Action Research The effectiveness of learning listening to model speakers on pronunciation Wong Siu Fun Introduction Pronunciation is part of language teaching. The correct pronunciation can bring a systematic and well-structured spoken language. That is why pronunciation is very important to communication. With an appropriate pronunciation, message can be clearly conveyed. However, compared with the teaching of grammar, reading, writing, listening and even vocabulary, the teaching of pronunciation gains less teachers’ attention. Since pronunciation tends to be neglected by most English teachers, pronunciation problems are easily found in every student’s spoken English. After analyzing the recorded utterances elicited by various means: conversation, reading aloud, picture description, a common pronunciation error has been discovered in my school is the substitution of [d] as in “ladder” for [ð] as in “lather”. The research presented here focuses on two aspects: first, to examine the effectiveness of the most dominant and traditional strategy: listening to model speakers in dealing with the target pronunciation problem; second, to find out whether learners enjoy learning pronunciation through this approach. This research was done with forty-two Secondary Two students who are of mixed ability. Measurement of the subjects’ performance was made by means of one performance test. Interviews were conducted with the six selected subjects to find out the learners’ experiences and feelings towards the target approach. Rationale It is accepted that intelligibility is the most sensible pronunciation goal for most learners. Kenworthy stated that intelligibility means “ being understood by a listener at a given time in a given situation”( p.13 ). That means, the more words a listener can identify accurately when said by a particular speaker, the more intelligible that speaker is. Conversely, if a speaker substitutes one sound ( e.g., /d/ )for another ( e.g., / ð/ ), the result is that the listener hears a different word ( e.g., ladder ) from the one the speaker was aiming to say( e.g., lather ), then the speaker’s speech is unintelligible.( Kenworthy, 1987 ). 1 The pronunciation problems Chinese speakers tend to have are caused by the difference between the pronunciation of English and Chinese. In order to cope with that problem, Chinese learners tend to employ a learner pronunciation strategy ( i.e. sound substitutions ). For instance, as Chinese does not have the “th” sound, /ð/ as in “lather” will be substituted by the sound /d/ as in “ladder”, however, it inevitably creates a potential source of intelligibility. Among the numerous approaches to pronunciation teaching, listening to model speakers on a cassette and repeating what is heard is a dominant and traditional strategy. This is clearly demonstrated by the fact that there are a great deal of oral/aural exercises and material found in many pronunciation and phonetics textbooks. Kenworthy (1987) pointed out that “many learners will be able to produce new sounds simply by imitating what they hear”( P.69 ). MacCarthy (1978) also thought that “ in the end hearing is more important than either seeing or feeling” ( p.28 ). These imply that listening to model speakers can help learners produce new sounds. Methodology To examine whether the effectiveness of the inferring strategy in dealing with the target pronunciation problem and also the feelings towards pronunciation learning through the target strategy, three sets of data were collected: First, the result of the diagnostic test which was conducted to check how serious the students’ pronunciation problem was.( substitution of [d] for [ð] ) Second, the result of the performance test which was conducted to find out the learning outcomes after the experiment.( the trial ) Third, the result of the interviews which were conducted with the six selected subjects to find out the learners’ experiences and feelings towards the target approach. Implementation of the action research The subjects of the research come from a Form Two Class of mixed ability. The class consists of 42 students and they had to finish a diagnostic test (Appendix A) before the experiment. The diagnostic test consists of words in which the [ð] sound occurs. It aims at checking if the subjects produce the [ð] sound accurately. All speech samples were tape recorded for data analysis. The findings of the test were tabulated and analysed for being compared with the results of the performance test (Appendix A) to check if the effectiveness of the target strategy in dealing with the inferring pronunciation problem. 2 A brief summary of the activities in the experiment is displayed in the following table:( Appendix B ) Lesson Task 1-2 Learners’ activities Class organisation Skills Teacher models the [d] sound as in “dance”, “reduce” and “panda” T→Ss Listening Imitate the sound Ss→T Speaking Listen to the tape and repeat the words in Part A M→Ss Listening and Speaking Teacher models the [ð] sound T→Ss Listening Imitate the sound Ss→T Speaking Listen to the tape and repeat the words in Part B M→Ss Listening and Speaking 3 Listen to the tape and repeat the words in Part C M→Ss Listening and Speaking 4 Listen to the tape and repeat the words in Part D M→Ss Listening and Speaking 1 2 as in “they” and “mother”. Followed the trial, interviews were conducted with the six selected subjects to find out the learners’ experiences and feelings towards the target approach. The interviews were conducted individually and recorded for analysis. Findings and Discussion In the research study, there were two pronunciation tests (one was a diagnostic test and another one was a performance test) examining the effectiveness of listening to model speakers. Each pronunciation test was divided into three parts. There were altogether thirty words in which the [ð] sound could be found. The results of the two pronunciation tests were tabulated in the table below: Diagnostic Test Performance Test Number of Number of Percentage Number of Number of Percentage occurrences students ( of students ( occurrences students ( of students ( n=42 ) %) n=42 ) %) 26-30 3 7% 26-30 0 0% 21-25 12 29% 21-25 9 21% 16-20 15 36% 16-20 9 21% 11-15 6 14% 11-15 12 29% 6-10 6 14% 6-10 12 29% 0-5 0 0% 0-5 0 0% 3 As mentioned earlier, there were altogether thirty words, which are composed of the [ð] sound, in each pronunciation test. It is assumed that the subjects whose pronunciation problem occurred less than 15 times were classified as “acceptable”. The results of the diagnostic test showed that the percentage of the students who were classified as “acceptable” is 28%. After the trial, the percentage of students classified as “acceptable” is 58% in the performance test. The difference between the percentage of students in the two tests is 30%. Apart from this, the figure showed that the subjects under the range from 16-20 occurrences of the pronunciation errors decreased from 15 to 9. Even under the range from 21-25 occurrences and from 2630 occurrences, the percentage also dropped from 29% to 21% and from 7% to 0% respectively. It can be seen that the subjects had great improvement after the trial and it is quite an effective strategy in dealing with the target pronunciation problem. From the interviews conducted with the selected subjects after the trial, one of them stated that she eventually learnt how the[d] and [ð] sounds were produced differently simply by imitating what she heard. This findings was supported by the Innocence Principle mentioned by Strevens (1974). Although listening to model speakers is an effective approach to pronunciation learning, one of the subjects mentioned in the interview that it was quite boring to listen to the model speakers and repeat after them and they were losing interests after several trials. It indicated that the subjects did not really enjoy learning through listening to model speakers though it helped them achieve better results in the performance test. It is found in the data collected from the interviews conducted with the six selected subjects who are of different ability that students who were less capable may find it useful in mastering the sound production and they did enjoy learning through the target strategy. Conclusion It is believed that more than one approach to pronunciation teaching should be employed at a time. One of the reasons is that all the strategies have their strengths and weaknesses. Therefore, as a teacher, should not over-emphasis on one of the strategies. Another reason is that different learners enjoy learning through different strategies. A variety of strategies can suit the majority needs. It is interesting to find out that though the strategy of listening to model speakers is quite effective in dealing with the target pronunciation problem, it may not be favoured by all the learners. It seems that not many research studies focus on exploring learner’s preference. What’s more, it is suggested that further studies 4 should not only investigate the effectiveness of a strategy but also find out how it can match the personality and the needs of the learners as to maximize the effectiveness of the strategies. References Kenworthy, J. (1987). Teaching English Pronunciation. London: Longman Group UK Limited. MacCarthy, P. (1987). The Teaching of Pronunciation. Cambridge [Eng.]: Cambridge It can be seen that the subjects had great improvement after the trial.University Press. Strevens, P .( 1974) . A rationale for teaching pronunciation: the rival virtues of innocence and sophistication. ELT Journal. 28:185.[ Oxford, Eng ] : Oxford University Press. 5 Appendix A Pronunciation Test Date: Name: Class: F.1 Marks: Part I ) Say These words. their brothers the mother rather gather then this that these another there others leather together they Part II Say these sentences. 1. They’re brothers, aren’t they? 2. Is there any milk in the fridge? 3. Who is taller than Paul? 4. I’m sorry I missed that word. Did you say ‘those’? Part III ( them Read the paragraphs aloud. My name is Candy. I live with my parents and my brother in Causeway Bay. My father is a teacher. He enjoys playing tennis and watching videos. My mother works in a cake shop. She likes cooking. My family often do things together at weekends. We go to restaurants and visit my cousins. We play tennis on Saturdays and sometimes we walk in the park. We are a very busy family. 6 Appendix B Experiment (Trial 1) Part A Listen to the tape and repeat these words after you hear each one. 1. dog 5. adapt 2. daughter 6. calendar 3. director 7. confident 4. dollar 8. product Part B Listen to the tape and repeat these words after you hear each one. 1. they 5. feather 2. their 6. whether 3. there 7. mother 4. then 8. either Part C Listen to the tape and repeat these words after you hear each pair. 1. dare there 2. Dan than 3. day they 4. drill thrill 5. doze those 6. door thaw 7. ladder lather 7 Part D 1a) b) 2a) b) Listen to the tape and repeat these sentences after you hear each one. Is ‘dare’ a pronoun or a verb? Is there any sugar in the cupboard? Who is taller, Dan, Sam or Paul? Who is taller than Sam? 3a) Day came suddenly. b) They came suddenly. 4a) b) We had a big drill at school today. He gets his thrills from rock-climbing. 5a) I’m sorry I missed that word. Did you say ‘doze’? b) I’m sorry I missed that word. Did you say ‘those’? 6a) Look at the door. b) Look at the thaw. 7a) b) Did you put up the ladder here? Dad always works up a lather on his chin before shaving. 8