Knowledge Representation: Expertise in Negotiations Ron Caneel rcaneel@media.mit.edu Massachusetts Institute of Technology Abstract Do people who perform well in negotiation have a different representation of important factors in their mind? To answer this question, I analyzed the objective negotiation outcome for creating and claiming value in regard to the knowledge representation of subjective measurements. I used the Thurstone approach and individual difference scaling (INDSCAL) to find an answer to this question. The only finding that can be reported is that claiming value seems to have a more distinct separation in knowledge representation between skill groups than creating value. Introduction to Negotiations Negotiation is viewed by many people as art and by no fewer as science. Usually when we hear the word negotiation, we think about two or more business partners with the goal to make a favorable deal. But many situations in our daily lives can be viewed as negotiations: deciding with a friend where to have dinner, arguing with ones child who wants the latest toy, or finding a vacation destination that matches the taste of both a wife and her husband. The most dominant and obvious way to evaluate a negotiation is the objective outcome. Was the goal achieved? How well did I compare to the other person? But there are other important factors. Often a negotiation is not only a one-time event. People have to continue to work or to live with each other. In an ongoing relationship, both private and business, a person often does care what the other party thinks about him/her. In addition to the relationship factor, a person’s own feelings should not be neglected. This is true on an emotional as well as on a moral level. Looking again at the objective outcome there are two important dimensions: creating value and claiming value. Creating value is beneficial for both parties. By enlarging the pie for both sides this aspect is often called a win-win solution. By working together it is important to discover the hidden opportunities that increase the share for both parties. Claiming value refers to the aspect of how the total available value is distributed. Each party tries to get as much as possible from the fixed pie. In this case we have a win-lose situation; the better one side performs, the worse the other performs. So, when looking at the outcome of a negotiation, both dimensions have to be considered. A person could successfully create value but then leave all the shares to the other person, or the opposite could occur: from the little value that was created, a person can claim most of it. The purpose of this paper is to analyze how knowledge representation of different values can help a person to do well on either of the two objective outcomes. Background The standard answer for what distinguishes an expert from a novice is her/his knowledge. This is definitely true but what does this exactly mean? For most people this answer is related to content. Experts in their field simply know more than novices. They have cumulated more knowledge over time and have it at hand when necessary. Based on their greater knowledge, they are also able to better distinguish between different items and have a clearer understanding of the material. In addition to the difference in amount of knowledge, past research also discovered difference in how the data is internally structured and accessed. Here, we talk about knowledge representation. In this case, experts and novices use conceptually different systems. Traditionally there are two ways to analyze different skill levels. The first is recall performance. In chess [3], for example, experts show faster recall for meaningful situations than novices. Salomon [6] explains this with the chess master’s different sense of how pieces are clustered. The disadvantage of the recall method is that it doesn’t give us any insight into how knowledge is structured. The other category is the measurement of knowledge proximity between different items. For this approach subjects have to rate the similarity between different objects. The researchers’ goal is then to identify underlying structure that could explain the differences. Several analytical techniques can be used to extract the underlying structure: Multidimensional Scaling (MDS), principal component analysis (PCA) or hierarchical clustering. Not all of them are suited for measuring skill level differences. In addition to the three techniques mentioned above Ye [9] reports the use of correlation techniques, which can be used to study skill groups. By comparing the intra- and intergroup correlation coefficient, the different groups can be identified. The weakness of this method is the lack of indication of statistical significance. The standard MDS does not provide the necessary tools for comparing multiple people or groups since it only displays the dominant dimension for one subject. The metric method introduced by Caroll [2] solves this problem. Her method of Individual Difference Scaling (INDSCAL) considers the weighting of the axes between multiple similarity matrices. Past research analyzed several domains where the different skill levels between experts and novices can be explained with knowledge representation. As mentioned earlier, de Groot [4] discovered differences between experts and novices chess players. Solomon [6] studied conceptual difference in wine expertise. He found experts would use more specific features when describing wines and they tend to sort the wines explicitly by grape types. Correlation techniques were used by Shavelson [5] to understand different skill level in physics students. In the computer domain Ye looked at how programming concepts vary between experts and beginners. Research Design I had performance data from a negotiation session that was conducted with first-year Sloan students. It was a version of the new recruit case study. One person played the vice-president who hirers a new middle manager for his company. The two parties have to come to an agreement on eight different dimensions. Based on the agreement, two objective scores were calculated: one individual score (claiming value) and one dyadic score (creating value). These values were taken to distinguish between experts and novices. The assumption was that a certain skill set and approach helped them to generate a higher outcome. As a next step I had to find some criteria for how people would first evaluate their performances and what their attitudes towards negotiating is. In his research Curhan et al [3] found that people care about four basic domains: feelings about instrumental outcomes, feelings about themselves, feelings about the process, and feelings about their relationships. They created the Subjective Value Inventory (SVI), which should help a person to conceptualize his negotiation performance. The idea was that these dimensions not only might be asked after a conducted negotiation to evaluate the performance but that the weighting of these values in general might be a indicator for the objective outcome as well. Instead of asking the subject to rate each question of the SVI, I used Thurstone’s comparative judgment approach [7]. Several reasons influenced me trying the pairwise comparison approach. One problem with the widely used Linkert scale is the ceiling effect. Also is the Linkert scale more open to conscious interpretations by the subject and less likely to be an indicator of the real underlying structure. From economical psychology we know that people are influenced by the anchoring effect [8]. Derived from this we can conclude that it is not easy for subjects to come up with a robust rating of different values on a value scale. Ariely [1] discovered in his research that subjects can easily tell which item or activity they prefer in a pairwise comparison but are very inconsistent when mapping them onto a monetary scale. The Thurstone methodology is analogous to knowledge proximity measures generally used for MDS. Multidimensional scaling extracts a partial representation of the knowledge structure, which can be used to study different skill groups. I hoped to be able to extract a hidden structure by pairwise weighting the SVI questions. With the total number of SVI questions this would mean one hundred twenty comparisons. This is an overbearing and nearly impossible amount of comparisons for MBA students. I therefore reduced the sixteen questions to ten and modified the expression in a way that they could be compared with each other, e.g. “Do you feel your counterpart(s) listened to your concerns” was changed to “Feeling that your counterpart listened to you”. The possible answers for each statement were ‘much more important’, ‘more important’ or ‘slightly more important’. The final version (see figure 1) was on the web in the form of an online survey where the subjects could weight all forty-five pairs. The order of the comparisons was randomly assigned. Figure 1: Web form: example question Results 5. Fair The total number of completed survey was very small. Only ten students participated in this study. Several attempts to recruit more students failed. This means that the dataset is to small to come up with any significant findings. This implies as well that any differences that could be found could just be random and no meaningful interpretation can be derived from the data. The overlap between the two different skills (creating and claiming value) will also be big since I had to use the same data for both dimensions. Thurstone Analysis Since the Thurstone analysis only uses binary comparison as input, I transformed the ratings to binary weights. (This was taken into consideration during the survey design: An even rating scale prevented participants from selecting a neutral answer). Table 1 shows the preferred order for the creating value dimension, for both the experts and novice groups. A zscore value of +1.0 means a support of 84%; a score of 0 means support of 50% and a score of –1.0 means a support of 16%. 0.273 1. Competence 0.202 1. Competence 0.067 10. Trust 0.141 2. Consistent -0.340 5. Fair 0.070 3. Balance -0.408 8. Relationship -0.135 10. Trust -0.613 3. Balance -0.408 6. Appropriate -0.684 6. Appropriate -0.684 INDSCAL Even though the collected data was not similarity ratings in its proper means, the pairwise rating on a weighting scale allows running a multidimensional scaling algorithm. I used the INSCAL version included in the sas-application. Figure 2 and 3 show the different mappings for the creating value dimension. Table 1 Order for creating value Expert order z-value Novice order z-value 7. Lose Face (self) 0.751 7. Lose Face 1.035 4. Benefit (inst) 0.545 4. Benefit 0.918 2. Consistent (inst) 0.407 9. Listened 0.776 1. Competence (self) 0.337 5. Fair 0.259 8. Relationship (rel) 0.272 1. Competence 0.193 10. Trust (rel) 0.208 8. Relationship 0.077 5. Fair (proc) 0.003 3. Balance -0.041 9. Listened (proc) -0.137 10. Trust -0.291 6. Appropriate (self) -0.272 2. Consistent -0.334 3. Balance (inst) -0.472 6. Appropriate -0.949 Figure 2 INDSCAL plot for experts in creating value We can see some minor differences in the ranking but at the same time, the order of the two highest ranked items is the same. Table 2 shows the weighting for the claiming value dimension. In this case, a difference between the experts and the novice group seems to be more reasonable. Table 2 Order for claiming value Expert order z-value Novice order z-value 7. Lose Face 1.229 4. Benefit 1.021 9. Listened 1.091 2. Consistent 0.684 8. Relationship 0.478 7. Lose Face 0.340 4. Benefit 0.343 9. Listened 0.205 Figure 3 INDSCAL plot for novice in creating value The key for the “term_nr” pair is included in the appendix. The number does not have any meaning; it is just based on the random order of the questions. We can see several differences between the two figures. But there is no clear, obvious distinction for the two plots that could indicate two different structure with a higher then chance possibility. Figure 4 and 5 display the results for the claiming value dimension. Again, similar to the Thurstone analysis, we see a clearer difference between the two skill groups. Discussion As mentioned before the response to my call for participation in this project remained mostly unanswered. I tried different methods: snow ball method, handing out little notes with URL after asking students directly and sending personalized emails. But the success rate remained very low. Now after the project I also question my approach to use weighting instead of similarity to extract knowledge representation. Even though I mentioned advantages to this method earlier in the paper, I have to conclude that it does not work. In order to get meaningful scaling the proximity measurement should be taken. It might also be that the assignment to experts and novices was not really appropriate. First, I only used the performance of the subgroups to determine the skill level. Second, there was a 50% overlap between the two objective dimensions due to the small answer size. References [1] D. Ariely, G. Loewenstein and D. Prelec, "Coherent arbitrariness: Stable demand curves without stable preferences," Quarterly Journal of Economics, No.118 (1), 73-105, 2003. [2] J.D. Caroll and J.J Chang " Analysis of individual differences in multidimensional scaling via an n-way generalization of " Eckart-Young " decomposition ", Psychometrika, 35, 238-319, 1979. Figure 4 INDSCAL plot for experts in claiming value [3] Curhan, J. R., Elfenbein, H. A., & Xu, A. (2004). What do people care about when they negotiate? Mapping the domain of subjective value in negotiation, Manuscript in preparation (www.subjectivevalue.com) [4] A. D. de Groot, Thought and Choices in Chess. The Hague, The Netherlands: Mouton, 1965. [5] R.J, Shavelson. Some aspects of the correspondence between content structure and cognitive structure in physics instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 63, 225234, 1972. [6] G. E., Solomon. Conceptual change and wine expertise. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6 (1), 41–60, 1997. [7] Thurstone LL: "A law of comparative judgment". Psychological Review; 34: 273-286, 1927. Figure 5 INDSCAL plot for novice in claiming value A clustering according to the SVI values cannot be identified although many times at least two items seem to be in close to each other. [8] T.D., Wilson, C. Houston, K.M. Etling,, &N.A. Brekke, new look at anchoring effects: Basic anchoring and its antecedents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 4, 387-402, 1996. [9] N. Ye. The MDS-ANAVA technique for assessing knowledge representation differences between skill groups. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetic Part A 28(5): 586-600, 1998. APPENDIX SVI Nr Full Text Question dimension 1 'Feeling competent as a negotiator' self 1 2 'Feeling like your outcome is consistent with principles of legitimacy or objective criteria' instr 1 3 'Feeling satisfied with the balance between your own outcome and your counterpart's outcome' instr 2 4 'Feeling satisfied with the extent to which the terms of your agreement benefit you' instr 3 5 'Feeling that the negotiation process was fair' process 1 6 'Feeling that you behaved appropriately in terms of your own principles and values' self 2 7 'Feeling that you didn't "lose face" (i.e., damage your sense of pride)' self 3 8 'Feeling that you have a good relationship with your counterpart' relationship 1 9 'Feeling that your counterpart listened to you' process 2 10 'Feeling trust for your counterpart' relationship 2