Sexual diversity in urban Norwegians The Journal of Sex Research New York Nov 2002 Authors: Authors: Authors: Volume: Issue: Pagination: ISSN: Subject Terms: Bente Traeen Hein Stigum Dagfinn Sorensen 39 4 249-258 00224499 Homosexuality Love Behavior Geographic Names: Norway Abstract: The purpose of this study is to describe homosexual and heterosexual experiences in terms of sexual fantasies, sexual attraction, sexual conduct, and falling in love in an urban Norwegian population. In 1997, a random sample of 5, 000 persons (18-49 years) was drawn from the population register in Oslo to participate in a sexual behavior survey. Data collection was carried out by means of postal questionnaires and 45% responded. The results indicated that exclusive homosexuality was rare in the population. One exception was the prevalence of homosexual fantasies in women. Compared to respondents who had had exclusively heterosexual contact, respondents reporting bisexual contacts had a significantly higher number of lifetime sex partners, higher frequency of anal and oral sex and masturbation, and lower age at orgasm and masturbation debut. What significantly separated the groups of exclusive heterosexuals, bisexuals, and exclusive homosexuals were attitudes towards various expressions of sexuality and number of lifetime sex partners. Copyright Society for the Scientific Study of Sex Nov 2002 Full Text: The purpose of this study is to describe homosexual and heterosexual experiences in terms of sexual fantasies, sexual attraction, sexual conduct, and falling in love in an urban Norwegian population. In 1997, a random sample of 5,000 persons (18-49 years) was drawn from the population register in Oslo to participate in a sexual behavior survey. Data collection was carried out by means of postal questionnaires and 45% responded. The results indicated that exclusive homosexuality was rare in the population. One exception was the prevalence of homosexual fantasies in women. Compared to respondents who had had exclusively heterosexual contact, respondents reporting bisexual contacts had a significantly higher number of lifetime sex partners, higher frequency of anal and oral sex and masturbation, and lower age at orgasm and masturbation debut. What significantly separated the groups of exclusive heterosexuals, bisexuals, and exclusive homosexuals were attitudes towards various expressions of sexuality and number of lifetime sex partners. This research was supported by the Ministry of Social and Health Affaires, the Norwegian Research Council, and Wyeth Lederle Norway. Address correspondence to Bente Traeen, Department of Psychology, University of Tromso, 9037 Tromso, Norway; e-mail: bentet@psyk.uit.no. Expressions of sexuality have been shown to vary according to historical period, as well as between and within different societies and cultures. Culture shapes sexual beings, and what is perceived as normal, natural, true, good, bad, right, or wrong is connected to culture-specific norms, rules, values, and expectancies. No two cultures have exactly the same expressions of homosexuality. For instance, the least homosexual behavior is observed in countries with the least acceptance of homosexuality (Sandfort, 1998). During the past decades there has been a growing social acceptance of homosexuality in Norway, as reflected by recent legislation. In 1993, the Norwegian parliament granted lesbian and homosexual couples the same rights and responsibilities that heterosexual couples automatically receive through marriage, except the right to adopt children and to be married in the church. Between 1987 and 1997, the number of Norwegians who reported ever having sexual contact with a person of the same gender increased from 4% to 5% among men, and from 3% to 6% among women (Stigum, Tren, & Magnus, 2000). However, the prevalence of homosexual behavior is likely to be higher in larger cities than in a country as a whole (Sandfort, 1998; Wellings, Wadsworth, and Johnson, 1994). Practicing homosexuality is probably easier in urban than in rural areas, in the sense that the social control of sexuality is less strict and visible in a big city. Also, there are more places in a city for homosexual people to meet and socialize. Life in a city is likely to allow for greater anonymity and as such allow for sexual diversity (Wellings et al., 1994). This does not exclude the existence of pockets of the urban population with unrealized homosexual desires. However, to explore this possibility further, sexual behavior surveys in representative samples of the population are necessary. Gagnon and Simon (1977) have claimed that sexual behavior surveys are important in providing an empirical framework for recognition of sexual diversity. The term diversity here refers to a broad spectrum of sexual expressions, and no more value is attached to the one or the other. Thus sexual diversity can be expressed through specific behaviors, attitudes, feelings, and preferences, including the choice of gender of the person being sexually engaged with. Gender of the sex partner is, however, closely connected to the concept of sexual orientation, which is commonly perceived as a stable part of personality, including not only sexual feelings and behaviors but also feelings of love, commitment, and other relational aspects (Sell, 1997). Sexual diversity may thus be confused with sexual orientation. In population surveys, it is often the prevalence of same-gender and oppositegender sexual contact that is measured, rather than sexual orientation. Some people report exclusively heterosexual contacts, others exclusively homosexual contacts, and still others bisexual contacts. The reporting may be indicative of sexual orientation, but the extent to which sexual behavior in itself can be used to define sexual orientation remains uncertain (Sell, 1997). As shown in several general population studies on sexual behavior, the number of people reporting exclusively homosexual contact is small (Sandfort, 1998). In his extensive analysis of homosexual and bisexual behavior in European countries, Sandfort (1998) concludes that most people with homosexual experience have also had heterosexual experience in some form at some point in their lives. A form of bisexuality prevalent in early adulthood may represent a traditional phase in which preferences are tested through experimentation with different lifestyles and relationships (Wellings et al., 1994). The question in the present study is not when or how often a particular individual has chosen the same or opposite sex as the objects) of his or her sexual attention; rather, this study aims to explore the extent to which people report exclusively or mostly to have had sexual feelings for and/or have had sex with persons of the same gender and opposite gender. Sandfort (1998) concluded that little is known about respondents who report homosexual experience in population studies. Stokes, Variable, and McKirnan (1997) also concluded that little is known about what distinguishes exclusively homosexual persons from bisexual persons. This study aims to contribute to the body of knowledge in these areas. In past decades it has become more and more common to regard the choice of same-gender sex partners as part of a broad spectrum of sexual expressions (Wellings et al., 1994). The present study aims to investigate sexual diversity in groups of the urban Norwegian population. The terms exclusively homosexual, exclusively heterosexual, and bisexual will be used in the text to describe subgroups of the population. However, this is not necessarily equivalent to sexual orientation. To avoid the theoretically problematic concept of sexual orientation and at the same time broaden the concept of sexual experience, the gender-relative part of the expression of sexual diversity is operationalized as four different aspects of gender preference: sexual fantasies about, having sexual conduct with, and falling in love with. The purpose of this study is thus to explore sexual diversity in urban Norwegians. One guiding question is what is the prevalence of sexual experience in terms of having sexual fantasies about, feeling sexual attraction toward, having sex with, and falling in love with persons of the same gender? Another question is what is the relationship between these four different aspects of sexual diversity, and are they influenced by the same factors related to social background, attitudes, and lifestyles? METHODS Study Sample In November 1997, the Central Bureau of Statistics drew a random sample of 5,000 persons aged 18-49 years residing in Norway's capital, Oslo, from the national population register to participate in a survey on sexual behavior, the use of contraception, and lifestyle (Central Bureau of Statistics, 1997). Data was obtained by postal, anonymous, self-administered questionnaires. The questionnaire contained questions referring to social background, lifestyle, leisure-time activities, alcohol consumption, type and amount of sexual experience, use of STD protection and birth control, beliefs about the use of oral contraception (OC), and attitudes toward sexuality. Most of the questions were of a multiple-choice type. Several of the questions were identical to those used in recent studies of sexual behavior in Sweden, Finland, and the United Kingdom (Johnson, Wadsworth, Wellings, & Field, 1994; Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 1995; Lewin, Fugl-Meyer, Helmius, Lalos, & Mansson, 1997). One reminder was sent to all individuals during the ensuing two weeks. Of the original study sample, 199 individuals did not receive the questionnaire because they were dead, had moved permanently out of the city, or lived in institutions. Of the 4,801 persons in the remaining sample, 2,135 answered the questionnaire, giving a response rate of 44.5%. The response rate was lower among men than among women, lowest among 18- to 24-year-olds, and highest among 30- to 34-year-olds. The response rate was 60.6% among persons with university education, compared to 30.7% among persons with 9 years or less of formal education. Statistical Analyses In the statistical analyses of the data, SPSS 10.0 was employed. In addition to contingency table analysis, discriminant analysis and exploratory factor analysis were applied to analyze the different problems under study. Factor analysis is frequently used to study underlying structure or basic dimensions of a set of variables (Kim & Mueller, 1978). Furthermore, factor analysis is a method of condensing a larger set of observed variables into a smaller number of conceptual variables, designated factors, or dimensions. These factors or dimensions have themselves not been directly observed. The factor loadings for each variable indicate the strength of association with each of the factors. The results of the analysis were reported in terms of a varimax rotated factor matrix. To explore the differences in lifestyle, attitudes, and behavior between heterosexuals, homosexuals, and bisexuals, discriminant analyses were performed on six groups of respondents: men and women with exclusively heterosexual experiences, men and women with exclusively homosexual experiences, and men and women with bisexual experiences. The variables were entered stepwise into the analysis by Wilk's lambda method; that is, at each step the variable that resulted in the smallest Wilk's lambda for the discriminant function was selected for entry (Klecka, 1980). A lambda of 1 occurs when the mean of the discriminant scores is the same in all groups and there is no between-groups variability. Wilk's lambda provides a test of the null hypothesis that the population means are equal. The larger lambda is, the less discriminating power is present. In discriminant analysis certain assumptions about the data must be met. First, each group must be a sample from a multivariate normal population. Second, the population covariance matrices must be equal. Discriminant analysis assumes that the variables are jointly normally distributed. If some of the variables in the analysis have markedly nonnormal distributions, as is the case in this study, one must take into account that the multivariate normality assumption may be violated. To test for equality of the group covariance matrices, we performed Box's M test. Variables Sexual diversity. The questions on sexual diversity in this survey were adopted from the Swedish Sexual Behaviour Survey (MAnsson, 1997). Homosexual experiences were assessed in terms of fantasies, attraction, behavior, and love. The questions were developed as a version of the Kinsey Scale of Sexual Orientation (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948; Sell, 1997) and read as follows: (a) "When you think about the persons you have had sexual fantasies about, which gender have they had?"; (b) "When you think about the persons you have been sexually attracted to, which gender have they had?", (c) "When you think about the persons you have had sex with, which gender have they had?"; and (d) "When you think about the persons you have been in love with, which gender have they been?" For each of these questions the response categories were I = always women; 2 = most often women, sometimes men; 3 = equally often men and women; 4 = most often men, sometimes women; and 5 = always men. We created a new variable with the categories I = exclusively opposite gender, 2 = bisexual, and 3 = exclusively homosexual. Lifestyle. According to Gullestad (1989), Norwegian lifestyles are influenced by international trends. At the same time they are strongly connected to our country's history and living conditions. Apart from cooking and dressing, Norwegians tend to emphasize creating a personal home decoration. The symbolic value of these elements is high, and many put a lot of emotional effort and money into them. As Norwegians generally have a tendency to compare themselves to others of the same social status and situation as themselves, the following question was asked to measure lifestyle in this survey: "Do you think you spend more money than most people do on any of the following articles?" The respondents could make a mark for 26 different activities; a mark was interpreted as a yes, and no mark was interpreted as a no. An exploratory factor analysis resulted in eight factors. However, as two of these factors were difficult to interpret, a six-factor solution was chosen. A varimax rotated principal component analysis resulted in the following dimensions of lifestyle (n = 2,135): 1. Factor 1-Restaurants (Eigenvalue 2.3, variance 8.7%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.65): dining at restaurants (factor score 0.70); pubs, bars, cafes (factor score 0.67); clothes (factor score 0.48); travelling (factor score 0.47); cinema (factor score 0.38). 2. Factor 2-House and Home (Eigenvalue 2.1, variance 7.9%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.57): house and home (factor score 0.64); permanent partner (factor score 0.55); motors, cars, equipment (factor score 0.53); computers, electronic equipment (factor score 0.51); cooking at home (factor score 0.49); boats (factor score 0.37). 3. Factor 3-Art and Culture (Eigenvalue 2.0, variance 7.7%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.54): theatre (factor score 0.73); opera (factor score 0.69); arts (factor score 0.53); books (factor score 0.46); quality wines (factor score 0.37). 4. Factor 4-Sex, Drugs, and Rock 'n Roll (Eigenvalue 2.0, variance 7.5%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.52): illegal drugs (factor score 0.61); dance, disco (factor score 0.61), alcohol (factor score 0.55); casual partners (factor score 0.51); music (factor score 0.38). 5. Factor 5-Beauty and Health (Eigenvalue 1.7, variance 6.4%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.44): cosmetics and hygiene products (factor score 0.62); healthy products (factor score 0.62); healthy food (factor score 0.53). 6. Factor 6-Physical Activity (Eigenvalue 1.4, variance 5.5%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.53): outdoor leisure time activities (factor score 0.76); physical exercise or activities and equipment (factor score 0.73). Attitudes toward expressions of sexuality. As an introduction to the eight questions on attitudes towards various expressions of sexuality, the respondents read the following text: "The expressions of sexuality are shown to vary, and people become sexually stimulated by different things. Below follows some descriptions of certain things one may become sexually aroused by. We want to know what you think about each of these things: Is there something you may want to do yourself, something you do not want to do yourself but can accept that others do-for instance your partner-or is it something you think is absolutely unacceptable?" The response categories were "would want to do it myself"; "would not want to do it myself, but would accept it if my partner wanted to"; "would not want to do it myself and cannot accept my partner doing it, but I accept that others functions this way"; and "unacceptable." In this study a varimax rotated principal component analysis resulted in the following two attitude dimensions (n = 1,993): 1. Factor ]-Socially Acceptable Expressions of Sexuality (Eigenvalue 2.1, variance 26.2%, Cronbach's alpha = 0.65): being positive toward becoming sexually stimulated by persons of the same gender (factor score 0.68; 21% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), dominance and submission (factor score 0.68; 22% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), cross-dressing (factor score 0.67; 25% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), and shoes and underwear (factor score 0.66; 25% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them). 2. Factor 2-Socially Unacceptable Expressions of Sexuality (Eigenvalue 2.0, variance 25. 1 %, Cronbach's alpha = 0.63): being positive toward becoming sexually stimulated by violence (factor score 0.78; 89% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), flashing of the naked body (factor score 0.70; 69% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), pain (factor score 0.61: 53% of the respondents claimed this was unacceptable to them), and spying on what others do sexually (factor score 0.58; 51% of the respon-dents claimed this was unacceptable to them). RESULTS Table 1 shows the percentage of the respondents who reported experience with the four different aspects of sexual diversity, by gender. A total of 92.5% reported to have been in love with only persons of the opposite gender, 90.9% had had sex with only the opposite gender, 87.9% reported to have felt sexually attracted to only the opposite gender, and 75.9% had fantasized about having sex with only the opposite gender. More women than men reported never to have had bisexual fantasies or felt homosexual attraction, but there was no gender difference in bisexual contact and love. More men than women reported exclusively homosexual experiences. Very few respondents reported experiences equally often with partners of the same and opposite genders. [IMAGE TABLE] Captioned as: Table 1. [IMAGE TABLE] Captioned as: Table 2 With one exception, there was no statistically significant difference in the reporting on the four different aspects of sexual diversity, by age group or by level of education. The exception was the bivariate relationship between age and sexual conduct. Bisexual experience increased from 4.8% among the 18- to 24-year-olds to 11.1% among the 40- to 44-- year-olds, and decreased to 7.4% in the age group of 45 to 49 years. Although not statistically significant, people with low education were more likely to report homosexual experience than were people with high education. To see if the four different measures of sexual diversity were overlapping, we performed a correlation analysis. All correlations were high. The strongest correlation was found between the gender of persons having had sex with and the gender of persons having been sexually attracted to (Pearson's r = 0.85), and the least strong correlation was between the gender of persons having had sexual fantasies about and the gender of persons having been in love with (Pearson's r = 0.74). When comparing groups of respondents who had experienced exclusively heterosexual conduct, bisexual conduct, and exclusively homosexual conduct on various behavioral measures, some differences emerged (Table 2). Compared to respondents who had had exclusively heterosexual contact, respondents reporting exclusively homosexual contact had a statistically significant longer time interval since the most recent intercourse, a higher frequency of anal sex (males) and masturbation during the past 30 days, and a lower age at orgasm debut. Note that the mean estimates for exclusively homosexual contact are calculated on the basis of 22 to 26 men and six to eight women. The estimates thus express mainly male, and not female, behavior. Compared to respondents classified as exclusively heterosexual, those classified as bisexual reported a higher number of lifetime partners and partners during the past 12 months, longer time since most recent intercourse, higher monthly frequency of masturbation and oral and anal sex, lower age of masturbation and orgasm debut, and a higher number of sexual activities performed during the most recent intercourse. When comparing respondents in the homosexual and bisexual group, only one statistically significant difference emerged. Those classified as bisexuals reported a higher median number of lifetime partners than did those classified as homosexuals. However, some in the homosexual group reported very high numbers, so their mean was higher. A cumulative plot was performed, and this plot showed that the highest number of lifetime partners was among the homosexuals. Also, the standard deviation was higher in the homosexual than in the bisexual group, indicating greater variation among those in the group of exclusive homosexuals. It is interesting to note that the median number of lifetime partners of bisexuals is almost exactly double that of the exclusively heterosexual or homosexual groups. This seems to underpin Woody Allen's words that "by being bisexual you double your chances for Saturday night." To explore the differences in attitudes, lifestyle, and sexual behavior between respondents classified as heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual, discriminant analyses were performed for each of the outcome variables. The results from these analyses in terms of Wilk's lambda of the functions, the group centroids, and the standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients of the variables included in the model are presented in Table 3. The standardized canonical discriminant function coefficients show the relative association between the discriminating variables and the discriminant functions. Except for the analysis on sexual conduct among men, two statistically significant discriminant functions were identified (see Wilk's lambda). Among women, the second discriminant factor of the four outcome variables was not statistically significant. In all four analyses, the first discriminant function was dominated by variables on attitudes towards expressions of sexuality (in men and women), frequency of anal intercourse (in men), and dimensions of lifestyle (in men). For this reason the first discriminant function was called attitudes & lifestyle. As can be seen from the magnitude of the standardized canonical discriminant coefficients across the four different analyses, the second discriminant function was dominated by the variable on number of lifetime partners. In accordance with this finding, the discriminant function was called sexual history. However, this latter function carries much less weight and its importance should not be overemphasized. Box's test of equality of covariance matrices was performed to test the null hypothesis of equal population covariance matrices in each discriminant analysis. All tests were statistically significant, leading us to reject the null hypothesis. To focus more on the group differences, the group centroids were studied. Group centroids are the mean discriminant scores for each group on the respective functions. The centroids summarize the group locations in space defined by the discriminant functions. This is visualized in Figure 1, where the group centroids are plotted on a graph defined by the two discriminant functions in each of the four analyses. Note that the axes do not have exactly the same definition in all eight cases. For all four outcome variables, heterosexual men and women grouped in the direction of the negative pole but close to the point of intersection between the two function lines on the attitudes & lifestyle function, and close to the point of intersection between the two functions on the sexual history function. In terms of love, fantasies, sex, and attraction, exclusively homosexual men (denoted gay men in Figure 1) grouped close to the positive pole on both functions. Exclusively homosexual women (denoted lesbians in Figure 1) grouped close to the positive pole on the attitudes & lifestyle function, and to the negative pole on the sexual history function for all aspects of sexual diversity (except sexual fantasies where lesbians grouped close to the positive pole on function 2). In terms of love, sex, and attraction, bisexual men and women tended to group close to the positive pole on the attitude and lifestyle function. However, bisexual men grouped on the negative pole on the sexual history function and bisexual women grouped close to zero. As indicated by Wilk's lambda for function 2, little between-groups variability on sexual conduct, attraction, and love existed among the groups of women. DISCUSSION The results from our study show that the great majority of the respondents claim they have exclusively heterosexual experiences, but a small group of people who have collected various types of homosexual experiences also exist parallel to this majority. Our results have shown that except for homosexual fantasies in women, homosexual experiences in terms of love, attraction, and actual contact is rare in the population. Exclusive homosexual experiences appear to be even more rare than bisexual experiences. Before discussing these findings in detail, a comment on response bias and generalizability is appropriate since the low response rate may pose a problem for comparison with other studies if nonresponses are selective. [IMAGE TABLE] Captioned as: Table 3. [IMAGE CHART] Captioned as: Figure 1. Sexual behavior studies among representative samples of 10,000 Norwegians were performed in 1987, 1992, and 1997. The response rate in this study was lower than in the sexual behavior surveys in 1987 (63%) and in 1992 (48%), but higher that that in 1997 (38%; Stigum, Magnus, Harris, Samuelsen, & Bakketeig, 1997; Stigum et al., 2000). The previous studies in 1987 and 1992 indicate that response bias is not a major problem in this kind of sexual survey (Stigum, 1997; Stigum et al., 1997; Stigum, Magnus, Veierod, & Bakketeig, 1995). In the 1992 study, a subsample survey to assess response bias was performed. A short questionnaire asking whether the recipient had responded to the original survey was sent to 2,000 of the original 10,000 subjects. The majority of subjects gave reasons for not responding that were generally unrelated to differences in sexual behavior. The 1992 survey also kept track of the date the questionnaires were received. The distribution fell into two easily distinguishable parts: before and after reminders were sent. Several measures of sexual behavior were the same in the two groups. The underlying interpretation was that the three groups-response before reminders, response after reminders, and nonresponse-represented increasing magnitude of "unwillingness to respond." In this terminology, a bias occurs if the variable of interest is associated with unwillingness to respond. If the two first groups are similar with respect to the variables of interest, then we expect responders and nonresponders also to be similar. A Danish survey on sexual behavior also found similar patterns for the group that responded before and the group that responded after a reminder (Melbye & Biggar, 1992). Comparison with other studies on sexual behavior in Norway, Sweden, and Finland also suggests that nonresponse is close to random with respect to sexual behavior (Kontula & Haavio-Mannila, 1995; Lewin et al., 1997; Stigum et al.. 1997). The findings from this survey show no dramatic deviance from these studies, for instance with regard to the proportion having experience of sexual intercourse and use of condoms. That the majority of the respondents had medium or higher education is most likely due to the fact that the majority of Oslo's population has medium or higher education (Central Bureau of Statistics, 1997). In Oslo, 9.4% of the population has lower education, 47.7% has medium education, and 42.6% has higher education. To further explore the generalizability of our results, we used unpublished data from subsamples of the 1987 to 1997 National Institute for Public Health's sexual behavior surveys as comparison. Of persons 18 to 49 years old with residence in Oslo, the following percentages reported homosexual experience: 7.1% in 1987 (7.5% men, 6.6% women, n = 550, p > 0.05), 11.7% in 1992 (15.1% men, 9.3% women, n = 480, p < 0.05) and 10.2% in 1997 (8.1% men, 11.6% women, n = 512, p > 0.05). There was a statistically significant difference in the reporting between the three surveys (p < 0.05).1 In our Oslo study, 9.1% reported homosexual conduct (10.3% of the men, 7.8% of the women). Some remarks on the possibility of information bias shall also be made. How homosexuality is viewed socially is likely to influence the responses to the questions we asked (Wellings et al., 1994). People with homosexual experiences may have been less willing to participate in the survey. Furthermore, since homosexuality in Norway still must be characterized as being socially stigmatized, it is likely to be underreported rather than overreported. However, it is likely that attitudes towards homosexuality have become more liberal in the wake of the AIDS epidemic. Accordingly, the percentage reporting homosexual experience is higher in the 1990s than in the 1980s (Stigum et al., 2000). However, we still do not know how large the underreporting is at present. By comparing with the other sexual behavior surveys, however, we may assume that if underreporting exists, it seems fairly similar in many countries, and irrespective of whether data were collected by postal questionnaires or by personal interviews. On this basis it is concluded that although the low response rate creates the possibility of selection bias, the 1992 subsample survey, the studies comparing responses given before and after reminder, and comparison with measures of homosexual experience in other Norwegian sexual behavior surveys point to nonresponse being nonselective. However, an information bias in terms of underreporting may exist. In terms of gender of persons having been in love with, had fantasies about, had sex with, and felt attracted to, the reporting on exclusive homosexuality was rare, and less frequently reported by women than by men. This has also been found in other studies (Sandfort, 1998; Wellings et al., 1994). The gender differences may be explained by the fact that female sexuality is generally more strictly controlled and sanctioned than male sexuality is (Sandfort, 1998). However, in this material nearly one in three women reported having had homosexual fantasies. This is similar to the findings from Sweden (Mansson, 1997). One proposed explanation for the high prevalence among women has been that the fantasy reflects a common fantasy among heterosexual men about two women and one man (Mansson, 1997; Stoltenberg, 1990). This so-called menage a trios can be regarded as part of the heterosexual script. A sexual script names the actors and charts what is appropriate behavior and in what circumstances this behavior may occur (Gagnon & Simon, 1977). Within this script, the homosexual behavior takes place with the (heterosexual) women as actors and the man as a participant observer. The homosexual conduct may thus also be performed to increase the pleasure of the heterosexual man (Mansson, 1997). The opposite scenario with two men and an observing woman is likely to represent a deviation from the valid heterosexual script. This is supported by the fact that in this material more men (25.1%) than women (3.7%) claim they would accept that their partner was sexually attracted to a person of the same gender. It is also likely that the gender differences observed in this study reflect underlying gender differences in homophobia. It is commonly viewed that men in Western societies are more afraid of homosexual stigmatization (e.g., when expressing physical affection toward other men, exposing nakedness in the company of other men, and so forth) than are women (Schmidt, Klusmann, Zeitzschel, & Large, 1994). Women are commonly socialized into greater comfort about expressing affection toward others and becoming intimate in nonsexual contexts, both physically and verbally, regardless of the other person's gender. Because women have a higher level of intimacy in relations with same-sex persons, one might think that sexual feelings could more easily enter into the relationships, in terms of fantasies if not in terms of actual sexual contacts. The latter might be attributed to the hypothesis that the social control of sexuality is less strict upon fantasies and attraction than upon behavior. In this study, age was statistically significantly related only to having had sex with a person of the same gender: The percentage who had had homosexual conduct was lowest among the young and highest among those 40 to 44 years old. In their study from the United Kingdom, Wellings et al. (1994) concluded that the age differences they observed seemed to reflect a general liberalizing effect of the early 1960s following the deliberalization of the law against same-sex sexuality. Homosexual experience was thus found to be more a cohort effect than an age effect (Wellings et al., 1994). The results from this study support this conclusion. Although not statistically significant, respondents in this study with low education were more likely to report homosexual experience than were people with high education. In the literature, it seems it is most commonly found that the prevalence of homosexual behavior is highest in the group with high education. One explanation has been that the tolerance and acceptance for homosexuality increases with social status (Wellings et al., 1994). However, the nature and direction of this relationship is poorly understood (Sandfort, 1998), and the opposite trend is also reported, or the difference between the various education groups is not statistically significant. Even though prevalences of homosexual experiences may be influenced by how homosexuality is viewed socially, the association between such experiences and predictors may not suffer negative influences. Homosexual experiences in terms of love, fantasies, attraction, and contact were connected to and influenced by lifestyle factors and attitudes toward sexual expressions. Excluding vaginal intercourse, persons who have had bisexual or exclusively homosexual experiences appear to be more sexually active than persons exclusively heterosexual do. We found a decline in the percentage of respondents who reported homosexual experiences, from fantasies to attraction to sex and lastly to love. In people's perception of the world, love may be interpreted as signifying a more stable emotional state than the more situational, physically oriented, and sexually explicit categories of attraction towards, fantasies about, and sexual contact with. At the same time the correlation between the four aspects of sexual diversity was very high. The high degree of correlation between sexual attraction, fantasies, love, and sexual experience is of interest in the context of social and cultural influences on sexual orientation. It may be that that the stigmatization of homosexuality would discourage activity and inhibit behavior but not attraction, fantasies, or love. In the U.K. no major discordance between preference and experience was found. As in the English study (Wellings et al., 1994), there is no support in our study for the theory that large proportions of the population harbor unrealized fantasies about samegender sex. In our study the highest numbers of lifetime partners were found in the homosexual group. This has also been reported in other studies (Stokes et al., 1997), and may be explained by contextual or subcultural factors such as acceptance for having causal and anonymous partners versus lovers. Furthermore, a common finding in sexual behavior studies is a broad repertoire of sexual acts amongst those with same-gender partners (Wellings et al., 1994). This finding was partly confirmed in our study. It seems that persons who have had bisexual contact have a broader sexual repertoire than those who are exclusively homosexual or heterosexual. The reasons for this difference may be multiple and include psychological, physiological, and social factors. Lifestyle may be defined as the distinctive pattern of personal and social behavior characteristic of an individual or a group (Veal, 1993). Anal intercourse is traditionally associated with male homosexuality, and may also be interpreted as part of a gay (subcultural) lifestyle. In this study, lifestyles connected to beauty and health, physical activity, and art and culture were also shown to differ between groups of heterosexual men, bisexual men, and homosexual men. It is likely that there is greater tolerance and acceptance for expressing homosexuality in some environments-ne of these being intellectual and creative environments-than in others. It may be that the ideology about equality is more deeply anchored in these groups of the population than in others (Trien & Hovland, 1998). The Nordic sexual culture is characterized by an ideology of equality between individuals and groups of individuals (Lewin, 1979). This implies that ideally, men and women as well as homosexuals and heterosexuals have the same rights to express their sexuality, and the same duties. There is, however, a discrepancy between the real and the ideal. This is shown to be the case in sports, where the social climate in several areas seems rather homophobic (Griffin, 1992; Eng, 1997; Kolnes, 2000; Fasting, 2000). Homosexual men may thus avoid participation in lifestyles connected to sports. Lifestyles are shown to be connected to social background factors and to attitudes (Aaro, 1986). Furthermore, attitudes toward sexuality may be regarded as profoundly connected to an individual's values, opinions, and personality (Lewin & Helmius, 1983). Attitudes toward what may be termed the socially acceptable expressions of sexuality was the most significant variable separating groups of exclusively heterosexual, bisexual, and exclusively homosexual men and women on sexual diversity. However, this does not necessarily imply that attitudes are the cause of experience-seeking. It may just as well be that an attitudinal change may take place following experiences (Bem, 1970). The plotting of the group centroids gives an image of distinct systems of attitudes and lifestyles, and sexual history, between exclusive heterosexuals, bisexuals, lesbians, and gay men. The mapping of groups shows the distance between these systems. Of particular interest is how the exclusive heterosexuals are different from the other groups on attitudes and lifestyle, and how gay men and lesbians position themselves in different compartments on the sexual history axis. This may indicate that gay men and lesbian women accumulate sexual experiences according to different patterns, and a high partner turnover represents a male rather than a female pattern. In sum, however, it is attitudes toward various expressions of sexuality which seem most important in separating groups of persons with and without homosexual experience. 1 It should be noted that homosexual experience in 1987 and in 1992 was measured by the question "Have you ever had any kind of sexual contact with a person of the same gender?", with response categories yes and no. In 1997 it was measured the same way as in this study and recoded to a dichotomous variable in the analysis above. REFERENCES Aaro, L. E. (1986). Health behaviour and socioeconomic status: A survey among the adult population in Norway. Unpublished master's thesis. University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway. Bem, D. J. (1970). Beliefs, attitudes and human affaires. Belmont: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Central Bureau of Statistics. (1997). The population's highest level of education. Oslo: Author. Eng, H. (1997, June). Queer studies and sport. Paper presented at the International Sociology Sport Association Symposium, Oslo, Norway. Fasting, K. (2000). Homofobi, kvinner og eliteidrett: Norske og svenske lotballspilleres erfaringer [Homophobia, women and top athletes: Norwegian and Swedish soccer players' experiences]. Kunnskap om Idrett, 4(3), 21-28. Gagnon, J. H., & Simon, W. (1977). Sexual conduct: The social sources of human sexuality. Chicago: Aldine. Griffin, P (1992). Changing the game: Homophobia, sexism, and lesbians in sport. Quest, 44, 251-265. Gullestad, M. (1989). Kultur og hverdagsliv [Culture and everyday life]. Oslo: Universitetsforlaget AS. Johnson, A. M., Wadsworth, J., Wellings, K., & Field, J. (1994). Sexual attitudes and lifestyles. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Kim, J. E., & Mueller, C. V. (1978). Factor analysis: Statistical methods and practical issues (Sage University paper series on quantitative applications in the social sciences, 07-017). London: Sage Publications. Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behaviour in the human male. Philadephia: W. B. Saunders. Klecka, W. R. (1980). Discriminant analysis (Sage University paper series on quantitative applications in the social sciences). London: Sage Publications. Kolnes, L. J. (2000). Heterosexuality as organizing principle in women's sport. Kunnskap om Idrett, 3, 5-19. Kontula, O., & Haavio-Mannila, E. (1995). Sexual pleasures: Enhancement of sex life in Finland, 1971-1992. Aldershot: Dartmouth Publishing Company Ltd. Lewin, B. (1979). Om ogift samboende i Sverige; Med tonvikt pa samtida forhallanden [On unmarried cohabitation in Sweden; With emphasis on contemporary issues]. Uppsala: Almqvist & Wiksell International. Lewin, B. (Ed.), Fugl-Meyer, K., Helmius, G., Lalos, A., & Mansson, S. A. (1997). Sex i Sverige: Om sexuallivet i Sverige 1996 [Sex in Sweden: On the sex-life in Sweden 1996]. Uppsala: Uppsala University Press. Lewin, B., & Helmius, G. (1983). Ungdom och sexualitet: En sociologisk studie av ungdoms sexuella f6restallingar och erfarenheter [Youth and sexuality: A sociological study of young people's sexual beliefs and experiences]. Uppsala: Uppsala University Press. Mansson, S. A. (1997). Sexuella forhallanden mellan manniskor av samma kon [Sexual relationships between persons of the same gender]. In B. Lewin (Ed.), Sex i Sverige: Om sexuallivet i Sverige 1996 (pp. 169-183). Uppsala: Uppsala University Press. Melbye, M., & Biggar, R. J. (1992). Interactions between persons at risk for AIDS and the general population in Denmark. American Journal of Epidemiology, 135(6), 593-602. Sandfort, T. (1998). Homosexual and bisexual behaviour in European countries. In M. Hubert, N. Bajos, & T. Sandfort (Eds.), Sexual behaviour and HIV/AIDS in Europe (pp. 68105). London: UCL Press. Schmidt, G., Klusmann, D., Zeitzschel, U., & Lange, C. (1994). Changes in adolescents' sexuality between 1970 and 1990 in West Germany. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23(5), 489-513. Sell, R. L. (1997). Defining and measuring sexual orientation: A review. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26(6), 643-658. Stigum, H. (1997). Mathematical models for the spread of sexually transmitted diseases using sexual behaviour data. Norwegian Journal of Epidemiology, 7(Suppl. 5). Stigum, H., Magnus, P, Harris, J. R., Samuelsen, S. O., & Bakketeig, L. S. (1997). Frequency of sexual partner change in a Norwegian population: Data distribution and covariates. American Journal of Epidemiology, 145,636-643. Stigum, H., Magnus, P., Veierod, M., & Bakketeig, L. S. (1995). Impact on sexually transmitted diseases spread of increased condom use by young females, 1987-1992. International Journal of Epidemiology, 24, 813-820. Stigum, H., Traen, B., & Magnus, P. (2000). Rapport fra seksualvaneundersokelsene i 1987, 1992 og 1997 [Report from the sexual behavior surveys in 1987, 1992 and 1997]. Oslo: National Institute of Public Health. Stokes, J. P., Variable, P., & McKirnan, D. J. (1997). Comparing gay and bisexual men on sexual behavior, condom use, and psychosocial variables related to HIV/AIDS. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 26(4), 383-397. Stoltenberg, J. (1990). Pornography and freedom. In M. Kimmel (Ed.), Men confront pornography (p. 66). New York: Crown Publishers. Treen, B., & Hovland, A. (1998). Games people play: Sex, alcohol and condom use among urban Norwegians. Contemporary Drug Problems, 25, 3-48. Veal, A. J. (1993). The concept of lifestyle: A review. Leisure Studies, 12 (4), 233-252. Wellings, K., Wadsworth, J., & Johnson, A. (1994). Sexual diversity and homosexual behaviour. In A. M. Johnson, J. Wadsworth, K. Wellings, & J. Field (Eds.), Sexual attitudes and lifestyles (pp. 183-224). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Manuscript accepted February 18. 2002