Kaczensky et al. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 6 September 2005 Dr. Petra Kaczensky Department of Wildlife Ecology and Management Institute of Forest Zoology University of Freiburg Tennenbacher Strasse 4 D-79085 Freiburg, Germany Tel. +49-761-203-3799 Fax +49-761-203-3667 e-mail: petra.kaczensky@wildlife.uni-freiburg.de 11 12 13 RH: Activity patterns of brown bears 14 Activity patterns of brown bears in Slovenia and Croatia 15 16 17 Petra Kaczensky1,*, Djuro Huber2, Felix Knauer3,*, Hans Roth4, Axel Wagner5, and Josip 18 Kusak2 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 1 Institute of Wildlife Biology and Game Management at the Agricultural University of Vienna, Peter Jordan Straße 76, A-1190 Vienna , Austria. 2 Biology Department, Veterinary Faculty of the University of Zagreb, Heinzelova 55, HR10000 Zagreb, Croatia. Department of Wildlife Ecology and Management, Institute of Forest Zoology, University of Freiburg 3 4 Centro Studi Ecologici Appenninici, Parco Nazionale d´Abruzzo, Viale S. Lucia, I-67032, Italy. 5 Institute of Evolutionary Biology and Ecology, University of Bonn, An der Immenburg 1, D53121 Bonn, Germany. * Current address: Department of Wildlife Ecology and Management, Institute of Forest Zoology, University of Freiburg, Tennenbacher Strasse 4, D-79085 Freiburg, Germany. E-mail: petra.kaczensky@wildlife.unifreiburg.de 1 Kaczensky et al. 1 ABSTRACT 2 3 In most of Europe true wilderness areas do not exist and brown bears (Ursus arctos) generally 4 have to cope with human disturbance and infrastructure. The few studies in Europe that 5 investigated brown bear activity demonstrated a predominantly nocturnal and “shy” behaviour 6 in bears. There is still quite a debate whether the shy, nocturnal bears of Europe are the result 7 of centuries of persecution by men (genetically fixed trait) or if hunting and the high 8 disturbance potential in the multi-use landscapes are the driving force (individual learnt trait). 9 We analysed the activity pattern of 16 individual bears monitored for 3,372 hours between 10 May and October 1982-1998 in the Dinaric Mountains of Slovenia and Croatia. The data were 11 collected via time sampling and basically analysed with two approaches: a general linear 12 model (GLM) with seasonal component to delineate the most important variables influencing 13 the activity pattern and level and cluster analysis to group bears according to their 24-hour 14 activity pattern. Time of the day and age were the most important variables predicting 15 activity. Although individual variation in the activity pattern was high among individual 16 bears, in general, yearlings were more diurnal and had a less distinct difference between day- 17 and night time activity levels than adult bears. Subadults were somewhat intermediate to 18 adults and yearlings. We believe that nocturnal behaviour is most likely driven through 19 negative experiences with humans, giving space for much individual variation. More research 20 is needed to prove the causal relationship of nocturnal behaviour and the degree of 21 disturbance an individual bear is exposed to. 22 23 24 Key words: activity pattern; brown bear; circadian rhythm; Croatia; disturbance; multi-use landscape, Slovenia; Ursus arctos. 25 2 Kaczensky et al. 1 INTRODUCTION 2 3 The activity pattern of animals is determined by internal and external factors. Most 4 vertebrates are either active during the day (diurnal) or during the night (nocturnal), often with 5 peaks of high activity around dawn or dusk (Ashby, 1972 cited in Halle & Stenseth, 1994, 6 Enright 1970). Light and temperature act as external synchronizer for a certain activity pattern 7 (Nielsen, 1983), while the availability of resources, competition, predation (Geffen & 8 MacDonald, 1993) or disturbance (Liddle, 1997) may alter the genetically fixed and 9 physiologically regulated circadian rhythm. 10 Few studies in Europe have addressed activity patterns in brown bears (Ursus arctos) due 11 to the difficulties of observing and following a far-ranging, forest dwelling species (Swenson 12 et al., 2000). The few studies in Europe that investigated brown bear activity demonstrated a 13 predominantly nocturnal activity pattern (Roth, 1980; Roth & Huber, 1986; Clevenger, Purroy 14 & Pelton, 1990; Wabakken & Maartmann 1994; Rauer & Gutleb, 1997). In addition, bears are 15 known to avoid people and are considered “shy” (avoid people, e.g. by running upon first 16 sight of humans) and un-aggressive animals (Metz, 1990; Swenson, et al. 1996; Swenson et 17 al., 1999; Swenson, 1999; Zedrosser et al., 2001). 18 In North America, on the other hand, brown bears seem largely diurnal (Klinka & Reichen, 19 2002) and more aggressive than their European counterparts (Swenson et al., 1996; Swenson 20 et al., 1999). However, contrary to North America, brown bears in Europe co-exist with 21 humans in densely settled, multi-use landscapes (Mattson, 1990; Swenson et al, 2000; Linnell 22 et al., 2002). Bear habitat in Europe is almost exclusively restricted to forested area (Knauer, 23 2000; Knauer, Kaczensky & Rauer, 2000), but these forests are heavily managed by foresters 24 and hunters and are used by the general public for camping, hiking, mountain biking, and 25 berry- and mushroom picking. Because almost all of the human activities are confined to 26 daytime hours, bears can largely avoid encounters with humans by being nocturnal. 3 Kaczensky et al. 1 In North American diurnal activity levels of brown bears may also vary with the intensity 2 of human presence. In areas with low intensity of human utilization, bears are largely diurnal, 3 while in areas with high intensity of human utilization or during periods of frequent human 4 access, bears shift to nocturnal behaviour (Gunther, 1990; Klinka & Reichen, 2002; 5 MacHutchon et al., 1998; Olson, Squibb & Gilbert, 1998). There are also situations when in 6 absence on aggressive human behaviour (hunting) and with access to abundant forage, bears 7 may become human habituated and shift back to a diurnal activity pattern (Smith, Herrero & 8 DeBruyn, 2005). 9 In Europe the pressure on bears to behave inconspicuously and to avoid humans is high. 10 Bears that do not avoid people run a high risk of being shot in the hunted populations, and to 11 become food conditioned and subsequently removed as “problem bears” in protected 12 populations (Rauer, Kaczensky & Knauer, 2003). In addition, diurnal bears have a higher risk 13 for surprise encounters with humans, which occasionally have resulted in human injury or 14 death (Adamič, 1996; Swenson et al., 1996; Swenson et al., 1999) and subsequently in the 15 killing of the involved bears. Public acceptance of brown bears is greatly challenged by such 16 accidents and may hinder or even stop bear conservation efforts (Kaczensky, 2000a+b; 17 Kaczensky, Blazic & Gossow, 2004). Therefore, shy and nocturnal behavior in European 18 brown bears is preferable and deserves more intensive studies. 19 There is still quite a debate whether the shy, nocturnal bears of Europe are the result of 20 centuries of persecution by men (genetically fixed trait) or if hunting and the high disturbance 21 potential in the multi-use landscapes are the driving force (individual learnt trait; also see 22 Swenson, et al. 1996; Swenson, 1999). This question could be tested by comparing activity 23 patterns and behaviours of brown bears in wilderness environments versus those in a multi 24 use landscapes. Whereas these comparisons are possible in North America and data suggest 25 the importance of individual learning; in densely settled Europe large wilderness areas do not 4 Kaczensky et al. 1 exist any more (EUROPARC & IUCN, 2000) and human population densities are high, even 2 in the remaining bear areas (Mattson, 1990; Swenson et al., 2000). In addition, almost all 3 protected areas are smaller than an individual bears’ home range (Huber & Roth, 1993; 4 Linnell et al., 2002), hunting is an integral part of game and bear management in most areas 5 (Swenson et al., 2000), and human access to forested areas is facilitated by a dense network of 6 forest roads (Kaczensky, 2000b). Unfortunately, data on brown bears from wilderness areas in 7 Asia is largely anecdotal (Swenson, 1999). 8 Thus the question whether nocturnal behaviour in European brown bears is genetically 9 fixed or learnt can only be approached indirectly. If the nocturnal behaviour is genetically 10 fixed, we would expect the same nocturnal activity pattern for all bears with a low degree of 11 individual variation. Though the activity pattern might vary among age classes because of 12 maturity effects, there should be little individual variation within the same age class. 13 If, on the other hand nocturnal activity is a result of individual learning and experience, we 14 would expect a high level of individual variation. Because age is a surrogate for other, more 15 germane effects, more difficult to measure - in particular, the cumulative effects of learning 16 and life experience, as well as somatic changes that include increasing size - we would expect 17 age to be a key variable that determines the activity pattern in bears. 18 In order to study this question, we analysed the activity pattern of 16 individual bears from 19 the continuous brown bear population in the Dinaric mountain range of Slovenia and Croatia. 20 Our expectation was that nocturnal activity is a result of individual learning and experience 21 and we expected to see: 22 (1) Age is a key variable explaining differences in the activity pattern of individual bears 23 24 (2) Young, un-experienced bears are be less nocturnal than adult, experienced bears 25 (3) Individual variation is high among and between different age classes 5 Kaczensky et al. 1 STUDY AREA 2 3 All three study areas were located in the Dinaric Mountain range in Slovenia (Menisija), 4 and Croatia (Gorski Kotar and Plitvice Lakes) (Fig. 1). They are within the range of the 5 contiguous Dinara-Pindus bear population stretching from Slovenia in the north into Albania 6 and Greece in the south. The total number is estimated at about 2,800 bears, of which 300-500 7 are believed to live in Slovenia and about 600-1,000 in Croatia ( Dečak et al., 2004; Servheen, 8 Herrero & Peyton, 1998; Swenson et al., 2000; Zedrosser et al., 2001). In both countries bears 9 are hunted between 1 October and 30 April after a quota system (Huber & Frkovic, 1993; 10 11 Simonic 1994). The relief shows typical karst phenomena, water sink holes (dolines), steep canyons, caves 12 and shallow soils. Surface water is rare as water run off is largely underground. Periodical 13 lakes (poljes) and rivers that submerge after short distances are typical landscape features. 14 Elevations range from 300m to 1200m in the Menisija region, from 600m to 1500m in Gorski 15 Kotar and from 500m to 1200m in the Plitvice Lakes area. Bear habitat consists of mixed, 16 uneven aged forest stands. The most common forest community in our study areas (Abieti- 17 Fagetum dinaricum) is dominated by beech (Fagus silvatica) and fir (Abies alba) - 18 intermingled with varying amounts of spruce (Picea abies), maple (Acer pseudoplatanus) and 19 elm (Ulmus spec.). Only selective cutting is allowed, resulting in a dense network of forest 20 roads (1.5-2.0 km roads per km²; Kusak & Huber, 1998; Kaczensky, 2000b), most of them 21 opened for public use. Overall forest cover is high and varies between 66% in Gorski Kotar 22 and 74% and 75% in the Menisija and Plitvice Lakes area, respectively. 23 Human population density is low to moderate for European standards and ranges from 13 24 inhabitants per km² in the Plitvice Lakes area to 27 and 42 inhabitants per km² in the Gorski 25 Kotar and Menisija area, respectively. Concerning the intensity of human use, the Plitvice 26 Lakes area is famous for Plitvice Lakes National Park (200 km2), which is a major tourist 6 Kaczensky et al. 1 attraction. The Menisija area (1,500 km²) is located only 30 km from Ljubljana, the capital of 2 Slovenia and is heavily used for recreation. The Gorski Kotar area (1,500 km2) is known for 3 Risnjak National Park (30 km2). Even though there is no bear hunting within Risnjak and 4 Plitvice Lakes National Park, movements of all monitored bears covered ranges larger than 5 the parks (Huber & Roth, 1993) and it can be assumed that bears in all three areas are exposed 6 to hunting pressure at least in part of their range. Additional disturbance arises from forestry 7 operations, collections of plant parts, and various recreational uses (camping, hiking, 8 mountain-biking etc.). 9 10 A more detailed description of the study areas can be found in Huber & Roth (1993), Roth & Huber (1986), Kusak & Huber (1998), and Kaczensky (2000b). 11 12 MATERIAL AND METHODS 13 14 Capture and radio marking 15 16 We captured bears with Aldrich foot snares at bait sites and chemically immobilized them 17 with either Tiletamin and Zolazepam or a mixture of Ketamin and Xylazine. Traps were 18 checked each morning in the Croatin study area, and continuously monitored with trap 19 transmitters in the Slovenian study area. Trapping, chemical restrain and radio-marking 20 procedures followed methods described by Huber, Kusak & Radisic (1996) and Kaczensky et 21 al. (2002). A rudimentary first premolar tooth was extracted for age estimation (Stoneberg & 22 Jonkel 1966). 23 Bears were fitted with different types of radio collars (Croatia: Advanced Telemetry 24 Systems (ATS), USA, AVM Instruments Company, USA and Telonics, USA; Slovenia: 25 MOD-600, MOD-400 Telonics, USA) or ear tag transmitters (EL-2(42), Holohil, Canada). 7 Kaczensky et al. 1 Each collar was fitted with a drop off system made of corroding wires or cotton spacers to 2 prevent the life long wearing of the device. 3 4 Activity monitoring 5 6 Bear activity was monitored via time sampling (Tyler, 1979). Only clearly audible signals 7 were used to determine the activity status of monitored bears. We used analogue receivers 8 with a meter for signal strength that can be adjusted to reception strength (Croatia: AVM 9 Instruments Company, USA; Slovenia: YAESU, Wagener, Germany). 10 In Croatia, bear activity was recorded at sampling intervals of 15 minutes. Observers 11 listened to at least 30 signal pulses (“beeps”) and classified the bear “active” if at least 4 out 12 of 30 signal pulses clearly differed in signal strength, else “inactive” (for details refer to: 13 Roth, 1980; Roth & Huber, 1986). Visual observations during wake-up and random 14 observations during field work showed that ttypically an active animal showed more than 4 15 fluctuation of signal volume over 30 beeps (D. Huber, unpubl. Data). In this way we realized 16 a maximum of 4 activity samples per bear and hour during each monitoring session. 17 In Slovenia, bear activity was recorded slightly different. Activity was checked at sampling 18 intervals of 10 minutes and observers listened for a one-minute sampling period. Like in 19 Croatia, we considered the bear active if the strength in the signal pulse was clearly different 20 in ≥4 beeps. This activity criterion was previously determined from Zoo experiment, where 21 we had simultaneously radio-monitored and observed a radio-collared bear (Kaczensky, 22 Wagner & Walzer, 2004). In Slovenia we realized a maximum of 6 activity samples per bear 23 and hour during each monitoring session. Previous tests with different sampling intervals 24 have shown only minor differences for sampling intervals of 10 versus 15 minutes (I. 25 Reinhardt unpubl. Data). 8 Kaczensky et al. 1 Each activity sample resulted in information on the activity status, coded as a dichotomous 2 variable and the time of the day (middle European standard time (MEZ)). To compare 3 different bears we only used data collected between 1 May and 31 October, the time when all 4 bears were active outside their winter dens. We grouped bears according to three age-classes: 5 yearlings (1 year), subadults (2-3 years) and adults (> 4 years). All yearlings monitored had 6 already separated from the mother. All adult females monitored were without offspring. 7 For analysis we only used bears that were monitored for at least 48 hours evenly 8 distributed over all hours (equally covering day- and nighttime hours), which means a 9 minimum of 192 activity samples for the Croatian dataset and 288 activity samples for the 10 Slovenian dataset. In the Croatian study areas, several bears were monitored over several 11 years. The data were pooled if the bear did not change its age class status (3 adult bears), or 12 otherwise subdivided according to age class (1 bear). For all multi-individual analysis, each 13 bear was only used in one age class and the one bears that changed the age class was assigned 14 to the age class with the smaller sample size. 15 For analysis we only used hours with ≥3 activity measures and calculated the average 16 activity for each hour a bear was monitored on a given day. In total we had 3,372 hours of 16 17 individual bears available for analysis (Table 1). For age class specific comparison of activity 18 patterns we additionally used 170 hours from 13 individual bears (each with less than 200 19 activity samples) pooled by age class (Table 2). 20 21 Triangulation of monitored bears 22 23 In the Slovenian study area, we continuously followed bear movements during 24-hour 24 monitoring sessions by car or on foot. Due to a dense network of forest roads, the distance 25 between observer and bear was generally less than 1,000m. After each activity sample we 9 Kaczensky et al. 1 checked whether the bear had changed its position. If so, we determined the new position by 2 triangulation taking successive bearings by a single observer. The accuracy of the position 3 was estimated by the observers from the angle between the different bearings, the signal 4 strength and the topography, and was classified as: 5 (1) Location error < 50m: bear circled on close range and/or radio signal close to maximal 6 (2) Location error < 250m: bear only partly circled or circled at a longer distance and/or 7 8 9 topography limits the maximum distance between bear and observer (3) Location error < 500m: bear not circled, or circled on long distance (>1 km), and/or azimuth between most distant bearings less than 120° apart 10 11 Accuracy of locations was opportunistically confirmed when searching for daybeds, 12 feeding signs, or scats the following day (D. Huber & P. Kaczensky, unpubl. Data). We 13 classified bears as travelling (travelling activity), when the distance between two locations 14 was more than the expected location error, as active on the same area (stationary activity) if 15 the change was within the expected location error; these information were only available for 16 the Slovenian data set. 17 18 Standardization of the variable time 19 20 We compared activity among bears based on average activity per hour. However, the data 21 were collected between May and October and thus sunset and sunrise as well as their centre, 22 local noon and local midnight, change over the season. To compare hours with the same light 23 regime we standardized the variable time to the average day of the observation period. On 24 average sunrise was at 4:53 and sunset at 19:10. On days with later (earlier) sunset and earlier 25 (later) sunrise than on the average day, the time between sunrise and sunset was relatively 10 Kaczensky et al. 1 stretched (shortened, respectively). We separately conducted a linear transformation for each 2 day to fit the time intervals to an average day: 3 t st MPst MPr t r MPst Sst , where 4 MPr S r tst is the transformed time, 5 tr the measured time (Central European time), 6 MPst the standardized midpoint: local noon (12.00) or midnight (0.00 after midnight or 24.00 7 before midnight), 8 MPr the real midpoint, 9 Sst the standardized sunrise/sunset and 10 Sr the real sunrise/sunset. 11 Noon was used as midpoint (reference) for the time between sunrise and sunset, else 12 midnight. Sunrise was used for the time between midnight and noon, else sunset. This 13 resulted in four different formulas (combinations of different midpoints and sunrise/sunset). 14 Local noon and midnight change over the year due to the elipsoid trajectory of the earth 15 around the sun. By using this transformation long and short days can be directly compared. 16 Although, the real time available during night- and daytime is slightly changed, this does not 17 seem to be problematical, because season had no significant influence on bear activity (see 18 Table 3). 19 20 General linear model 21 22 In order to test for the influence of different variables on activity we used a general linear 23 model (GLM) with age, sex, season, and study area as factor variables. From a first visual 24 analysis of activity patterns, it seemed that most bears roughly followed a monophasic or 24- 25 hour activity rhythm and to a less extent a biphasic or 12-hour rhythm. Due to this periodic 11 Kaczensky et al. 1 behaviour and the circular structure of the time we fitted sine curves with 24 hours and 12 2 hours rhythms (seasonal component) into the model (Zar 1999). As time series data are 3 usually autocorrelated we used time lagged variables of the dependent variable as explanatory 4 variables. The periodic structure of the data results in the absence of a linear trend and thus 5 we did not have to de-trend the data, as done usually in time series analysis. 6 For the GLM we used the following independent variables: 7 Act_n: activity of n hours before - covariate 8 Age: Age class (1=yearling, 2=subadult, 3=adult) - factor 9 Sex: Sex (1=male, 2=female) – factor 10 Season: Month (1=May, 2=June, …. 6=October) - factor 11 Area: Study area (1=MN, 2=GK, 3=PL) - factor 12 24-hour rhythm - covariate 13 12-hour rhythm - covariate 14 and interactions of these variables. 15 We selected variables stepwise in a backwards fashion, removing those that failed to be 16 significant at the 0.05 significance level. For the final model we tested the residuals for 17 normal distribution (which was roughly the case) and confirmed that they were not 18 autocorrelated. 19 20 Comparing diel activity patterns of individual bears 21 22 To describe similarities in the diel activity pattern we run a cluster analysis using squared 23 Euclidean distances between all bears based on average activity level of each hour. Bears 24 were attributed to clusters using the between group linkage - this method uses the average 12 Kaczensky et al. 1 distance between all samples in a cluster to determine the distance to a new cluster, this way 2 considering all samples of a cluster. 3 4 Data from bears with small datasets of activity samples 5 6 We pooled activity samples of an additional 13 bears that did not have the minimum 7 number of activity measures required for individual analysis, first by bear and monitored 8 hours and than by age class (Table 2). We visually compared activity patterns and mean 9 activity levels with the results from the individual analysis. 10 All statistical analysis was done in SPSS 12.0.1 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA). 11 12 RESULTS 13 14 Main variables influencing activity 15 16 The outputs of the GLM showed that the activity in the hour before, the underlying 12- and 17 24-hour activity rhythm, and the 24-hour activity rhythm modified by age class were the most 18 important factors predicting activity or non-activity of a given hour (Table 3). Less important 19 were age class alone and the 24-hour activity rhythm modified by sex. Sex by itself was not a 20 significant variable, nor was season or study area. The model explains 45% of the variation 21 (Table 3) in activity and shows differences between age classes in the activity rhythm as well 22 as the overall activity level (Table 3). Differences in overall activity levels were significantly 23 different between adults (52%) and yearlings (62%) with subadults ranging somewhat in- 24 between (57%; post-hoc t-test, p<0.01). Yearlings have a distinct break in activity from 23:00 25 to 3:00, which is comparable to their mid-day break (12-hour or biphasic pattern). Adults, on 13 Kaczensky et al. 1 the other hand, have a more distinct and extended break during the day, but are basically 2 active all night (monophasic or 24-hour rhythm). Subadults were somewhat intermediate 3 (post-hoc t-test, p<0.01 for 24-hour-rhythm and p<0.01 for 12-hour rhythm). All age classes 4 show a combination of both patterns. 5 Bivarate comparisons among age classes showed a significant difference between yearlings 6 and adults only for daytime activity levels (adults 39%, subadults 52%, yearlings 64%; 7 ANOVA P=0.02, Padult-yearling<0.01; Padult-subadult=0.50; Pyearling-subadult=0.48 ), but not for 8 nighttime activity levels, mainly due to higher individual variation (adults 72%, subadults 9 68%, yearlings 60%; ANOVA P=0.32). 10 One bear (LILI) changed the age class during the monitoring period and showed a 11 reduction in the daytime activity level with increasing age. As a yearling (LILI82) she was 12 active 66% during the day and 78% during the night, whereas as a subadult (LIL83) she was 13 active 46% during the day and 77% during night (T-test Pday(yearling-subadult)<0.01, Pnight(yearling- 14 subadult)=0.85; Fig. 3). 15 16 Comparing diel activity patterns of individual bears 17 18 The results of the cluster analysis grouped bears into three distinct groups. Although the 19 grouping did not sharply separate bears by age, adults tended to be in group 1 (five adults, 20 two subadults and one yearling) or group 3 (two adults) and yearlings in group 2 (one adult, 21 one subadult and three yearlings; Fig. 4). 22 The main differences between groups were that bears in group 2 have a less distinct 23 difference between day and night activity levels. They show a more biphasic activity pattern 24 (12-hour cycle) as compared to the largely monophasic activity pattern (24-hour cycle) of 25 bears in group 1 and group 3 (Fig. 5). However, both groups had peak activities in the early 14 Kaczensky et al. 1 morning, a depression around noon, a second peak in the early evening and another 2 depression around midnight. However, contrary to bears in group 1 and 3 minimal activity 3 levels of bears in group 2 are higher during the day than during the night (Fig. 5). One bear 4 from group 2 (females LUCIA) did not follow the general pattern and showed a peak at noon 5 (Fig. 5). The two adult males in group 3, like bears in group 1, had a distinct depression in 6 their activity level during the day. However, contrary to bears in group 1, the activity 7 gradually sloped down from peak activity levels in the morning to a low at 16:00 and than 8 steeply rose again to high activity levels during the night. 9 10 Stationary versus travelling activity 11 12 The difference in the activity patterns between adult and younger bears was even more 13 distinct when comparing the 24-hours distribution of travelling activity. While three adult 14 females hardly travelled at all between 8:00 and 17:00, three yearlings frequently travelled 15 during daytime (Fig. 6). One female yearling (VERA) even showed a peak of travelling 16 activity around noon. Of the subadults, one male (SRECKO) showed a similar pattern than the 17 adult females, whereas a female (LUCIA) almost exclusively travelled during the day, with a 18 small depression around noon. This pattern of almost no activity during the day is much less 19 distinct in adult bears, when looking at stationary activity (activity without displacement). The 20 adult female MAJA, for reasons unknown, even showed a relative high level of stationary 21 activity during the day, similar to the pattern observed in the yearlings (Fig. 6). 22 Although mean travelling activity levels were lowest for adults during the day (adults 9%, 23 subadults 20%, yearlings 22%) and highest during the night (adults 27%, subadults 25%, 24 Yearlings 18%) differences were not significant (ANOVA Pday=0.54, Pnight=0.12), nor were 25 differences in mean stationary activity levels (day: yearlings 41%, subadults 35%, adults 15 Kaczensky et al. 1 21%, ANOVA P=0.21; night: yearlings 37%, subadults 35%, adults 31%, ANOVA P=0.66). 2 However sample sizes in each age group were very small. 3 4 Data from bears with less than 200 activity samples 5 6 Twenty-four-hour activity patterns of an additional 13 bears pooled by age classes, also 7 produced the expected difference between the age classes, that is low activity levels for adults 8 and peaks of high activity for yearlings throughout the day (Fig. 7). Again, subadults were 9 somewhat in between adults and yearlings. 10 Whereas at night all age groups had significantly different activity levels, during the day 11 only adults and yearlings differed significantly (day: adults 43%, subadults 48%, yearlings 12 60%, ANOVA P<0.01, Padult-yearling=0.05, Padult-subadult=0.85, Psubadult-yearling=0.10; night: adults 13 81%, subadults 60%, yearlings 38%, ANOVA P=0.02, Padult-yearling<0.01, Padult-subadult=0.08, 14 Psubadult-yearling=0.02). 15 16 DISCUSSION 17 18 Diurnal young bears and nocturnal adult bears 19 20 Our results showed a clear difference in the activity patterns of yearlings and adult bears, 21 with subadults being somewhat in between. The general pattern was that adults are mainly 22 nocturnal, whereas yearlings could be found active at any time. Individual variation was quite 23 large and not all bears followed this general pattern. Our results support the assumption that 24 the nocturnal activity pattern observed in European brown bears is more likely the result of 25 individual learning than a genetically fixed trait. 16 Kaczensky et al. 1 Difference between adults and yearlings were even more pronounced when comparing 2 travelling activity only. Adults hardly travelled at all during daytime, whereas yearlings and 3 some subadults could also be found travelling during the day. Consequently their chances to 4 encounter people were much higher. In the few cases (<20 occasions) where we saw bears, it 5 was either a yearling or the subadult female LUCIA. However, contrary to the yearlings, 6 LUCIA did not seem to be afraid of people and appeared to be habituated to human presence. 7 This data corresponds well with anecdotal observations by hunters and foresters in Slovenia 8 and Croatia, who also claim to mainly see small bears (D. Huber & P. Kaczensky, unpubl. 9 Data). 10 For one adult female (MAJA) with a relatively high daytime activity level, this activity was 11 largely confined to stationary activity that is activity in or around the daybed area. As daybeds 12 were mainly located in inaccessible areas (high cover or steep slopes, Kusak & Huber, 1998; 13 Kaczensky, 2000b) the chances for bear-humans encounters were minimal. However, as can 14 be expected when dealing with individual learning and behaviour that is believed to be shaped 15 by individual experience, not all bears strictly followed the same pattern. One adult bear (the 16 male HANS) showed a high level of daytime activity. Unfortunately we did not have data to 17 distinguish between stationary and travelling activity, nor did we have any information about 18 this experience with humans to better explain this behaviour. 19 The fact that subadult bears are more active during the day and expose themselves more 20 frequently to humans as compared to adults is frequently described also from North America 21 (Ison, Gilbert & Squibb, 1997). It is often assumed that subadult bears are generally less 22 affected by humans because of greater habituation (MacHutchon et al., 1998). But other 23 authors noted that subadults do not show the same tolerance than habituated adults (Braaten, 24 1988 cited in Olson, Squibb & Gilbert, 1998). The lower wariness is often explained with 25 food competition or the avoidance of aggressive adult bears. In our study areas yearling bears 17 Kaczensky et al. 1 did not actually shift from a nocturnal to a diurnal activity pattern, but rather showed equally 2 high activity levels during day and night and thus did not really avoid the time most adult 3 bears are active. Contrary to other regions (e.g. for Sweden see Swenson et al., 1997), we had 4 no evidence of interspecific killings, possibly because young bears grow rapidly and yearling 5 body masses in early spring often exceeded 55 kg (Kaczensky et al., 2002). 6 We speculate that young bears have a rather uniform activity pattern, with activity bouts 7 throughout the day and night, which through individual negative experience is changed into a 8 predominately nocturnal pattern in adults. Anecdotal evidence from Austria and Croatia 9 suggests that adult females are more diurnal in years with cubs than in years without cubs 10 (Rauer & Gutleb, 1997; Rauer, Kaczensky & Knauer, 2003, D. Huber, unpubl. Data). 11 Possibly this is the result of nocturnal adult females having to cope with relatively high 12 diurnal cub activity levels. Cubs of a wary, nocturnal mother probably have few encounters 13 with humans and thus lack negative experience with people. After family break-up at 1 ½ 14 years, young bears (yearlings) are no longer forced into a nocturnal activity pattern, but rather 15 will be equally active during day and night. 16 Diurnal activity might allow young bears to access food bonanzas, like high productivity 17 berry patches or bait sites, which may be monopolized by large adults at night. This would be 18 in accordance with the fact that it is mainly small bears that are observed at bait sites during 19 daytime hours in Croatia and Slovenia (D. Huber & P. Kaczensky, unpubl. Data). Klinka & 20 Reimchen (2002) also speculate that the higher daytime foraging activity of females with cubs 21 and subadult bears on spawning salmon on Knight Inlet, coastal British Columbia was mainly 22 due to avoid large nocturnal males. 23 Perhaps young bears first consider other bears more dangerous than humans. However, 24 their high daytime activity will result in frequent encounters with humans and will in 25 combination with the fast gain in body mass (other bears are not a threat any more) lead to a 18 Kaczensky et al. 1 shift in the activity pattern in order to avoid people. That this is a process of individual 2 learning is also suggested by the fact that bears can become habituation to humans when a 3 negative stimulus is missing or even food conditioning when “not running from people” is 4 positively enforced by food (Rauer, Kaczensky & Knauer, 2003). 5 Because we were not able to measure human activity or the degree of disturbance an 6 individual bear was exposed to, we are unable to prove the causal relationship of nocturnal 7 behaviour (used as a surrogate to avoiding humans) and age (used as a surrogate of 8 experience). All bear populations in Europe are exposed to humans and in addition, the 9 observed behaviour might well be a result of past experiences that would be impossible to 10 assess. Even a bear that lives in a remote area might have dispersed from an area of high 11 human impact, as especially males are known to disperse long distances (Taberlet et al., 1994; 12 Knauer, 2000; Knauer, Kaczensky & Rauer, 2000). However, all bears live in landscapes 13 inhabited by people and in all areas hunting occurs, consequently we assume that all bears 14 were exposed to some negative experiences with humans. 15 We believe that hunting is one important negative stimulus to keep bears shy and 16 nocturnal. Many game animals have adopted a nocturnal behaviour due to human persecution 17 (Georgii & Schröder, 1983) and readily switch back to a diurnal activity when protected (e.g. 18 Kitchen, Gese & Schauster, 2000), also suggesting a high degree of individual learning. The 19 first major selection against diurnal behaviour in bears of the Dinaric Mountains takes place 20 during the fall hunting season (Frković et al., 1987). By this time most yearlings have masses 21 around 70-100 kg (Kaczensky, 2000b) and are considered reasonable trophies. There is a 22 quota system, but within a hunting unit there are often several hunters that are interested in 23 shooting a bear. Often different hunters have to take turns at a bait site and thus seize the first 24 opportunity to shoot a bear. The earlier a bear comes to a feeding site, the higher the chances 25 are to get shot. The radio collared female bear LUCIA was the only bear we monitored in 19 Kaczensky et al. 1 Slovenia that was active during the day and did not show fear of people. She was frequently 2 seen by hunters at bait sites, but was spared because of her radio collar. 3 However, removing a diurnal bears does not have much to do with individual learning at 4 least not for the bear shot dead. But bears are missed by bullets or get wounded: a collared 5 bear shot in Croatia in 1997 had old fractures caused by bullets in the jaw and pelvis, and the 6 skeletons of two other bears had old bullets imbedded in bones (D. Huber, unpubl. Data). In 7 addition, bears often come to established bait sites in groups: siblings, several males courting 8 the same female in oestrous, or females with cubs (D. Huber & P. Kaczensky, unpubl. Data). 9 Normally only one bear gets shot at any given bait site thus providing a negative stimulus to 10 the spared bears. Even if bears are not the target of a hunt and do not get physically harmed, 11 they are often accidentally chased by hunters and their dogs during drive hunts for other 12 game. In North America, it has been shown that black bears (Ursus americanus) become 13 more nocturnal during the hound training season, possibly in an attempt to avoid people and 14 their dogs (Bridges, Vaughan & Klenzendorf, 2004). 15 In Austria, where bears are strictly protected, several young bears started to be active 16 during the day and did not show much fear of people. They were frequently observed by 17 people with no negative consequences; on the contrary, some might have even been fed by 18 people to allow for photographs and video sequences (Rauer & Gutleb, 1997; Rauer, 19 Kaczensky & Knauer, 2003). At least three of these bears were females and two had already 20 raised cubs. Especially when accompanied by cubs these females were often observed at close 21 range and cubs never learnt to avoid people, predisposing them to come into trouble. 22 Counteracting this process is a major concern of the Austrian bear conservation program and 23 several attempts were made to aversively condition these bears (Rauer, Kaczensky & Knauer, 24 2003; Zedrosser, Gerstl & Rauer, 1999). One of the pre-requisites for aversive conditioning is 25 that behaviour can be changed through a negative stimulus. Because in Europe shy behaviour 20 Kaczensky et al. 1 is closely linked to a nocturnal activity pattern, aversive conditioning can only be successful if 2 the activity pattern can be changed through individual learning. Based on the results of this 3 study, this seems highly likely. 4 We believe that maintaining nocturnal behaviour in bears is one important prerequisite for 5 the coexistence of brown bears and people in the multi-use landscapes of Europe. For this 6 coexistence it is essential to separate bears and people, either in space or time. As it is almost 7 impossible to restrict human access into bear habitat and because a viable bear population 8 needs a huge area, a separation in time reduces the probability of encounters between bears 9 and humans. We are well aware that this will not solve all problems. Access to human 10 provided food and livestock needs to be minimized and people need to be educated not to feed 11 bears intentionally or accidentally. Especially in small protected bear populations this is a 12 difficult task. People have lost the experience of living with bears and a bear that becomes 13 visible will either be perceived as a threat to live and property or as a sensation and will 14 attracts heaps of people. Unfortunately the most likely outcome for both scenarios is a dead 15 bear. How much and what negative feedback is necessary to maintain shy, nocturnal 16 behaviour in protected population is unknown and should be the focus of further studies. 17 18 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 19 20 We would like to thank Hartmut Gossow, Blaž Krže, Wolfgang Schröder and Miha 21 Adamič for scientific, organizational and logistic support for the Slovenian part of this field 22 project. Field work for activity monitoring was very time consuming and especially tiring at 23 night. Without the help of employees, students and volunteers it would not have been possible 24 to collect that many data. Special thanks for data collection in Slovenia go to: Mateja Blazic, 25 Matjaz Prosen, Evi Tschunko, Thomas Speierl, Christian Leitenberger, Ilka Reinhardt, Ferry 26 Pickavet, Jennifer Clarke, Janez Adamič, Antonio Di Croce, Alessia Gallastroni, Chiara 21 Kaczensky et al. 1 Braschi, Susanne Falkenstett, Marcus Regelmann, Sandra Heyer, Philipp Ferstere and Sonja 2 Sinnmayer and for data collection in Croatia to: numerous veterinary students from the 3 University of Zagreb including Sanja Morić, Violeta Dabanović, and Andrea Poljak. Special 4 thanks also to Helmut Küchenhoff from the Statistical Advice Office at the University of 5 Munich and to his students Regina Schwarz, Brigitte Dorsch and Christina Bindl for help with 6 the statistical analysis. 7 Funding for this study came from the Austrian Science Foundation (FWF project: P 11529- 8 BIO), the Austrian Federal Ministry of Science and Research (project: G.Z. 30.435/-23/92), 9 the Croatian Ministry of Science and Technology (Project 053026), the Donors Association 10 for the Promotion of Science and Humanities in Germany, the Forest Faculty at the University 11 of Munich in Germany, The National Geographic Society, EURONATURE, the Slovenian 12 Hunters Association and the Bevins Memorial Fundation of the International Bear 13 Association. 14 15 LITERATURE CITED 16 17 Adamič, M. (1996). Decision making in the conservation management of problem wildlife 18 species in Slovenia, based on the knowledge gained through current research projects. 19 Gozd 54:297-306. [In Slovenian with English abstract] 20 Bridges, A.S., Vaughan, M.R. & Klenzendorf, S. (2004). Seasonal activity in American black 21 bear Ursus amercanus activity patterns: qualification via remote photography. Wildlife 22 Biology 10:277-284. 23 Clevenger, A. P., Purroy, F. J. & Pelton M. R. (1990). Movement and activity patterns of a 24 European brown bear in the Cantabrian Mountains, Spain. International Conference on 25 Bear Research and Management 8:205-211. 22 Kaczensky et al. 1 Dečak, D., Frković, A. Grubešić, M., Huber, D., Iviček, B., Kulić, B., Sertić, D. & Štahan, Ž. 2 (2004). The Brown Bear Management Plan for The Republic of Croatia. Ministry of 3 Agriculture, Forestry and Water Management, Department for Hunting Ministry of 4 Culture, Department for Nature Protection, Zagreb, Croatia. 5 6 7 Enright, J.T. (1970). Ecological aspects of endogenous rhythmicity. Annual Reviews 1:221238. EUROPARC & IUCN. (2000). Guidelines for protected area management categories - 8 interpretation and application of the protected area management categories in Europe. 9 EUROPARC and WCPA, Grafenau, Germany. 10 Frković, A., Ruff, R. L., Cicnjak, L. & Huber D. (1987). Brown bear mortality during 1946- 11 85 in Gorski Kotar, Yugoslavia. International Conference on Bear Research and 12 Management 7:87-92. 13 14 15 16 Geffen, E., & MacDonald, D. W. (1993). Activity and movement patterns of Blanford´s foxes. Journal of Mammalogy 74:455-463. Georgii, B. & Schröder. W. (1983). Home range and activity patterns of male red deer (Cervus elaphus L.) in the Alps. Oecologia 58:238-248. 17 Gunther, K. A. (1990). Visitor impact on grizzly bear activity in Pelican valley, Yellowstone 18 National Park. International Conference on Bear Research and Management 8:73-78. 19 Halle, S., & Stenseth, N. C. (1994). Mictotine ultradian rhythm of activity: an evaluation of 20 21 22 23 24 different hypotheses on the triggering mechanism. Mammal Review 24:17-39. Huber, D. & Frkovic, A. (1993). Brown bear management in Croatia, IUGB Congress. 21: 287-292, Halifax. Huber, D., & Roth, H. (1993). Movements of European brown bears in Croatia. Acta Theriologica 38:151-159. 23 Kaczensky et al. 1 2 3 4 Huber. D., Kusak, J. & Radisić, B. (1996). Analysis of efficiency in live-capture of European brown bears. Journal of Wildlife Research 1:162-166. Ison, T. L., Gilbert, B. K. & Squibb, R. C. (1997). The effects of increasing human activity on brown bear use of an Alaskan River. Biological Conservation 82:95-99. 5 Kaczensky, P. (2000a). Bear politics in Slovenia. International Bear News 9(2):9. 6 Kaczensky, P. (2000b). Co-existence of brown bears and men in Slovenia. PhD thesis 7 8 9 Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany. Kaczensky, P., Wagner, A. & Walzer, C. (2004). Activity monitoring of a brown bear - a model approach to test field methods. Mammalian Biology 69(3):1-5. 10 Kaczensky, P., Knauer, F. Jonozovic, M. Walzer, C. and Huber, T. (2002). Experiences with 11 trapping, chemical immobilization, and radiotagging of brown bears in Slovenia. Ursus 12 13:347-356. 13 14 15 Kaczensky, P., Blazic, M. & Gossow, H. (2004). Public attitude towards brown bears (Ursus arctos) in Slovenia. Biological Conservation 118:661-674. Klinka, D. R. & Reimchen T.E. (2002). Nocturnal and diurnal foraging behaviour of brown 16 bears (Ursus arctos) on a salmon stream in coastal British Columbia. Canadian Journal of 17 Zoology 80:1317-1322. 18 19 20 Kitchen, A. M., Gese, E. M. & Schauster, E. R. (2000). Changes in coyote activity patterns due to reduced exposure to human persecution. Canadian Journal of Zoology 8:853-857. Knauer, F. (2000). Dispersal und Ausbreitung von Braunbären in die Ostalpen. PhD thesis 21 Technische Universität München, Munich, Germany. [Dispersal and expansion model for 22 brown bears in the eastern Alps; In German] 23 24 Knauer, F., Kaczensky, P. & Rauer, G. (2000). A habitat model for bear expansion from Slovenia into the eastern Alps. In Co-existence of brown bears and men in the cultural 24 Kaczensky et al. 1 landscape of Slovenia. Kaczensky, P. (Ed.). Unpublished report of the Institute of Wildlife 2 Biology and Game Management at the Agricultural University of Vienna, Austria. 3 4 5 Kusak, J., & Huber, D. (1998). Brown bear habitat quality in Gorski Kotar, Croatia. Ursus 10:281-291. Linnell, J. D. C., D. Steuer, J. Odden, P. Kaczensky, and J. E. Swenson. 2002. European 6 brown bear compendium. Safari Club International Foundation: Wildlife Conservation 7 Issues-Technical Series, 004A. Available from: 8 http://www.lcie.org/Publicaciones/BB/European%20Brown%20Bear%20Compendium.pdf 9 [last accessed August 2005] 10 11 12 13 14 15 Liddle, M. (1997). A classification of disturbance to animals. In Recreation ecology: 347-442. Liddle, M. (Ed.). Chapman and Hall, University Press, Cambridge, Great Britain. MacHutchon, A. G., Himmer, S. Davis, H. & Gallagher, M. (1998). Temporal and spatial activity patterns among coastal bear populations. Ursus 10:539-546. Mattson, D. J. 1990. Human impacts on bear habitat use. International Conference on Bear Research and Management 8:33-56. 16 Metz, C. 1990. Der Bär in Graubünden. Desertina Verlag, Disentis, Switzerland. [The brown 17 bear in the Kanton of Graubünden, In German] 18 Nielsen, E. T. (1983). Relation of behavioural activity rhythms to the changes of day and 19 20 21 22 23 night. A revision of views. Behavior 89:147-173. Olson, T. L., Squibb, R. C. & Gilbert, B. K. (1998). Brown bear diurnal activity and human use: a comparison of two salmon streams. Ursus 10:547-555. Rauer, G., & Gutleb, B. (1997). Der Braunbär in Österreich. Umweltbundesamt, Monographie 88:64pp. [in German] 24 Rauer, G., Kaczensky, P. & Knauer, F. (2003). Experiences with aversive conditioning of 25 habituated brown bears in Austria and other European countries. Ursus 14(2):215-224. 25 Kaczensky et al. 1 2 3 Roth, H. (1980). Diel activity of a remnant population of European brown bears. International Conference on Bear Research and Management 5:223-229. Roth, H., & Huber, D. (1986). Diel activity of brown bears in Plitvice Lakes National Park, 4 Yugoslavia . International Conference on Bear Research and Management 6:177-181. 5 Servheen, C., Herrero, S. & Peyton, B. (1998). Conservation action plan for the world bears. 6 7 IUCN, Gland, Switzerland. Simonic, A. (1994). The legal protection of the brown bear in Slovene territory-past and 8 present, and some suggestions for the future. In Braunbär in den Ländern Alpen-Adria: 43- 9 75. Proceedings of a workshop, Ljubljana, Slovenia. 10 11 12 13 Smith, T. S., Herrero, S. & DeBruyn, T. (2005). Alaskan brown bears, humans, and habituation. Ursus 16: 1-10. Stoneberg R. P. & Jonkel C. J. (1966). Age determination of black bears by cementum layers. Journal of Wildlife Management 30: 411-414. 14 Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F. Heim, M. Brunberg, S. Sorensen, O. J. Söderberg, A. Bjärvall, 15 A. Franzen, R. Wikan, S. Wabakken, P. & Overskaug, K. (1996). Er den skandinaviske 16 bjornen farlig? NINA Oppdragsmelding 404:1-26. [in Swedish] 17 18 19 20 21 Swenson, J. E. 1999. Does hunting affect the behavior of brown bears in Eurasia? Ursus 11:157-162. Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F. Söderberg, A. Bjärvall, A. Franzen, R. & Wabakken, P. (1997). Infanticide caused by hunting of male bears. Nature 386:450-451. Swenson, J. E., Sandegren, F. Söderberg, A, Heim, M. Sorensen, O. J. Bjärvall, A. Franzen, 22 R. Wikan, S. & Wabakken, P. (1999). Interactions between brown bears and humans in 23 Scandinavia. Biosphere Conservation 2:1-9. 24 Swenson, J. E., Gerstl, N. Dahle, B. & Zedrosser, A. (2000). Action Plan for the conservation 25 of the brown bear in Europe. Convention on the Conservation of European Wildlife and 26 Kaczensky et al. 1 Natural Habitats (Bern Convention) , Nature and Environment 114, Council of Europe 2 Publishing, Strasbourg, France. 3 Taberlet, P., Swenson, J. E. Sandegren, F. & Bjärvall, A. (1994). Localization of a contact 4 zone between two highly divergent mitochondrial DNA lineages of the brown bear Ursus 5 arctos in Scandinavia. Conservation Biology 9:1255-1261. 6 Tyler, S. (1979). Time-sampling: a matter of convention. Animals Behavior 27:801-810. 7 Wabakken, P. & Maartmann, E. (1994). Sluttrapport for bjorn - sauprosjektet i Hedmark 8 1990-93. NINA forskningsrapport 58: 41pp. [in Norwegian with English abstract] 9 10 11 12 13 14 Zar, J. H. (1999). Biostatistical analysis. Prentice-Hall International, Inc., Simon & Schuster/A Viacom Company, Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, USA. Zedrosser, A., Gerstl, N. & Rauer, G. (1999). Brown bears in Austria. Federal Environment Agency, Volume M-117:42pp, Vienna, Austria. Zedrosser, A., Dahle, B. Swenson, J.E. & Gerstl, N. (2001). Status and Management of the brown bear in Europe. Ursus 12:9-20. 15 27 Kaczensky et al. 1 Table 1: Dataset used for individual analysis of brown bear activity in Menisija (MN), 2 Slovenia, and Plitvice Lakes (PL) and Gorski Kotar (GK), Croatia. Bear Age Sex Activity monitoring Activity Total Study Start readings hours1 Area2 End Adults: FRANJO 5 male 21.06.1982 09.10.1982 268 6 male 14.05.1983 04.10.1983 254 7 male 20.05.1984 09.09.1984 713 8 male 26.05.1985 07.09.1985 6 HAL 6 male 16.05.1983 19.09.1983 260 HANS 5 male 18.05.1985 28.10.1985 422 7 male 10.05.1987 05.09.1987 209 DADO 4 male 16.05.1986 06.10.1986 197 GABI 5 female 14.06.1986 12.10.1986 512 6 female 21.05.1987 19.09.1987 136 9 female 01.05.1997 19.06.1997 600 103 MN adult female 14.05.1998 09.10.1998 1,982 338 MN 5 female 15.05.1998 24.10.1998 2,259 385 MN LILI83 2 female 09.05.1983 30.07.1983 500 108 PL INGA 3 female 27.05.1987 10.09.1987 220 43 GK LUCIA 2 female 04.05.1997 08.10.1997 3,334 564 MN SRECKO 3 male 04.05.1997 09.10.1997 2,634 448 MN (LILI822 1 female 01.05.1982 17.10.1982 798 144 PL) PEPI 1 male 10.07.1987 19.09.1987 414 91 GK VANJA 1 female 01.05.1997 11.08.1997 1,615 277 MN VERA 1 female 07.05.1997 28.05.1997 590 97 MN DUSAN 1 male 10.05.1997 09.07.1997 718 125 MN 18,641 3,372 MAJA ANCKA POLONA PL 285 PL PL 49 135 45 135 PL PL PL GK Subadults: Yearlings: total 3 1 Monitoring hours with ≥3 activity samples 4 2 Was used to compare with data from LILI83, but not in multi-bear comparisons 28 Kaczensky et al. 1 Table 2: Dataset of bears with less than 200 activity samples, used for analysis pooled by age 2 class. Area1 Activity monitoring Bear Age Sex Start End Activity Surveyed readings Adults: hours 309 51 VIOLETA 10 female 10.07. 1987 05.09. 1987 87 18 GK NIVA 13 female 02.05. 1990 13.10. 1990 71 11 GK BOB 5 male 16.05. 1986 29.08. 1986 6 1 PL VLADO 5 male 16.05. 1986 17.05. 1986 5 1 PL NENO 12 male 30.07. 1986 19.10. 1986 140 20 GK 380 60 Subadults: JURICA 2 female 03.05. 1985 31.05. 1985 160 24 PL JURA 3 male 18.05. 1983 30.07. 1983 28 6 PL GORAN 3 male 06.07. 1986 17.09. 1986 49 6 GK FRKO 3 male 27.05. 1987 03.08. 1987 143 24 GK 446 59 Yearlings: LINDA 1 female 07.07. 1990 12.12. 1990 69 9 GK DARKO 1 male 03.05. 1985 11.09. 1985 152 24 PL MIKI 1 male 11.06. 1990 12.10. 1990 84 7 GK NEJC 1 male 12.06. 1998 14.07. 1998 141 19 MN 1,135 170 total 3 1 MN=Menisija (SLO), GK=Gorski Kotar (HR), PL=Plitvice Lakes (HR) 4 2 Hours monitored with ≥3 activity samples 5 6 29 Kaczensky et al. 1 Table. 3: Effects of different variables on the activity pattern of bears using a general linear 2 model (GLM) with seasonal component (12-hour and 24-hour rhythm). Most effect on activity 3 (according to the sum of squares) has the activity of the hour before (Act_1), followed by the 4 periodic activity rhythms (12 and 24-hour rhythm) and interactions of them with age and to a 5 lesser degree with sex. Age alone has only a slight effect and sex alone has no significant 6 effect. Note, that age and sex affect more the pattern of the daily activity, than the level. Sum of Mean Parameter square df square F P value Corrected model 188.19 18 10.46 116.26 <0.01 51.08 1 51.08 568.04 <0.01 Age 1.35 2 0.67 7.48 <0.01 Sex 0.05 1 0.05 0.58 0.45 82.18 1 82.18 913.81 <0.01 24-hour rhythm 5.17 2 57.46 <0.01 12-hour rhythm 5.63 2 62.63 <0.01 Age * 24-hour rhythm 4.77 4 26.51 <0.01 Age * 12-hour rhythm 0.75 4 4.18 <0.05 Sex * 24-hour rhythm 2.34 2 25.97 <0.01 Error 227.79 2533 Corrected total variation 415.99 2551 Constant Act_1 0.09 R² = 0.452 7 8 30 Kaczensky et al. 1 Fig.1: Location of the three study areas in Slovenia and Croatia (MN = Menisija, GK = 2 Gorski Kotar, PL = Plitvice Lakes). 3 4 Fig. 2: Activity level during day- (white) and night time (gray) hours for adult, subadult and 5 yearling bears. 6 7 Fig. 3: Activity patterns of one bear that were monitored during different seasons and 8 changed from the yearling to the subadult age class. 9 10 Fig. 4: Dendrogram using Average Linkage (Euclidean distances between Groups) of the 11 cluster analysis comparing diel activity patterns between individual bears. Age composition of 12 group 1: 5 adults, 2 subadults and 1 yearling, group 2: 1 adult, 1 subadult and 3 yearlings, and 13 group 3: 2 adults. 14 15 Fig. 5: Activity per hour of all bears within the 3 groups identified by cluster analysis. 16 17 Fig. 6: Travelling and stationary activity per hour for 8 bears in three age groups. These data 18 were only available for the Slovenian study area. 19 20 Fig. 7: Activity per hour for an additional 13 bears pooled by age class. 31 Kaczensky et al. 1 32 Adults Subadults G A8 7 LI 83 PE PI SA N VE R A JA 0.90 D U VA N C IA LU IN LI SR EC KO KA D AD O G AB I M AJ A H AN S AN C H AL AN J PO O LO N A 1 FR Day- and nighttime activity levels Kaczensky et al. 1.00 night day 0.80 0.70 0.60 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 Yearlings 33 Kaczensky et al. 1 Bear bear: lili82 LILI82 LILI83 lili83 1.00 1,00 Activity level activity level 0.80 0,80 0.60 0,60 0.40 0,40 0.20 0,20 0,00 0.00 22 20 18 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 hours Hour 34 Kaczensky et al. Rescaled Distance Cluster Combine 1 CASE Label Num FRANJO SRECKO POLONA ANCKA GABI MAJA LILI83 VANJA INGA LUCIA PEPI HANS DUSAN VERA DADO HAL 4 14 13 1 5 11 9 15 8 10 12 7 3 16 2 6 0 5 10 15 20 25 35 Kaczensky et al. 1 1.00 1.00 Group 1 0.90 0.90 0.80 0.80 0.60 0.60 activity level Activity level 0.70 0.70 0.50 0.50 0.40 0.40 0.30 0.30 0.20 0.20 0.10 0.10 0.00 0.00 1.00 1.00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 0.90 0.90 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 Group 2 hour 0.80 0.80 0.60 0.60 activity level Activity level 0.70 0.70 0.50 0.50 0.40 0.40 0.30 0.30 0.20 0.20 0.10 0.10 0.00 0.00 1.00 1.00 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 hour Group 3 0.90 0.90 0.80 0.80 0.60 0.60 activity level Activity level 0.70 0.70 0.50 0.50 0.40 0.40 0.30 0.30 0.20 0.20 0.10 0.10 0.00 0.00 0 0 11 22 33 44 55 66 77 88 9 9 10 11 11 12 12 13 13 14 14 15 15 16 16 17 17 18 19 20 20 21 21 22 23 10 18 19 22 23 hour Hour 36 Kaczensky et al. Stationary activity 1 Traveling activity Adults: 1.00 ANCKA MAJA POLONA Activity level 0.80 0.60 0.40 0.20 subadult 1,0 0,8 0.80 0,8 activity level 0,6 0.60 0,4 0.40 0,0 0.00 0,0 lucia LUCIA srecko SRECKO 22 20 18 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 22 20 18 16 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 0.80 Subadults: 0,4 0,2 hour lucia srecko 0,6 0,2 0.20 1.00 Activity level subadults 1,0 1.00 activity level Activity level 0.00 hour Yearlings: DUSAN VANJA VERA 0.60 0.40 0.20 0.00 37 Kaczensky et al. 1 1.00 2 adults subadults yearlings 0.90 0.80 Activity level activity level 0.70 0.60 0.50 0.40 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 hours Hour 38