NEURO article Genesis Learning and non-learning effects of Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 in Aplysia californica Mary Petrosko1 and Robert Calin-Jageman2 1 Department of Psychology, Dominican University Correspondance should be addressed to Mary Petrosko (petr0308@umn.edu) 2 Department of Neuroscience, Dominican University SUMMARY: Existing research about Ginkgo’s ability to improve memory is mixed, and the neural mechanism by which it may affect behavior has yet to be determined. To explore claims of efficacy, the behavioral and physiological effects of Ginkgo extract EGb 761 (Schwabe Pharmaceuticals) were observed here in Aplysia californica. The effects of EGb 761 were measured on spontaneous activity, reflex sensitivity, rates of learning and forgetting in a long-term habituation paradigm and subsequently in spontaneous and siphon-evoked central nervous system (CNS) activity. Effects were measured after acute and long-term exposure to EGb 761. No significant effects of EGb 761 were observed on any of the dependent measures, suggesting that the EBg 761 compound is not bioactive in the Aplysia CNS. These findings suggest the following: 1) EGb 761’s mechanism of action is outside of the CNS (e.g. cerebral blood flow); 2) its mechanism of action is not conserved across the animal kingdom; or 3) that EGb 761 is not bioactive. Supplements to improve learning and memory abound in supermarkets and health food stores. These supplements are not regulated by the Food and Drug Administration and do not undergo testing to verify their efficacy. Rigorous and controlled testing must to be conducted to determine the efficacy and safety of the supplements. A popular learning and memory supplement is Ginkgo biloba, which has been used historically for medicinal purposes. The patented extract EGb 761, marketed by Schwabe Pharmaceuticals (Karlsruhe, Germany), contains a formula of 24% flavones and 6% terpenoides derived from the ginkgo plant (DeFeudis & Drieu, 2000). This formula has been marketed as the most ‘effective’ extract of ginkgo (Muller & Chatterjee, 2003). The purported memory effects of ginkgo have been supported by the argument that it increases circulation in the brain and has antioxidant properties (DeFeudis & Drieu, 2000; Gold, Cahill & Wenk, 2003; Petkov, Belcheva & Petkov, 2003). The formula of flavones and terpenoides in EGb 761 are thought to enhance the antioxidant and circulatory properties of the extract. For the purposes of this paper, “EGb 761” will identify the ginkgo extract patented by Schwabe Pharma. Any other extract of ginkgo will be referred to as “ginkgo extract.” Evidence of efficacy The learning and memory effects of ginkgo have been investigated in humans and other mammals, with most studies focusing on the effect of EGb 761. Despite several studies investigating the drug, the literature presents mixed conclusions regarding the efficacy of gingko supplements. Evidence does exist for EGb 761 effectiveness in alleviating learning and memory deficits in human and animal subjects (Kanowshi, Hermann, Stephan, Wierich & Horr, 1996; Lin, Cheng, Hsu & Chang, 2003; 14 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 Tchantchau, Xu, Wu, Christen & Luo, 2007; Wang, Wang, Wu & Cai, 2006); on the other hand, some studies report opposing results (Birks & Grimley, 2009; Dekosky et al., 2008). However, there is consensus regarding EGb 761 efficacy in subjects with normally functioning brains; in these populations, EGb 761 provides no improvement in memory (Burns, Bryan, & Nettelbeck, 2006; DeKosky et al., 2008; Solomon et al., 2002). The learning and memory effects of ginkgo have been investigated in humans and other mammals, with most studies focusing on the effect of EGb 761. Despite several studies investigating the drug, the literature is mixed regarding its efficacy. Studies in human and animal subjects with brain abnormalities has supported EGb 761’s effectiveness in alleviating deficits in learning and memory (Kanowshi, Hermann, Stephan, Wierich & Horr, 1996; Lin, Cheng, Hsu & Chang, 2003; Tchantchau, Xu, Wu, Christen & Luo, 2007; Wang, Wang, Wu & Cai, 2006); however, other studies have failed to support this effect (Birks & Grimley, 2009; Dekosky et al., 2008). In normally functioning brains, however, most research indicates that EGb 761 provides patients with no clear improvement in memory (Burns, Bryan, & Nettelbeck, 2006; DeKosky et al., 2008; Solomon et al., 2002). In rodent animal models, ginkgo has demonstrated selective influence on learning and memory. In studies with older or impaired rats, EGb 761 has been shown to be relatively effective in alleviating learning deficits (Kanowshi et al., 1996; Lin et al., 2003; Tchantchau et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2006). Lin et al. (2003) examined the effect of ginkgo in rats with bilateral common carotid artery ligation, which produced chronic cerebral insufficiency. EGb 761 improved impairments in spatial learning and motor function more effectively than an extract of local ginkgo. However, it is important to note article that performance did not return to normal function, and impairments in spatial learning persisted beyond EGb 761 treatment . Wang et al. (2006) found that EGb 761 administered to older rats improved their spatial learning for a maze task. Furthermore, they found that hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) was increased in subjects treated with EGb 761. This increase in LTP may account for memory improvements in treated subjects, as the hippocampus is critical in forming new memories and spatial learning. Research on gingko efficacy in humans with cognitive deficits is less conclusive. In a large, double-blind, placebo-controlled study conducted by Le Bars et al. (1997), a slight but consistent memory improvement was observed in participants with dementia on gingko supplementation. Participants treated with EGb 761 performed better on the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive subscale and on the Geriatric Evaluation by Relative’s Rating Instrument. However, the Ginkgo Evaluation of Memory (GEM) by DeKosky et al. (2008) found that EGb 761 failed to reduce the incidence of dementia in older adults (≥75 years old). GEM is a large and wellcontrolled study supported by the National Institutes of Health to test the efficacy of EGb 761 as a preventative treatment for Alzheimer’s disease. The study tracked participants for an average of five years and found that EGb 761 failed to prevent or delay the incident of dementia in older adults. Based on these conflicting results, EGb 761 may work through a mechanism which improves memory in those who have developed dementia but is not involved with pre-dementia memory mechanisms. While results are contradictory in impaired populations, similar studies with normal populations have shown little overall effectiveness of the ginkgo extract. Solomon, Adams, Silver, Zimmer, and DeVeaux (2002) examined the effect of ginkgo extract in healthy individuals aged 60 years or older during six weeks of supplementation. No measurable learning, memory, or attentional improvements were found for treated individuals. Similarly, no significant learning or memory improvement occurred for a younger population of participants--aged 18 to 43 years--observed across twelve weeks of gingko extract supplementation (Burns, Bryan, & Nettelbeck, 2006). In addition, the studies that do report disparate positive effects of EGb 761 in healthy adults, their conclusions may be statistically unreliable because of small sample sizes and unclear outcome measures (Birks & Grimley, 2009; Gertz & Kiefer, 2004). Outstanding questions Overall, the existing evidence suggests that EGb 761 may have neuroprotective qualities but that it does not actively improve learning or memory in normally functioning brains. One limitation with existing research, NEURO Genesis however, is the focus on a single indicator of efficacy: long-term recall measured at a single time point. This measure of memory and learning function may not be sensitive to changes in a normal population. To more fully evaluate the effects of EGb 761 in normal populations, it may be useful to collect a broad range of learning indicators, including rate and depth of learning, shortterm memory performance, and the duration of longterm memory. Another limitation in the study of gingko extract is our inability to identify biological mechanisms, which may be linked to the behavioral effects of EGb 761. One suggested mechanism by which EGb 761 may operate lies outside of the CNS; EBg 761 may improve blood circulation and increase the flow of oxygen to the brain (DeFeudis & Drieu, 2000). Other evidence points to the direct effects of EGb 761 in the CNS; older rats treated with EGb 761 exhibited increased LTP in vivo (Wang et al., 2006). Similarly, mice brains with applied EGb 761 exhibited increased LTP and neuronal excitatibility in vitro (Williams, Watanabe, Schultz, Rimbach, and Krucker, 2004). Additional studies have also found that EGb 761 increases neuronal plasticity, and protects against neuronal apoptosis in cell cultures (Defeudis & Drieu, 2000; Luo et al., 2002; Smith et al., 2002). Despite these findings, researchers have not yet explored the neural mechanisms by which EGb 761 influences behavior. Aplysia as a model system The marine gastropod, Aplysia californica (Figure 1), is a useful model organism in which to evaluate the effects of EGb 761. Aplysia provide sensitive measurements of the effects of EGb 761 effects in a biological system that can more easily be connected to behavioral measures. Aplysia have long proven useful for identifying basic mechanisms of learning and memory. One attractive feature is that Aplysia have a relatively small number of nerve cells, numbering less than 10,000 in its CNS (Kandel, 2001). Moreover, Aplysia have large nerve cells (up to 1 mm in diameter), which are both easy to monitor and manipulate (Moroz et al., 2006). These features make Aplysia uniquely useful for relating behavioral and neural function. As a result of their relatively simple neural circuitry, the sensory and motor neural circuit has been identified and studied extensively, relating its activity to the behavior it elicits (Kandel, 2001). In addition, the simple neural circuitry allows for extracellular and intracellular recording, with which neural activity can be analyzed and related to behavioral responses. Aplysia have been used to understand basic physiological mechanisms of habituation, sensitization, operant conditioning, place conditioning, and other forms of learning and memory (Brembs, 2003; Glanzman, 2006; Rankin, 2002; Stopfer & Carew, 1996). Vol 2 Issue 1 | Fall 2012 | neurogenesisjournal.com | 15 NEURO article Genesis Stimulus 30 stimuli, 1 min ISI 1 day Pre-test Training Post-test Figure 1: Procedure for Experiment 1, to elicit long-term habituation of S-SWR. Animals were assessed for baseline responding with the pretest, 3 stimuli administered at 10 minute ISI. Animals were then trained in 5 sessions, each session consisting of 30 stimuli at 1 minute ISI, with 24 hours between each session. After training, short- and long-term retention post-testing were administered in the same way as the pretest. Post-testing was administered 1 hour after the last training session, then once a day for the subsequent 6 days. Current study To address the diverse and discordant literature on EGb 761, this study used a three-part approach, which explored possible learning, arousal, and physiological effects of EGb 761 on acute and long-term exposure in Aplysia. The three experiments included measures on multiple time scales and explored possible effects on different behavioral and physiological outcome measures. The combined results offer a sensitive and inclusive analysis of possible behavioral and physiological effects of EGb 761. Effect of EGb 761 on long-term learning The first experiment examined the effect of EGb 761 on long-term habituation after long-term exposure. Habituation is a commonly used model of learning in Aplysia; it is a type of learning in which a subject’s response to a stimulus decreases as a result of repeated presentation of the stimulus. Although simple, habituation shares many attributes with more complex learning mechanisms (Carew, Pinsker, & Kandel, 1972; Stopfer, Chen, Tai, Huang, & Carew, 1996; Zolman & Peretz, 1987). Like more advanced learning mechanisms, habituation response rates can improve with training, are sensitive to the pattern and frequency of training, and can produce both short- and long-term memories (Abramson, 1994). A benefit of studying habituation is that it provides multiple indicators of learning and memory. This first experiment examined four separate indicators: depth of learning, rate of learning, short-term memory retention, and long-term memory retention. The behavior used for habituation training in this experiment was the siphon-elicited siphon withdrawal reflex (S-SWR). The S-SWR is a simple reflex in which the siphon is withdrawn into the mantle after mild stimulation to the siphon (Carew et al., 1972). The S-SWR 16 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 is easily observable and is easily quantified as the time from when the animal withdraws its siphon to the time it begins to relax the siphon back to the resting position. In the S-SWR, habituation can be elicited by repeatedly stimulating the siphon, producing a gradual decrease in the duration of the SWR. Depending on the pattern and duration of stimulation, it is possible to produce both short- and long-term habituation (Carew et al., 1972; Stopfer et al., 1996; Zolman & Peretz, 1987). This experiment explored the possible effects of long-term EGb 761 exposure on four separate learning and memory indicators of long-term habituation of the S-SWR. Effect of EGb 761 on arousal after acute exposure The second experiment explored the effect of EGb 761 on arousal and gross motor movement after acute exposure to EGb 761. Past research suggests that significant effects can be elicited from manipulations other than long-term gingko exposure; a single dose of EGb 761 before the administration of a narcotic resulted in increased arousal in treated animals (Brochet, Chermat, DeFeudis, & Drieu, 1999). Additional studies in humans have shown a slight improvement in reaction times on short-term memory tasks and sustained attention following acute exposure to EGb 761 (Elsabagh, Hartley, Ali, Williamson, & File, 1995; Kennedy, Scholey, & Wesnes, 2000; Rigney, Kimber, & Hindmarch, 1999). To test possible arousal effects of EGb 761, the second experiment examined behavioral measures of locomotion and S-SWR after acute exposure to EGb 761. Previous research in Aplysia has used both locomotion and SSWR as indicators of arousal after exposure to a treatment (Marinesco, Wichremasinghe, Kolkman, & Carew, 2004). Together, these measures serve as sensitive indicators of acute animal arousal. NEURO article Genesis Effect of EGb 761 on CNS activity The third experiment examined the physiological affect of EGb 761 on background and nerve-evoked neural activity in the CNS after acute exposure. An investigation of the possible biological mechanism by which EGb 761 affects the organism followed. Research with rats and mice has shown that EGb 761 can increase hippocampal activity and LTP when administered in vivo and in vitro (Lanahan, Lyford, Stevenson, Worley, & Barnes, 1997; Wang et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2004). In vitro studies have shown an increase in neuronal excitability and synaptic transmission when EGb 761 was applied to mice brain slices (Williams et al., 2004). This evidence indicates a correlation between EGb 761 and the neural mechanisms of memory. As Aplysia offer a simple neural circuit from which the activity of the CNS can be easily monitored and quantified, this study directly tested the claim that EGb 761 affects the CNS. The three experiments performed here allowed for a multi-dimensional analysis of the effects of EGb 761 on multiple behavioral and physiological outcome measures; such a comprehensive evaluation of the possible effects of EGb 761 in a model system may provide a link between physiology and behavior. Experiment 1 A number of Aplysia behaviors have been analyzed at the neural level. This particular study explored the effect of EGb 761 on long-term habituation of the S-SWR. To produce long-term decreases in S-SWR behavior, Aplysia received five separate training sessions consisting of 30 stimuli applied to the siphon, each eliciting a S-SWR. Training sessions were administered daily for five days (Figure 2). This protocol has shown significant long-term habituation learning lasting up to 14 days after training (Stopfer et al., 1996). Multiple indicators of learning and memory were assessed: depth of learning, rate of learning, short-term retention, and long-term retention. Depth of learning was measured as the change from baseline to minimum responding during training. Rate of learning indicated how fast the animal habituated and was measured as the number of trials during training to reach minimum responding. Short- and long-term retention was assessed by comparing each animal’s baseline responding to posttest responding, indicating the amount of learning retained after training. Together, these measures provide a sensitive composite of learning and memory outcomes. By using four separate learning measures, this study improves upon the standard reliance on one measure of memory at a single point in time and may reliably detect subtle changes in learning performance. Method Subjects We used 24 Aplysia californica. Animals (late juveniles, 75-80g) were purchased from the NIH Aplysia resource facility (Miami, FL) and housed in separate plastic containers in a 15° C tank of aerated artificial seawater (ASW). All animals were fed a diet of dried seaweed, delivered three times a week. Aplysia were maintained at a constant 12:12 light/dark schedule (light 6 am–6 pm, dark 6 pm – 6 am), and underwent treatment and training during light cycles. Drug administration Animals were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental groups (n=8): control, low dose and high dose. Animals were pre-treated with EGb 761 for ten days prior to training. The Gingko biloba supplement used was powdered EGb 761 containing 24% ginkgo flavone glycosides and 6% terpene lactones (Schwabe Pharmaceuticals, Germany). The supplement was dissolved 8 High 7 Low Control Seconds 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 BL TR 1 TR 2 TR 3 TR 4 TR 5 Figure 2: Long-term habituation learning curve data between experimental groups. S-SWR (s) throughout habituation training. Significant effect of habituation training, F(4, 84) = 8.02, p < .01. No significant effect of ginkgo on the rate or magnitude of learning (F < 1). Vol 2 Issue 1 | Fall 2012 | neurogenesisjournal.com | 17 NEURO article Genesis in an incubation chamber containing 2 liters of ASW. Treated animals were soaked in the incubation chamber for 1 hour each day; control animals were soaked in untreated ASW. Animals were housed and trained in their home tank throughout the experiment and were not handled at any time. For the low-dose group, EGb 761 was dissolved in the incubation tank in quantities that achieve a 0.17% solution. When equilibriated, each tank provided a peak dose of 0.133 mg for each 75g animal, or 1.77 mg/kg. The high-dose group received twice the low-dose exposure: incubation in a 0.35% solution providing a peak dose of 3.54 mg/kg. The solution was mixed by an outside researcher, and the researcher responsible for habituating and timing responses was blind to the experimental groups. S-SWR response The S-SWR behavior was quantified as the duration of the withdrawal reflex. A researcher manually stimulated the Aplysia by placing a thin, plastic stimulator against the inner wall of the siphon. The stimulator is then drawn up along the siphon rapidly to elicit the SWR in the mantle shelf. S-SWR behavior was measured in seconds by the researcher after administering the stimulation. Timing began once the siphon was contacted and ended once the siphon began to relax to its normal position. Habituation training Baseline responsiveness was determined as the mean response to three stimuli administered at a ten minute interstimulus interval (ISI). A ten minute ISI does not produce habituation. Habituation training consisted of five sessions of 30 stimuli administered at a 1 minute ISI with a 24-hour interval between training sessions (Figure 1). Memory tests After habituation training, animals were tested for both short- and long-term habituation. Short-term testing occurred one hour after the final training session 7 6 Seconds ± SEM 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 hr 1 Day 2 Day 3 Day 4 Day 5 Day 6 Day Figure 3: Forgetting curve for 6 days post habituation training between experimental groups. Average S-SWR response between groups. Significant effect of forgetting, F(5, 105) = 4.38, p < .01. No significant effect of ginkgo on the rate or magnitude of forgetting (F < 1). 18 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 on the fifth day of training. Long-term testing occurred once a day for six days following the final training day. Each memory test consisted of three siphon stimulations with a 10 minute ISI. Statistical approach Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The basic strategy was to compare task performance between treatment groups (control, low, and high). Univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA) assessed for group differences. Alpha levels below 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Results Baseline responding No significant effect of EGb 761 on baseline responding was found. After ten days of pre-treatment, baseline responding was characterized by timing three elicited S-SWRs at ten minute intervals in all three treatment groups (control, low, and high dose of EGb 761). S-SWR responsiveness was similar across treatment groups: control (M = 6.41, SD = 3.72), low (M = 7.03, SD = 2.72), and high (M = 7.79, SD = 4.60) treatment level (Figure 3). This result was confirmed with a one-way betweengroups analysis of variance (ANOVA); no significant main effect of treatment condition (F(2, 21) = .27, p > .05) was observed. Repeated exposure to EGb 761 did not affect baseline S-SWR. Any difference in learning or memory measures between experimental groups would not be due to unequal initial conditions between experimental groups, but instead due to experimental condition. Learning effects A mixed-factor ANOVA demonstrated that the means between the drug conditions were not significantly different, F(2, 21) = .97, p > .05, during any of the five training sessions (Figure 3). Therefore, animals in each of the three drug conditions acquired habituation at a similar rate, and EGb 761 did not affect rate of learning. A one-way between-groups ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the main effect of EGb 761 on average S-SWR at the end of last day of training. No significant differences were found between the conditions, F(2,21) = .97, p > .05 (Figure 2). This result is expected because no significant differences in rate of learning were found between the three conditions. All groups were given habituation training of 1 training session per day for 5 days (20 stimuli per session, 1 minute ISI). By the end of the training, S-SWR responsiveness had decreased substantially across all groups: control (M = 2.85, SD = .79), low (M = 2.92, SD = 1.55), and high (M = 2.92, SD = .51) treatment level (Figure 3). To evaluate the main effect of habituation training, average S-SWR across the last 5 trials in each training session were analyzed with a repeated measures ANOVA. NEURO article Genesis Stimulus 30 min ASW 1 day Gingko Figure 4: Procedure for experiment 2 to determine acute arousal effects of EGb 761. S-SWR measures were taken at the beginning and end of each session, 30 min ISI. Animals were exposed to EGb 761 during Days 3-4 of the experiment. The ANOVA indicated a significant training effect, F(4, 84) = 8.02, p < .01. Follow-up polynomial contrasts indicated a significant linear effect, with response means decreasing over time, F(1, 21) = 14.64, p < .01. To more fully understand the shape of the learning curve, a variety of simple equations were fit to the training data. For this analysis, all trials were included and data for each drug condition was collapsed because there was no main effect of drug condition and no interaction between drug condition and training session (F(4, 84) = .77, p > .05). The data best fit to an exponential function, which accounted for 75% of the variance in the training data. The parameters for this function were well constrained by the data: at the end of training, the animals’ S-SWR was 51.81% of their baseline responding, 95% confidence between 49.74% - 53.88%. After the first 19.91 stimulations, animals’ response decreased 40.68% from baseline responding. This means the greatest amount of learning typically occurred during the first training session (which consisted of 20 stimulations). Memory Effects No significant group difference was found on S-SWR one hour after the last training session, F(2, 21) = .96, p > .05 (Figure 3). This result indicates that the drug condition did not affect short-term memory retention: control (M = 3.61, SD = 1.60), low (M = 3.23, SD = .27), and high (M = 3.77, SD = 1.31). Similarly, no significant group differences were found on S-SWR for the six days following the end of training, F(2, 21) = .88, p > .05 (Figure 3). These results failed to support an effect of EGb 761 on short and long-term memory. To evaluate habituation training forgetting, a one-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to evaluate all animals’ S-SWR for the six days after training ended. The ANOVA indicated a significant forgetting effect, F(5, 105) = 4.38, p < .01 (Figure 3). Therefore, animals displayed significant forgetting during post-testing. A two-way mixed factor ANOVA was conducted to evaluate a possible interaction between EGb 761 condition and the forgetting curve over the six days following training. The ANOVA failed to support a significant ASW interaction between EGb 761 and forgetting, F(5, 105) = .84, p > .05. Therefore, the results indicate that animals forgot training at a similar rate regardless of EGb 761 condition. Experiment 2 In addition to possible learning benefits, EGb 761 has been purported to have acute arousal effects. Research indicates that EGb 761 can have an arousal effect in animals and humans after acute exposure (Brochet et al., 1999; Elsabagh et at., 1995; Rigney et al., 1999). To better document this effect in a physiologically tractable system, this experiment examined the acute arousal effects of EGb 761 in Aplysia. Two measures of arousal were used: S-SWR sensitivity and locomotion. Method Experimental design Experiment 2 used a within subjects design with all animals (n = 8) undergoing the same experimental sequence (Figure 4). Animals were transferred to an experimental tank containing 2 liters ASW while remaining in their home containers and were not handled at any time. While in the experimental tank, animals were videotaped to analyze locomotion. Animals were taped once a day for 30 minutes on 6 consecutive days. Drug administration Days 1-2, animals were only exposed to ASW and were not exposed to EGb 761. Days 3-4, animals were exposed to twice the recommended dose of Ginkgo. EGb 761 was dissolved in the 2 liters of ASW in the experimental tanks to achieve a 0.35% solution providing a peak dose of 3.54 mg/kg, double the recommended daily dose (120 mg recommended daily dose, average adult weights 67.5 kg, providing a dose of 1.77 mg/kg). Days 5-6, animals were only exposed to ASW and were not exposed to EGb 761. S-SWR response S-SWR response was measured in seconds by the researcher after administering the stimulation manually, as in Experiment 1. Each animal’s S-SWR was recorded once at the beginning of videotaping and a second time at the end of taping, 30 minute inter-stimulus interval (ISI). Vol 2 Issue 1 | Fall 2012 | neurogenesisjournal.com | 19 NEURO article Genesis Crawling The animals’ amount of crawling was analyzed using SwisTrack software (Lochmatter et al., 2008). SwisTrack provided the amount of pixels each animal moved during the 30 minute recording period by tracking their positions on an XY coordinate. Animal positions were calculated at an interval of 5 times per minute. The total number of pixels moved during recording was calculated using the Pythagorean Theorem. The distance moved in pixels was added together to generate a total crawl amount. The total pixels each animal moved was converted to the equivalent cm moved (Figure 5). The pixel to cm conversion was calculated using a calibration frame of the video; the amount of pixels generated was compared to the specified distance moved by a sample object. Results S-SWR response S-SWR was compared between EGb 761 exposed and non-exposed conditions. The within-subject control was highly reliable. S-SWR responses in each animal were very stable across days, alpha = 0.86 (Figure 6). A within-subjects ANOVA comparing the animals’ S-SWR behavior during pre-exposure, exposure, and postexposure days showed no significant effect of EGb 761: pre-exposure (M = 7.25, SD = 2.17), exposure (M = 6.44, SD = 2.39), and post-exposure (M = 7.01, SD = 2.16), F(2, 14) = .98, p > .05. Crawling The amount of crawling between the two conditions (non-exposed, EGb 761 exposed) was compared to detect a significant effect of ginkgo. A within subjects ANOVA comparing the animals’ total movement during treatment condition showed no significant effect of EGb 761on cm crawled: pre-exposure (M = 1441.28, SD = 1352.19), exposure (M = 2027.82, SD =1068), and post-exposure (M = 1632.22, SD = 1172.38), F(2, 14) = .57, p > .05 (Figure 7). However, the amount each animal crawled varied substantially between days, and the within-subject control was not reliable, alpha = 0.20 (Figure 7 and 8). Experiment 3 While EGb 761 failed to produce an effect in the Video Recording 1 3 Movement Tracking 2 1 2 3 4 4 Figure 5: Example SwisTrack locomotion tracking of 4 Aplysia during 30 min video recording session. 20 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 behavioral measures tested in Experiments 1 and 2, there is the possibility that it could have a direct effect on the CNS which is not manifested behaviorally. The biological mechanism by which EGb 761 is purported to operate is unclear. Some research indicates that it operates outside of the CNS, working through blood flow, etc. (DeFeudis, 2000). Other research, however, with other animal models and humans indicates that EGb 761 can increase neuronal activity and hippocampal function in subjects both in vivo and in vitro (Lanahan et al., 1997; Wang et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2004). A possible mechanism by which EGb 761 can affect the organism is through direct influence in the CNS. Neuronal activity and synaptic transmission has increased in some studies which look at the effect of EGb 761 when directly applied to the brain (Lanahan et al., 1997; Williams et. al., 2004). Experiment 3 tested the direct effects of EGb 761 on the spontaneous and nerve-evoked activity in the CNS. Nerve activity during three conditions (pre-exposure to EGb 761, exposure, and post-exposure) and was assessed by two measures. First, background nerve activity was measured as the normal spontaneous activity occurring in the CNS during the experiment. Second, nerve-evoked evoked activity was measured after an electrical shock was administered to a sensory nerve. The 1 minute preceding and following each nerve shock were excluded from background nerve analysis. Each minute segment was compared as a percentage of baseline responding (nerve activity during first 5 minutes of recording). Method Animal preparation Aplysia (n = 5) were anesthetized (50 mL MgCl) and dissected according to established protocol (Stopfer et al., 1996). The CNS (right and left P9 sensory nerves, ring ganglion, right and left connective nerves, abdominal ganglion, and siphon motor nerve) was removed from the animal and maintained in a 5 mL recording dish with profused ASW. The CNS was revived with ASW and rested 1 hour before recording. Electrophysiology Nerve activity was recorded extracellularly using suction electrodes with flexible Tygon tips. Nerve recordings were taken for the right P9, right connective, and siphon nerve in each animal (10hz high-pass filter, 3khz low-pass, 10k gain, A-M Systems AC/DC Differential Amplifier model 3000). Nerve recordings were quantified as the integral of the absolute value of the nerve trace (20 K/s sampling rate). Spontaneous background nerve activity Background nerve activity was calculated in one minute segments as the average of the nerve trace integral. The 1 minute proceeding and following each nerve shock were excluded from background nerve analysis. NEURO article Genesis 10 Seconds SEM 8 6 4 2 0 1 2 3 Day 4 5 6 Figure 6: S-SWR in experiment 2. Responses were stable (alpha = .86). No significant effect of EGb 761 on S-SWR (F < 1). 400 CM SEM 300 200 100 Figure 8: Example of SwisTrack locomotion tracking for one Aplysia across the six experimental days. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Day Figure 7: Average locomotion (cm) by day for experiment 2. Responses were not stable across days (alpha = .20). No significant effect of EGb 761 on locomotion (F < 1). 10 min ASW Gingko ASW Figure 9: Procedure for Experiment 3. This is used to determine background and nerve-elicited nerve activity in the Aplysia central nervous system (right P9 sensory nerve, right connective nerve, siphon motor nerve). 200µV Figure 10: Sample of spontaneous nerve activity in and out of EGb 176 bath application. Nerve-elicited nerve activity During nerve-elicited nerve activity trials, an 80-90 µA pulse (0.25 s duration, biphasic) was administered to the left P9 sensory nerve (A-M Systems Isolated Pulse Stimulator model 2100). The stimulus artifact (0.5 s after administration of pulse) was subtracted out of nerve-elicited nerve activity analysis. Nerve-elicited nerve activity was calculated as the average of the nerve trace integral for the 0.3 s following the stimulus artifact (0.5 s – 0.8 s after pulse administration). This protocol was validated by comparing SWR with nerve activity in a set of reduced siphon+tail preperations; the average of the nerve trace integral for the 0.3 s following the stimulus artifact (0.5 s – 0.8 s after pulse administration) was highly correlated with behavioral SWR response. Average nerve response after each pulse was compared as a percentage of baseline responding (nerve activity during first 2 pulses). Drug administration During the drug exposure condition, EGb 761 was added to the ASW in the recording dish (5µL EGb 761/5 mL recording dish capacity). Vol 2 Issue 1 | Fall 2012 | neurogenesisjournal.com | 21 NEURO article Genesis Experimental design Recordings were taken for the right P9 sensory nerve, right connective nerve, and siphon motor nerve continuously for 90 minutes. Ten pulses (80-90 µA) were administered to the left P9 sensory nerve throughout recording 10 minute ISI (Figure 9). For minutes 0-19 the CNS was exposed to ASW, establishing baseline background neural activity and nerve-elicited activity. During minutes 19-51 EGb 761 was directly applied to the CNS. After the 30 minutes of exposure, the profusion tube was reopened, washing out the EGb 761. For minutes 51-90 post-exposure, the CNS was exposed to ASW during post-test background activity and nerve-elicited activity recording. Results Background nerve activity A one-way within-subjects ANOVA was conducted for each nerve (right P9, right connective, siphon), comparing each nerve’s background activity across treatment conditions (pre-exposure to EGb 761, exposure, postexposure). EGb 761 demonstrated no significant effect on total background nerve activity (Figure 10): right P9: F(2, 8) = .50, p > .05; right connective: F(2, 8) = 1.06, p > .05; siphon: F(2, 8) = 2.22, p > .05 (Figure 11). Therefore, background nerve activity was not significantly different on any of the three nerves between the ASW and EGb 761 conditions. A one-way within-subjects ANOVA comparing each nerve’s nerve-elicited activity (right P9, right connective, siphon) across treatment conditions (pre-exposure to EGb 761, exposure, post-exposure) showed no significant effect of EGb 761 on nerve-elicited activity (Figure 12): right P9: F(2, 8) = .88, p > .05; right connective: F(2, 8) = .83, p > .05; siphon: F(2, 8) = .44, p > .05 (Figure 13). Thus, EGb 761 did not significantly change the nerveelicited activity in any of the three monitored nerves. 1.5 Percent of Baseline SEM R Connective Siphon 1 R P9 General discussion This study applied rigorous and controlled testing to determine the efficacy of the popular learning and memory supplement, Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761. Previous research is mixed as to EGb 761’s ability to improve learning and memory (Birks & Grimley, 2009; DeKosky, et al., 2008; Gertz & Kiefer, 2004; Le Bars et al., 1997; Tchantchau et al., 2007) in human and animal models. The first experiment explored the possible learning and memory effects of long-term exposure to EGb 761 on a long-term habituation protocol in the Aplysia. Despite sensitive learning measures which accounted for several factors of learning and memory (rate, depth, short-term and long-term retention), EGb 761 failed to produce an effect in any dependent measure. EGb 761’s purported effect of arousal after acute exposure was explored in Experiment 2 (Brochet, 1999; Rigney, 1999). Two arousal measures, locomotion and S-SWR sensitivity, were tested after acute exposure to the drug. Again, EGb 761 failed to produce an effect on any of the dependent measures. To further explore the suggested mechanisms by which EGb 761 is purported to work, Experiment 3 tested its effects in Aplysia CNS. Lanahan (1997) and Williams et. al. (2004) have suggested that EGb 761 operates on the CNS by increasing neuronal activity and synaptic transmission; however, no main effect of EGb 761 was found on the spontaneous background and nerve-elicited nerve activity in the present study. The general conclusion from these experiments is that EGb 761 is not bioactive in the Aplysia nervous system. Possible interpretations One possible explanation for the lack of positive results in these experiments is a lack of experimental power to detect the effects of EGb 761. The measure of locomotion after acute exposure to EGb 761 showed high within-subject variability and low power. However, this interpretations appears unlikely. Numerous other tested measures of the efficacy of EGb 761 demonstrated low within-group variability and high levels of internal reliability. In addition, the three experiments provide converging and consistent results. The preponderance of evidence provided by these experiments thus indicates no 0.5 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 Time Figure 11: Background resting neural activity by nerve (P9, right connective, and siphon). EGb 176 does not affect resting neural activity in Aplysia (F<1 for each nerve). 22 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 200µV 90 1s Figure 12: Representative siphon nerve recording of average nerveevoked activity as a percent of baseline (first 2 stimulations). NEURO article Genesis Percent of Baseline SEM 2 R Connective Siphon 1.5 1 R P9 0.5 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 Time Figure 13: Nerve-elicited nerve activity as a percent of baseline (activity after first two pulses) for each nerve (P9, right connective, and siphon). No significant difference during EGb 761 exposure (F < 1 for each nerve). effect of EGb 761. A second possible explanation for the null findings is that EGb 761 operates by a mechanism in humans which is not conserved in Aplysia. Aplysia are a useful animal model to study learning and memory, as their simple nervous system allows for identification of neural correlates of behavior. While the simplicity of Aplysia is an advantage in some respects, this simplicity also limits the applicability of invertebrate research to mammals. However, research with Caenorhabditis elegans, another invertebrate animal model, has indicated beneficial effects of EGb 761 (Luo, 2006; Smith & Luo, 2004; Wu et al., 2006). Moreover, recent research by Moroz et al. (2006) to sequence the Aplysia genome indicates that Aplysia may be more similar to humans than C. elegans genomically. Because Aplysia have NMDA receptors and are capable of synaptic plasticity, the animal model has proven useful for understanding mammalian learning and memory. Given the strong evolutionary conservation of the biochemistry of learning, it seems somewhat unlikely that Aplysia and mammals diverge in their sensitivity to EGb 761. Another possible explanation is that EGb 761 works outside of the CNS and its effects cannot be detected in Aplysia. Some research suggests that EGb 761 works through improving blood circulation and increasing the flow of oxygen to the brain (DeFeudis & Drieu, 2000). However, there is a lack of evidence to support EGb 761’s effects on blood flow through direct monitoring of these biological effects. Possible future research can explore this mechanism through direct monitoring of blood flow and oxygenation as a result of exposure to EGb 761. A fourth possible conclusion is that EGb 761 is not bioactive. The study by Solomon et al. (2006) found that EGb 761 was not active in normal healthy adults. There have been mixed results as to its effectiveness in impaired populations; however, the recent GEM study by DeKosky et al. (2008) concluded that EGb 761 failed to improve memory or prevent dementia in older adults. A recent meta-analysis also found that EGb 761 was not effective in improving memory in older adults overall (Birks & Grimley, 2009). Despite the majority of null results in large and well-controlled studies, mixed evidence to support EGb 761’s beneficial effects persists. This could possibly be due to publication bias, in which positive results are favored over negative results in publication. The proposed benefits of EGb 761 in positive studies are not consistently demonstrated, and the inconsistent purported effects do not appear in large, well-controlled studies. This study with Aplysia fits into the recent trend of evidence that EGb 761 does not appear to be bioactive in both Aplysia and humans. Abramson, C. (1994). A primer of invertebrate learning: The behavioral perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bear, M.F., Conners, B.W. & Paradiso, M.A. (2007). Neuroscience: Ex ploring the brain, 3rd edition. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins. Birks, J., & Grimley, E.J. (2009). Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia (review). The Cochrane Library, 1. Brembs, B. (2003). Operant reward learning in Aplysia. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(6), 218-221. Brochet, D., Chermat, R., DeFeudis, F.V., Drieu, K. (1999). Effects of single intraperitoneal injections of an extract of Ginkgo biloba (EGb 761) and its terpene trilactone constituents on barbital-induced narcosis in the mouse. General Pharmacology, 33, 249-256. Burns, N., Bryan, J., & Nettelbeck, T. (2006). Ginkgo biloba: No robust ef fect on cognitive abilities or mood in healthy young or older adults. Hu man Psychopharmacology, 21, 27-37. Carew, T.J., Pinsker, H.M., & Kandel, E.R. (1972). Long-term habituation of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science, 175(4020), 451-454. DeFeudis, F.V., & Drieu, K. (2000). Ginkgo biloba extract (EGb 761) and CNS functions: Basic studies and clinical applications. Current Drug Targets, 1, 25-58. DeKosky, S.T., Williamson, J.D., Fitzpatrick, A.L., Kronmal, R.A., Ives, D.G., Saxton, J.A., Lopez, O.L., Burke, G., Carlson, M.C., Fried, L.P., Kuller, L.H., Robbins, J.A., Tracy, R.P., Woolard, N.F., Dunn, L., Snitz, B.E., Nahin, R.L., & Furberg, C.D. (2008). Ginkgo biloba for preven tion of Dementia: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 300(2), 2253-2262. Elsabagh, S., Hartley, D.E., Ali, O., Williamson, E.M., & File, S.E. (2005). Differential cognitive effects of Ginkgo biloba after acute and chronic treatment in healthy young volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 179, 437446. Gertz, H.J., & Kiefer, M. (2004). Review about Ginkgo biloba special extract EGb 761 (Ginkgo). Current Pharmaceutical Design, 10, 261264. Glanzman, D.L. (2006). The cellular mechanism of learning in Aplysia: Of blind men and elephants. Biological Bulletin, 210, 271-279. Gold, P.E., Cahill, L., & Wenk, G.L. (2003). The lowdown on Ginkgo biloba. Scientific American, 288(4), 86-92. Kandel, E.R. (2001). The molecular biology of memory storage: A dialogue between genes and synapses. Science, 294, 1030-1038. Kanowski, S., Hermann, W.M., Stephan, K., Wierich, W., & Horr, R. (1996). Proof of efficacy of the Ginkgo biloba special extract EGb 761 in outpatients suffering from mild to moderate primary degenera tive dementia of the Alzheimer type or multi-infarct dementia. Pharma copsychiatry, 29(2), 47-56. Kennedy, D.O., Scholey, A.B., & Wesnes, K.A. (2000). The dose-dependent cognitive effects of acute administration of Ginkgo biloba to healthy young volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 151, 416-423. Lanahan, A., Lyford, G., Stevenson, G.S., Worley, P.F., & Barnes, C.A. (1997). Selective alteration of long-term potentiation-induced tran scriptional response in hippocampus of aged, memory-impaired rats. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2876-2885. Le Bars, P.L., Katz, M.M., Berman, N., Itil, T.M., Freedman, A.M., & Schatzberg, A.F. (1997). A placebo-controlled, double-blind, random Vol 2 Issue 1 | Fall 2012 | neurogenesisjournal.com | 23 NEURO Genesis ized trial of an extract of Ginkgo biloba for dementia: North Ameri can EGb study group. The Journal of the American Medical Associa tion, 278(16), 1327-1332. Lin, C.C.K., Cheng, W.L., Hsu, S.H., & Chang, C.M.J. (2003). The effects of Ginkgo biloba extracts on the memory and motor functions of rats with chronic cerebral insufficiency. Neuropsychobiology, 47, 47-51. Lochmatter, T., Roduit, P., Cianci, C., Correll, N., Jacot, J., & Martinoli, A. (2008). SwisTrack: A flexible open source tracking software for multiagent systems. Proceedings of the IEEE/RSJ 2008 International Con ference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, 4004-4010. Luo, Y. (2006) Alzheimer’s disease, the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans, and ginkgo biloba leaf extract. Life Science, 78(18), 2066-2072. Luo, Y., Smith, J., Paramasivam, V., Burdick, A., Curry, K., Buford, J., Khan, I., Netzer, W., Xu, H., & Butko, P. (2002). Inhibition of amyloid-beta aggregation and caspase-3 activation by the Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 12197-12202. Marinesco, S., Wichremasinghe, N., Kolkman, K.E., Carew, T.J. (2004). Serotonergic modulation in Aplysia: Cellular and behavioral conse quences of increased serotonergic tone. Journal of Neurophysiology, 92, 2487-2496. Moroz, L.L., Edwards, J.R., Puthanveettil, S.V., Kohn, A.B., Ha, T., Heyland, A., Knudsen, B., Sahni, A., Yu, F., Liu, L., Jezzini, S., Lovell, P., Iannucculli, W., Chen, M., Nguyen, T., Sheng, H., Shaw, R., Kala chikov, S., Panchin, Y.V., Farmerie, W., Russo, J.J., Ju, J., & Kandel, E.R. (2006). Neuronal transcriptome of Aplysia: Neuronal compartments and circuitry. Cell, 127, 1453–1467. Muller, W.E., & Chatterjee, S.S. (2003). Cognitive and other behavioral effects of EGb 761 in animal models. Pharmacopsychiatry, 36, 24-31. Perlman, A.J. (1979). Central and peripheral control of siphon-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia californica. Journal of Neurophysiology, 42(2), 510-529. Petkov, V.D., Belcheva, S., & Petkov, V.V. (2003). Behavioral effects of Ginkgo biloba L., Panax ginseng C.A. Mey. and Gincosan®. The American Journal of Chinese Medicine, 31(6), 841-855. Rankin, C.H. (2002). A bite to remember. Science, 296(5573), 1624-1625. Rigney, U., Kimber, S., & Hindmarch, I. (1999). The effects of acute doses of standardized Ginkgo biloba extract on memory and psychomotor performances in volunteers. Phytotherapy Research, 13, 408-415. Smith, J., Burdick, A., Golik, P., Khan, I., Wallace, D., & Luo Y. (2002). Anti-apoptotic properties of Ginkgo biloba extract EGb 761 in differ entiated PC12 cells. Cellular and Molecular Biology, 48, 699-707. Solomon, P.R., Adams, F., Silver, A., Zimmer, J., & DeVeaux, R. (2002). Ginkgo for memory enhancement: A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 288(7), 835-840. Stopfer, M., & Carew, T.J. (1996). Heterosynaptic facilitation of tail sensory neuron synaptic transmission during habituation in tail-induced tail and siphon withdrawal reflexes of Aplysia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16, 4933-4948. Stopfer, M., Chen, X., Tai, Y.T., Huang, G.S., & Carew, T.J. (1996). Site specificity of short-term and long-term habituation in the tail-elicited siphon withdrawal reflex of Aplysia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16(16), 4923-4932. Tchantchou, F., Xu, Y., Wu, Y., Christen, Y., & Luo, Y. (2007). EGb 761 enhances adult hippocampal neurogenesis and phosphorylation of CREB in transgenic mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology, 21, 2400-2408. Wang, Y., Wang, L., Wu, J., & Cai, J. (2006). The in vivo synaptic plasticity mechanism of EGb 761-induced enhancement of spatial learning and memory in aged rats. British Journal of Pharmacology, 148, 147-153. Williams, B., Watanabe, C.M., Schultz, P.G., Rimbach, G., & Krucker, T. (2004). Age-related effects of Ginkgo biloba extract on synaptic plastic ity and excitability. Neurobiology of Aging, 25, 955-962. Wu, Z., Smith, J. V., Paramasivam, V., Butko, P., Khan, I., J.R. Cypser, J.R. & Luo, Y. (2002) Ginkgo biloba Extract EGb761 Increases Stress Resistance and Extends Life Span of Caenorhabditis elegans. Cellular and Molecular Biology, 48, 725-731. Zolman, J.F., & Peretz, B. (1987). Motor neuronal function in old Aplysia is improved by long-term stimulation of the siphon/gill reflex. Behav ioral Neuroscience, 101(4), 524-533. 24 | neurogenesisjournal.com | Fall 2012 | Vol 2 Issue 1 article