Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 Faithfulness and Perception in Loanword Adaptation: A Case Study from Hawaiian Allison Adler, MIT anadler@mit.edu 1. Why study loans from English into Hawaiian? o We can examine the treatment of sounds borrowed into a language with a uniquely limited consonant inventory: No coronal stops: *t, d No oral fricatives: *f, v, s, z, , Table 1: Consonant inventory of Hawaiian (and English) labial coronal dorsal stop p (b) (t, d) k (g) fricative (f, v) (s, z, , ) nasal m n glide w liquid laryngeal h () l (r) o Hawaiian also has restricted syllable structure: (C)V(V) only o So… how are non-native segments adapted? How are consonant clusters and codas adapted? 2. Methods & Data o Preliminary corpus study: approx. 250 lexicalized loans from English into Hawaiian, taken from dictionary (Pukui & Elbert, 1979) o Elicited adaptations: non-lexicalized borrowings elicited from two native EnglishHawaiian bilinguals (n=200 and n=100) 3. Selected findings of this study o Positional variation: Consonants adjacent to a vowel or sonorant are overwhelmingly preserved – i.e., there is frequent epenthesis to save these segments; other consonants variably deleted o Unattested featural adaptations: changes in sonority, nasality: */b/m, */t/n, l change in major articulator: */t,d/p, *//m o Attested/frequent featural adaptations: devoicing, change in place of articulation blow [polóu] colby [kolopí:] tinker [kiníkə] delve [ke:lewə] green [kəlínə] Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 o Fricatives variably map to [h] or nearest stop (modulo place of articulation): variation in singleton onset and coda fricatives C stop Ch crease kəlíki crease kəlíki shark ká:like pressure pəlé:hu fork póka proof pulúhu half hápə floor holólə o Strident fricatives follow this pattern but can also exhibit deletion in non-salient positions: variation in /sC/ clusters: s k, h, Ø speech kəpíke ~ həpíke despot kepáe story kokóli ~ kóli dispatch kipá:ki stymie həkáimi beast píkə sloop kəlúpe ~ həlúpe grasp kələhépe non-prevocalic stridents (n=89): s k: 37% s h: 29% s Ø: 34% 4. Existing approaches to loanword adaptation 4.1 Phonological: Theory of Constraints and Repair Strategies: Paradis (1988, et seq.) Relies on standard feature geometry to motivate phonological closeness as the basis for segmental adaptations Principles of Minimality, Preservation, Threshold in loan adaptation 4.2 Perceptual: P-map: Steriade (2001); also Fleischhacker (2000), Kang (2002), Kenstowicz (2001, 2003) Hypothesis: adaptations in loanwords are generally perceptually-based; highly perceptible changes or deletions are avoided. Can capture positional asymmetries due to differential cue distribution by context cf. perceptual similarity/confusability of Steriade (2001) and perceptual cue studies (e.g., Steriade (1999), Côté (2000), Fleischhacker (2000)) and early work on speech perception. 4.3 Articulatory: Perceptual Assimilation Model: Best (1994, 1995, et seq.) Articulatory phonology motivates a model of non-native speech perception: assumes that listeners access gestural information in speech stream and categorize non-native sounds based on articulatory similarity to native phonological categories Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 5. How do existing approaches fare on this data? 5.1 Adaptation of coronal stops: 5.1.1 Phonological approach (TCRS): Problem: Counting feature changes yields the wrong result – not all changes are perceptually equal: predict t k or p, d n Table 2: Predicted adaptation of coronal stops under TCRS Constraint: *[cor], [-son] Predicted repair: t k, p; d n Adaptation voiceless /t/ Cp 1. delink coronal 2. insert labial Ck 1. delink coronal 2. insert dorsal Cn 1. insert [+son] 2. insert [+nasal] 3. delink [-voice]; insert [+voice] voiced /d/ 1. delink coronal 2. insert labial 3. delink [+voice]; insert [-voice] 1. delink coronal 2. insert dorsal 3. delink [+voice]; insert [-voice] 1. insert [+son] 2. insert [+nasal] 5.1.2 Perceptual approach (P-map): Predictions based on cue distribution by context are upheld, but perceptual information alone is not definitive: t k or p Correctly excludes mapping to sonorants [n, l]: change in sonority is highly perceptible and therefore disfavored CAN account for positionally-based deletion based on reduced perceptibility of place of articulation of word-final, unreleased stops. Problem: acoustic/perceptual info does not conclusively choose between [p, k] as possible outputs: perception studies of stop recognition and confusability are not able to single out any cue from release burst or formant transitions as constant across contexts 5.1.3 Perceptual Assimilation Model: Correctly selects as optimal the mapping which changes place of articulation but not major articulator: t k Based on “tube geometry” structure (see Diagram 1 below), the most minimal change is from coronal to dorsal, as observed in the data Problem: tube geometry not conclusive on minimal change for all segments Beyond the oral node, minimal featural change unclear: what determines that changing the position of the glottis (voiced vs. unvoiced) is more minimal than changing the position of the velum (oral vs. nasal)? Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 Diagram 1: Tube geometric representation of the oral cavity Oral Cavity Lips Tongue (labial) Central Tip (coronal) Lateral Body (dorsal) 5.2 Adaptation of fricatives: 5.2.1 TCRS: Variation predicted for /s/ – but not the observed mappings Table 3: Predicted adaptation of /s/ under TCRS Constraint: *[+strident] Predicted repair: s l? otherwise, variation sp Delink: 1. +strident 2. +continuant 3. Coronal sk Delink: 1. +strident 2. +continuant 3. Coronal Insert 4. Labial Insert 4. Dorsal sn sl Delink: Delink: 1. +strident 1. +strident 2. +continuant Insert 3. +sonorant 4. +nasal (5. +voice) Insert 2. +sonorant 3. +lateral (4. +voice) sh Delink: 1. Oral node 2. +strident 3. +consonantal Insert 4. +sonorant Preservation Principle favors adaptation with the least featural deletion has taken place – but these mappings are unattested (s n, l) Mapping to [k] and [p] equal: Coronal, Dorsal, Labial are all equidistant in the feature geometry Another problem: subjective counting of feature changes – i.e., what counts as a “step” in the repair process? “Default insertion” of features not always counted: depending on what counts as a step, prediction changes How to characterize “debuccalization” to [h]? 5.2.2 Perceptual approach: Incorrectly predicts retention of all stridents because of their high inherent salience Otherwise, positional variation is expected due to weakness of cues in wordfinal or non-prevocalic position. Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 5.2.3 PAM: Predictions unclear for unfamiliar stricture type; also, system not designed to account for segments in context PAM predicts that fricatives will be treated as speech-like even if they cannot be assimilated to a single native category BUT it is not clear which features will be targeted in attempting to map an unfamiliar stricture type to a native sound. Problem: system describes context-free categorization of sounds, thus is not designed to handle positionally-based variation of strident fricatives. 6. Combining articulatory and perceptual similarity: A comprehensive account Perceptual and articulatory information are necessary and complementary factors in forming the judgments of similarity which allow borrowers to select a mapping for non-native segments 6.1 Mapping of coronals /t,d/ k Articulatory information determines the closest mapping for the coronal stops even though perceptual information is not conclusive. Given the options available in Hawaiian, the optimal mapping preserves the major articulator by realizing /t/ as a lingual [k] rather than a labial [p] Cue-based, perceptual information informs judgments of similarity, predicting positional asymmetries in deletion vs. epenthesis 6.2 Mapping and deletion of stridents: /s/ k, h, Ø These findings do not actually call into question the high salience of stridents. A trade-off exists between the pressure to retain salient input segments (like stridents) and the need to change their features: Deletion is a viable option only because no native segment adequately captures exactly those features which make these segments highly salient. Thus, the feature loss/change is as dispreferred as segment loss Deletion is not viable for non-strident fricatives because, in the absence of the [+strident] feature, the available native options are closer (albeit still imperfect) mappings The cross-linguistics dispreference for deletion is a real phenomenon but it derives its power from the fact that languages are frequently able to map non-native segments without changing their most salient features. Deletion is a viable option only because no native segment adequately captures the salient features of stridents. The chosen mappings for input fricatives reflects a trade-off between retaining the presence of a stricture in the oral tract and retaining their salient [+continuant] feature. These two features are incompatible in the native Hawaiian inventory (recall that [h] is the only fricative in Hawaiian) An OT-based approach of violable constraints is well-suited to capturing the variation observed Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 6.2 The constraint system and selected tableaux 6.2.1 Adaptation of coronal stops Ident[±sonorant]: The output correspondent of input [sonorant] is [sonorant] Ident[articulator]: The output correspondent of input [articulator] is [articulator] Result: segments are mapped to a sound of equal sonority and same major articulator Tableau 1: Adaptation of coronal stops a. Ident[±son] >> Ident[place]: map to [-sonorant] /t/ Ident[±son] *t a. t b. n c. l d. ? p e. ? k Ident[place] *! *! *! * * b. Ident[artic], Ident[place]: map to [k], not [p] /t/ *t a. t b. h c. d. p e. k Ident[artic] Ident[place] *! *! *! * * * * *! 6.2.2 Adaptation of [+strident] fricatives Max-C[+strident]: A strident consonant in the input has a correspondent in the output Ident[±strident]: The output correspondent of input [strident] is [strident] Ranking: Ident[±stri], MaxC[+stri] Result: Deletion and strident feature change are equally (dis)favored Tableau 2: Adaptation of [+strident] fricatives *s Ident[±stri] MaxC[+stri] /s/ a. s b. p c. h d. k e. Ø *! * * * * Adler NELS 35 poster, October 2004 6.2.3 Adaptation of fricatives generally Max-fricative: A fricative consonant in the input has a correspondent in the output Ident[±continuant]: The output correspondent of input [continuant] is [continuant] Ranking: Ident[articulator], Ident[±continuant], Max-fricative Result: there is variation in preserving [+continuant] and the presence/location of a stricture in the oral tract Tableau 3: Adaptation of all fricatives a. deletion of stridents is not ruled out *s Ident[artic] Ident[±cont] Max-fric * * *! /s/ a. s b. p c. h d. k e. Ø *! * * b. deletion of nonstridents not optimal: Max/Ident[strident] irrelevant *f Max C[-stri] Ident[artic] Ident[±cont] Maxfric /f/ a. f b. Ø c. k d. h e. p *! *! * * * *! * 6.2.4 Positional/Contextual asymmetry Max-C/V: A consonant followed by a vowel in the input has a correspondent in the output Max-C//R, N: A consonant adjacent to a sonorant in the input has a correspondent in the output Ranking: Max-C/V, Max-C//R, N >> Ident Result: salient, well-cued consonants are retained despite the need for feature change