ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Stage Left, Stage Right? Position Effects on Perception of Spokesperson Nancy Puccinelli, Suffolk Business School Linda Tickle-Degnen, Boston University Robert Rosenthal, University of California Riverside Stage Left, Stage Right? Position Effects on Perception of a Spokesperson NANCY M. PUCCINELLI Suffolk Business School LINDA TICKLE-DEGNEN Boston University ROBERT ROSENTHAL University of California, RiversideDoes it matter where a spokesperson stands to display a product? It is suggested that the specialization of the right brain for person perception leads observers to perceive targets positioned to the left as more instrumental. Consistent with the hypotheses, targets positioned to the left were seen to guide an interaction more especially if shown with a partner, during an externally-focused puzzle task or when observed by men (experiments 1 and 2). Further, this effect was replicated for observers’ willingness to pay and suggests recall of visual information as a mediator of the target position effect (experiment 3). [to cite]: Nancy Puccinelli, Linda Tickle-Degnen, and Robert Rosenthal (2006) ,"Stage Left, Stage Right? Position Effects on Perception of Spokesperson", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 33, eds. Connie Pechmann and Linda Price, Duluth, MN : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 576-577. [url]: http://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/12301/volumes/v33/NA-33 [copyright notice]: This work is copyrighted by The Association for Consumer Research. For permission to copy or use this work in whole or in part, please contact the Copyright Clearance Center at http://www.copyright.com/. Stage Left, Stage Right? Position Effects on Perception of a Spokesperson Nancy M. Puccinelli, Suffolk University Linda Tickle-Degnen, Boston University Robert T. Rosenthal, University of California, Riverside EXTENDED ABSTRACT Experiment 2 also examined the moderating role of gender. Imagine that as you read reviews of a celebrity’s TV appearances, you are struck by how the reviews differ. On The Tonight Show the celebrity is seen as a leader, steering the interview. However, on Larry King Live she is seen as passive and as if bullied by Larry King. Your first inclination is to attribute the effect to the respective hosts, however, might the differences be accounted for by the position of the celebrity -on the Tonight Show the celebrity appears to the left of Jay Leno and on Larry King Live to the right? The present research sought to investigate this question. It is proposed that the specialization of the right hemisphere in processing faces (Gazzaniga 2000) and nonverbal behavior (Benowitz et al. 1983) mediates the effect of target position on observer perception. When an observer’s attention is drawn to the left visual field, person perception processing in the right hemisphere is activated and leads to greater recall of visual information of the target (Taylor and Fiske 1975) and perception of the target as more instrumental in the interaction. Experiments 1 and 2 Experiment 1 Method. Forty-eight participants were randomly assigned to sit on the left or the right while engaging in a puzzle (time 1) or discussion (time 2). Thirty-two observers rated 15second clips from each of the interactions on 38 behavior variables ranging from “Friendly” to “Similarity in movement” which were combined to form Critical, Dominant, Interpersonally Skilled, Polite, and Tempo Setting. Experiment 2 Method. To control for any position effects due to differences in target behavior, 29 observers in experiment 2 rated the mirror image of the target videos shown in experiment 1. Given the similarities across experiments 1 and 2 a 2 (target position: left or right) x 2 (target task: puzzle or discussion) x 2 (partner presence: alone or with partner) x 2 (experiment: 1 or 2) repeated-measures ANOVA was calculated using target pair as the unit of analysis (Rosenthal 1991). H1: Targets positioned to the observer’s left will be seen as more instrumental in the interaction compared to targets positioned to the right. H2: The effect of target position will be stronger when a target is viewed with a partner than when the partner is obscured. H3: The effect of target position will be stronger when a target is viewed during an externally-focused, puzzle compared to an internally-focused, discussion interaction. Consistent with the hypotheses, ratings were higher for targets on the left for Tempo Setting (p <.001), Interpersonally Skilled (p <.001), Critical (p <.001), and Dominant (p <.001) (H1) and this effect was stronger when the target was viewed with a partner for Tempo Setting (p <.001), Critical (p <.01), and Interpersonally Skilled (p <.05)(H2) and during an externally-focused puzzle interaction for Tempo Setting (p <.01), Critical (p <.01), and Dominant (p <.01)(H3). H4: The effect of target position will be stronger for male observers than female observers. Consistent with H4, when targets were observed by men the target position effect was stronger for Tempo Setting (p <.001) and Interpersonally Skilled (p <.01). In sum, targets positioned to the left were seen as more instrumental in the interaction as suggested by higher ratings on Tempo Setting, Critical, Interpersonally Skilled and Dominant. Further, it appears that this effect was moderated by presence of a partner, type of task, and observer gender. Experiment 3 In experiment 3, it is suggested that observers will be more persuaded by a target on the left and sought to explore recall as a mediator of observer judgment. H5: Observers will indicate greater willingness to pay for products endorsed by targets positioned to the left. H6: Observers will judge targets positioned to the left as more convincing than targets positioned to the right. H7: Observers will recall more visual information about the target when the target is positioned to the left than when the target is positioned to the right. The materials were virtually identical to those used in experiments 1 and 2. Sixteen participants viewed each of the videoclips and indicated the persuasiveness of the target as well as his/her memory for target attributes that contributed to their judgment (Fiske et al. 1982). Consistent with H5, participants indicated a greater Willingness to Pay for products when the target was positioned to the left (p <.001). However, contrary to H6, targets positioned to the right were judged as more Convincing (p<.001). Consistent with H7, more neutral and negative information was recalled for targets positioned to the left (p <.001 and p <.01, respectively), however, more positive information was recalled for targets positioned to the right (p<.001). Moreover, more positive, neutral, and negative information was recalled about targets positioned to the left when the observer was male as compared to when the observer was female (p <.001; p <.001 and p <.001, respectively). Thus, it appears that the results from experiments 1 and 2 extend to observer judgments of Willingness to Pay. Interestingly, the results for judgments of Convincing appear moderated by gender. While men show no effect of target position on judgments of Convincing, women do. Moreover, women tend to judge the target to the right as more convincing. This may be related to the differential recall observed and valence-specific laterality in women (Rodway et al. 2003). In conclusion, this study finds strong evidence for an effect of target position on the impression formed of a target individual and suggests a hemispheric specialization and recall interpretation of this result. The implications of such a result are both important and 576 Advances in Consumer Research Volume 33, © 2006 Advances in Consumer Research (Volume 33) / 577 far-reaching, opening a new window of insight into the complexity of consumer perception with applications to perceptions of spokespeople in advertising and salespeople in service settings. References Ambady, Nalini and Robert Rosenthal (1992), “Thin Slices of Expressive Behavior as Predictors of Interpersonal Consequences: A Meta-Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin, 111 (2), 256-274. Benowitz, Larry I., David M. Bear, Robert Rosenthal, Marsel M. Mesulam, Eran Zaidel, and Roger W. Sperry (1983), “Hemispheric Specialization in Nonverbal Communication,” Cortex, 19 (1), 5-11. Bernieri, Frank J., John S. Gillis, Janet M. Davis, and Jon E. Grahe (1996), “Dyad Rapport and the Accuracy of Its Judgment across Situations: A Lens Model Analysis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (1), 11029. Bowers, Dawn, Russell M. Bauer, and Kenneth M. Heilman (1993), “The Nonverbal Affect Lexicon: Theoretical Perspectives from Neuropsychological Studies of Affect Perception. Special Section: Neuropsychological Perspectives on Components of Emotional Processing,” Neuropsychology, 7 (4), 433-444. Brunswik, Egon (1956), Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Cronbach, Lee J. (1951), “Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests,” Psychometrika, 16 (297-334). Ellis, Andrew W. and Diane Miller (1981), “Left and Wrong in Adverts: Neuropsychological Correlates of Aesthetic Preference,” British Journal of Psychology, 72 (2), 225-229. Fiske, Susan T., David A. Kenny, and Shelley E. Taylor (1982), “Structural Models for the Mediation of Salience Effects on Attribution,” Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 18 (2), 105-127. Fitzsimons, Gavan J., J. Wesley Hutchinson, Patti Williams, Joseph W. Alba, Tanya L. Chartrand, Joel Huber, Frank Kardes, Geeta Menon, Priya Raghubir, J. Edward Russo, Baba Shiv, and Nader T. Tavassoli (2002), “Non-Conscious Influences on Consumer Choice,” Marketing Letters, 13(3), 269-279. Gazzaniga, Michael S. (2000), “Cerebral Specialization and Interhemispheric Communication: Does the Corpus Callosum Enable the Human Condition?,” Brain, 123 (7), 12931326. Hellige, Joseph B. (1993), Hemispheric Asymmetry: What’s Right and What’s Left, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Janiszewski, Chris (1990a), “The Influence of Nonattended Material on the Processing of Advertising Claims,” Journal of Marketing Research, 27, 263-278. (1990b), “The Influence of Print Advertisement Organization on Affect toward a Brand Name,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (June), 53-65. Larsen, Randy J. and Barbara L. Fredrickson (1999), “Measurement issues in emotion research,” in Well-being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, eds. Daniel Kahneman, Ed Diener and Norbert Schwarz, New York: Russell Sage, 40-60. Levy, Jerre (1976), „Lateral Dominant and Aesthetic Preference,“ Neuropsychologia, 14 (4), 431-445. McArthur, Leslie Z. and David L. Post (1977), “Figural Emphasis and Person Perception,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13 (6), 520-535. Meyers-Levy, Joan (1989), “Priming Effects on Product Judgments: A Hemispheric Interpretation,” Journal of Consumer Research, 16(June), 76-86. and Brian Sternthal (1991), “Gender Differences in the Use of Message Cues and Judgments,” Journal of Marketing Research, 28, 84-96. Puccinelli, Nancy M., Linda Tickle-Degnen, and Robert Rosenthal (2003), “Effect of Dyadic Context on Judgments of Rapport: Dyad Task and Partner Presence,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 47(4). (2004), “Effect of Target Position and Target Task on Judge Sensitivity to Felt Rapport,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28(3), 211-220. Rodway, Paul, Lynn Wright, and Scott Hardie (2003), “The Valence-Specific Laterality Effect in Free Viewing Conditions: The Influence of Sex, Handedness, and Response Bias,” Brain and Cognition. Rosenthal, Robert (1991), Meta-Analytic Procedures for Social Research, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. and Bella M. DePaulo (1980), “Encoders vs Decoders as Units of Analysis in Research in Nonverbal Communication,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5 (2). Sackheim, Harold A., Ruben C. Gur, and Marcel C. Saucy (1978), “Emotions Are Expressed More Intensely on the Left Side of the Face,” Science, 202 (4366), 434-436. Storms, Michael D. (1973), “Videotape and the Attribution Process: Reversing Actors’ and Observers’ Points of View,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27 (2), 165175. Taylor, Shelley E. and Susan T. Fiske (1975), “Point of View and Perceptions of Causality,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32 (3), 439-445. Tickle-Degnen, Linda and Elizabeth Gavett (2003), “Changes in nonverbal behavior during the development of therapeutic relationships,” in Nonverbal Behavior in Clinical Settings, P. Philippot and E. J. Coats and R. S. Feldman, Eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Vaid, Jyotsna and Maharaj Singh (1989), “Asymmetries in the Perception of Facial Affect: Is There an Influence of Reading Habits?,” Neuropsychologia, 27 (10), 1277-1287.