011. “Foreign Policy Beliefs in Contemporary Britain: Structure and Relevance1.” International Studies Quarterly 55(1):245–266. Richman, Alvin. 1996. “Trends: American support for international involvement: General and specific components of post-Cold War changes.” The Public Opinion Quarterly 60(2):305–321. Richman, Alvin, Eloise Malone and David B Nolle. 1997. “Testing foreign policy belief structures of the American public in the post-cold war period: Gross validations from two national surveys.” Political Research Quarterly 50(4):939–955. Rosati, Jerel A, Michael W Link and John Creed. 1998. “A New Perspective on the Foreign Policy Views of American Opinion Leaders in the Cold War and Post-Cold War Eras.” Political Research Quarterly 51(2):461–479. Rosati, Jerel and John Creed. 1997. “Extending the Three-and Four-Headed Eagles: The Foreign Policy Orientations of American Elites During the 80s and 90s.” Political Psychology 18(3):583– 623. Rosenau, James N and Ole R Holsti. 1983. “US leadership in a shrinking world: The breakdown of consensuses and the emergence of conflicting belief systems.” World Politics 35(3):368–392. Russett, Bruce M and Elizabeth C Hanson. 1975. Interest and ideology: The foreign policy beliefs of American businessmen. WH Freeman San Francisco. Russett, Bruce M et al. 1990. Controlling the sword: The democratic governance of national security. Harvard University Press Cambridge, MA. Russett, Bruce, Thomas Hartley and Shoon Murray. 1994. “The end of the Cold War, attitude change, and the politics of defense spending.” PS: Political Science and Politics 27(1):17–21. Sulfaro, Valerie A. 1996. “The role of ideology and political sophistication in the structure of foreign policy attitudes.” American Politics Research 24(3):303–337. Thomas, A. C. and J. K. Blitzstein. 2011. “Valued Ties Tell Fewer Lies: Why Not To Dichotomize Network Edges With Thresholds.” ArXiv e-prints . URL: http://adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2011arXiv1101.0788T Vail, Kenneth E., III and Matt Motyl. 2010. “Support for Diplomacy: Peacemaking and Militarism as a Unidimensional Correlate of Social, Environmental, and Political Attitudes.” Peace and Conflict 16(1):29–57. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1981. “The Structure of Foreign Policy Attitudes: An Alternate View.” Social Science Quarterly 62(1):108–23. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1986. “On the foreign policy beliefs of the American people: A critique and some evidence.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 425–445. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1987. “Elites and masses: Another look at attitudes toward America’s world role.” International Studies Quarterly pp. 131–159. 26 Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1990. Faces of internationalism: Public Opinion and American foreign policy. Duke University Press. Wittkopf, Eugene R. 1994. “Faces of internationalism in a transitional environment.” Journal of Conflict Resolution 38(3):376–401. Wittkopf, Eugene R and Michael A Maggiotto. 1983a. “Elites and masses: A comparative analysis of attitudes toward America’s world role.” Journal of Politics 45(2):303–334. Wittkopf, Eugene R and Michael A Maggiotto. 1983b. “The Two Faces of Internationalism: Public Attitudes toward American Foreign Policy in the 1970s–and Beyond?.” Social Science Quarterly 64(2):288–304. Ziegler, Andrew H. 1987. “The structure of western European attitudes towards Atlantic cooperation: Implications for the western alliance.” British Journal of Political Science 17(04):457– 477. 27