Determinants of Auditor Reliance on Decision Aids Cam Cockrell Ph. D student University of Kentucky Thanks to Bob Ramsay for his comments and guidance on this paper. I also wish to thank Dan Stone for his comments and suggestions. Also, thanks to the participants of the University of Kentucky brownbag for providing a multitude of helpful comments. Abstract Recently, audit firms have instituted striking increases in the levels of technology used during an audit. Firms continue to accept and incorporate new decision aids into their audits. In order to maximize the benefits of using decision aids, a vital concern for audit firms is to understand the implications for and determinants of decision aid reliance by auditors. Although extant research has attempted to isolate some of these factors, studies in this area have not been crafted from a uniform theory. This literature review synthesizes research on auditor decision aid reliance, and places it within the context of the Technology Acceptance Model (Davis 1989) and the Theory of Technology Dominance (Sutton and Arnold 2002). The results of this paper suggest a model of auditor decision aid reliance for empirical testing and future research. 1 1. Introduction and Motivation 1.0 Introduction Decision aids have the potential to add significant value to an audit. From choosing to accept a new client, to planning the engagement and making judgments throughout the audit, decision aids can help auditors prevent omissions and mistakes (Abdolmohammadi and Usoff 2001). In order for a decision aid’s benefit to be fully realized, some level of reliance must exist. Prior literature has gleaned valuable information about different factors that influence auditor reliance on decision aids; however, no uniform theory exists to unite these determinants of reliance and provide insight as to driving force between them. The purpose of this paper is to develop a framework for auditor decision aid reliance which draws from theory and extant literature. 1.1 Structure This paper is structured into five sections. The first section contains the introduction, and motivation. The next part of the paper discusses different types of decision aids and how they map into the audit (Section 2). The following segment of the paper (Section 3) describes auditor characteristics, and explains what is unique about auditors and their tasks, as they relate to decision aids. Next, I synthesize the literature under review (Section 4) and conclude with suggestions for future research (Section 5). 1.2 Motivation The auditing environment is unique from many other decision-making contexts. The regulatory environment surrounding an audit creates a need to justify decisions at all levels (Ashton 1992). Individual auditors must justify their decisions to their superiors; the audit firm 2 must justify its decisions during peer review; and both individuals and the firm may have to justify decisions during litigation. The emphasis that an audit places on the justification requirement cannot be overlooked when examining decision aids within an auditing context. As will be discussed in more detail, many over and under-reliance problems are related to justification issues. Experience can cause auditors to believe they have a better feel for particular situations than decision aids, thus leading to under-reliance. Similarly, inexperienced auditors may not question decision aid outcomes, or might be in decision-making situations that they have not yet been trained to handle, thus leading to over-reliance. Both under and over reliance on a decision aid can be problematic, as illustrated in figure 1 below. Depending upon the task, different costs can be associated with each type of error. Accountants avoid relying upon decision aid outcomes (e.g. Ashton 1990; Ashton 1992; Boatsman et al. 1997). This becomes problematic when a decision aid outperforms human judgment on a particular task, causing under-reliance and suboptimal decision-making. It is optimal to supplement decision aid outcomes with professional judgment, but not to rely solely on either one (Kleinmuntz 1990). Rely Do Not Rely Figure 1 Decision Aid is Decision Aid is correct incorrect/misleading 1) Efficient use of 2) Reliance is a aid reliance problem (Type I error)1 3) Non-reliance is 4) Efficient use of a problem (Type II self reliance error) 1 This can be thought of as a miss by the decision aid. Alternatively, in the reliance when decision aid is correct cell, a suboptimal decision to rely may still be made. For example, an inexperienced auditor performing some task may not question the decision aid or be able to incorporate their professional knowledge into the decision-making process within the task domain. A further discussion of this scenario can be found in section 6 of this paper. 3 Examining higher-level auditors’ views toward tasks that may benefit from decision aid use may prove valuable in evaluating the culture surrounding an audit. One might expect reliance to be improved in a culture that views decision aids as more useful than not. In scrutinizing how managers and partners view decision aid usefulness, Abdolmohammadi and Usoff (2001) found that between 1988 and 1996, higher level auditors increased their view of what tasks could benefit from decision aid use from 11% of audit tasks identified to 16%. This trend shows that decision aid usefulness may be gaining momentum as the degree of technological acceptance increases. However, despite these findings, managers and partners evaluated over half of the tasks identified in the study as needing human processing only.2 This outcome indicates that decision aids are not be perceived by higher-ranking auditors to be as useful as trained auditor judgment in most situations. For reliance implications to be a relevant research topic, auditors must have some flexibility to accept, modify, or reject the outcome of decision aids. In practice, this contingency is likely based on myriad factors including firm culture, aid type, and task factors. It is probable that decisions involving more flexible inputs are less likely to be restricted in terms of reliance. While firm policies evolve over time, recent studies support the existence of flexibility to alter decision aid outcomes in multiple situations. A study done by Bell et al. (2002) on a client acceptance/retention decision aid used by KPMG (KRisk), indicates a level of freedom in the decision to rely on the aid’s outcome. KRisk was designed such that individual risk assessments made by the aid can be adjusted up (more 2 Abdolmohammadi and Usoff define programmable tasks as those for which at least 50% of managers and partners questioned indicated some sort of decision aid as the optimal way to perform the task. The 11% to 16% increase therefore indicates those tasks where at least 50% of respondents indicated that “human processing only” was not optimal. 4 risky) if its risk determination seems inappropriately low, but not vice-versa. However, the final output of the aid cannot be altered, though the auditor may “initiate a dialogue with the appropriate reviewing partner(s) to discuss the ‘business case’ for continuing or accepting a client with a high risk score” (Bell et al. 2002 p. 109). Therefore, it can be argued that in one of the most important and presumably restrictive decisions an audit firm makes, some degree of flexibility exists regarding reliance upon a decision aid. The riskier the decision, the more likely the auditor must initiate dialogue with a superior in order not to rely on the decision aid outcome. However, the auditor using the aid often makes less risky reliance decisions without the assistance of superiors. Discussions with practitioners in the areas of sample size determination, and executing the audit plan indicates that decisions are often made to override a decision aid’s suggested outcome. With the audit program, the senior often selects alternative procedures when a checklist decision aid suggests a step that the auditor deems unnecessary or unduly difficult to complete. Likewise, if a tool such as ACL or IDEA suggests a large sample size that an auditor feels is excessive for a particular test of details, the auditor may attempt to justify choosing a smaller sample size. Although these examples of flexibility with decision aids exist, it is presumable that a decision not to rely must be documented, and is certainly subject to justification should the reviewer question it. The ultimate decision for an auditor to rely on a decision aid outcome is subject to many influences not limited to justification and experience. This leads to some interesting questions, including the following: “Under what conditions do auditors choose to rely or not rely on the outcome of a decision aid?” This paper synthesizes prior literature, examines the determinants of auditor decision aid reliance, and suggests a unification of theory and extant literature to develop a cogent model of auditor decision aid reliance. 5 2. Decision Aids 2.0 Introduction of Decision Aids Ideally, decision aids minimize the total cost of errors for the decisions in which they are used. However, this goal is difficult to achieve. In order to enhance decision aid success, many types of decision aids exists. Using an often-cited definition from Rohrmann (1986 p. 365) a decision aid may be thought of as “any explicit procedure for the generation, evaluation, and selection of alternatives (courses of action) that is designed for practical application and multiple use.” Following this characterization, decision aids may be as simple as a pencil and paper checklist, or as complex as a large group decision support system or an expert system. In order for them to be most effective, the type of decision aid used should be contingent upon task factors. For the purposes of this paper, I focus on three types of decision aids because they map directly into the audit. These decision aids are deterministic, expert systems, and group support systems. 2.1 Deterministic Decision Aids The least sophisticated decision aids listed in the previous discussion are simple or deterministic aids. These types of decision aids are defined by Messier (1995 p. 214) as aids “which may or may not be computerized (and) include any tools that aid judgment in a straightforward algorithmic manner.” Examples of deterministic decision aids include checklists and questionnaires. Messier and Hansen (1987) point out that deterministic aids are generally best suited for highly structured problem domains, sometimes for semi-structured ones, and virtually never for unstructured problems. Furthermore, deterministic aids commonly provide whole solutions to structured problems (Rose 2002). Due to this feature, evaluating reliance on 6 deterministic decision aids is more concrete relative to other decision aid types. Since an entire solution is provided by the aid, rather than simply guidance, the reliance decision may completely alter the decision outcome. Simple/deterministic decision aids are likely to be attached to tasks that are well defined, less risky, and require less experienced auditors to perform (Messier 1995). Each of those task factors may have an incremental effect on the reliance decision beyond decision aid type. 2.2 Expert Systems At the opposite end of the spectrum from deterministic decision aids are expert systems. This type of decision aid is defined by Leech and Sangster (2002 p. 66) as “a computer program intended to mimic the decisions of the human expert.” These are tools designed to augment a decision maker’s skill – essentially to that of some other highly skilled decision maker within the given task domain. In effect, it allows the firm to use the expert’s knowledge without using the expert to make the decision. This enables the firm to spread specialized knowledge throughout the organization, without overextending the expert(s) from whom the system is designed. When evaluating task structure, the general applicability of expert systems is opposite that of deterministic decision aids. Unstructured tasks are usually the most conducive for expert systems (Messier and Hansen 1987). Expert systems are sometimes applicable for semistructured tasks; however, since building expert systems can be quite expensive, it is often a better match to use decision support systems in those contexts. Expert systems are used in highly unstructured tasks, where the acquisition and evaluation of information is “ill defined” and “highly uncertain” (Messier 1995 p. 215). Because of these characteristics, expert systems generate suggested rather than deterministic guidance. Evaluating auditor reliance under this 7 circumstance is more challenging, because at issue is how to measure the degree of reliance on decision aids if the suggested outcomes and final decisions diverge. 2.3 Group Support Systems and Group Decision Making A third type of decision aid exists that is neither completely deterministic nor based solely on the decision-making prowess of an expert. A Decision Support System is “an interactive computer-based software that assists decision makers by using certain models (e.g. statistical or mathematical models in analytical review procedures) and data to make inferences for the use of the decision maker” (Abdolmohammadi 1999 p. 54-55). These systems generally are used in tasks that are semi-structured, and require some flexibility. As one might expect from the open-ended description of a DSS, they may be applied in virtually any task structure (Messier 1995). Although some overlap exists between the applicability of a DSS relative to deterministic aids or expert systems, as tasks become more (less) structured, a DSS loses its advantage relative to deterministic aids (expert systems) (Messier 1995). A special subtype of DSS applicable to auditing is Group Support Systems (GSS). The defining feature of a GSS is that multiple decision makers integrate information electronically to work toward some decision. That is, virtually any system where individuals working as teams on the same project can integrate decision-making information. Examples of such systems include, but are not limited to video conferencing, online chat sessions between teammates, and file-sharing software. Using Rohrmann’s decision aid definition, if the GSS is part of an explicit procedure to generate, evaluate, and choose amongst decision alternatives, then it is (or is part of) a decision aid. 8 3. Auditor Characteristics, Tasks, and how they relate to Decision Aids 3.0 Introduction of Auditor decision making When attempting to apply the findings of general decision-making research to auditors, one will find that a few differences exist. Solomon and Shields (1995) point out that the tendency in the literature has been for audit decision-making studies to build on a non-audit framework, rather than investigate the cause for subtle differences between the two. Although not all of these differences directly relate to the reliance issue, there is some significance to these contrasts. For example, Solomon and Shields suggest that auditors tend to show lower rates of overconfidence than non-auditor decision makers do. The result of this may be that auditors are not subject to the same tendencies of reliance as non-audit decision makers. In addition to individual factors, such as overconfidence, many other influences exist in the auditing domain that are unique, or amplified vis-à-vis a generic decision making environment. 3.1 Auditor Characteristics When compared to partners, senior managers are even less willing to accept decision aids as appropriate for decision support use, ceteris paribus (Abdolmohammadi 1991). Perhaps this is due to time pressure felt by senior management to complete the audit successfully, and the presumption that decision aided activities may take a longer time to complete than strictly human processed decisions. The factors, discussed in the preceding paragraph, qualifying one most to be an expert auditor emphasize a knowledgeable, experienced and confident auditor who is not afraid of relying on his or her own ability to make a tough decision. Those characteristics that may be consistent with reliance on a decision aid to assist in a decision-making task – such as being perceptive, flexible, and logical, are relatively deemphasized. While it is difficult to argue that any of these highlighted qualities are negative, the above ranking of attributes indicate a 9 perception that auditors become experts using qualities that encourage (discourage) self (decision aid) reliance. In an environment where legal liability and is so contingent upon justifying decisions, auditors certainly have pressure on them to try to optimize those decisions. Ashton (1990) provides a model (pressure-arousal-performance) demonstrating that the presence of incentives, feedback, and pressure actually reduce decision aid reliance. A possible reason for this behavior is that incentives drive decision makers to differentiate themselves in an effort to outperform the decision aid, thereby placing too much confidence in their own ability. Decision makers might even use decision aids as a starting point, and then apply heuristics in an attempt to outperform other decision makers (i.e. their peers). An alternative explanation is that decision makers who feel the need to separate themselves from colleagues may sometimes ignore the aids completely, in an attempt to optimize their individual decisions. These reasons may be related to underreliance on decision aids when decision makers have high self confidence in performing given tasks (Arkes et al. 1986; Ashton 1990; Whitecotton 1996). 3.2 Auditor Tasks Another important consideration to take into account when examining auditor decision aids is the task dimension. High-level auditors tend to view decision aid appropriateness on a continuum, as more (less) appropriate for use when the task is highly (un-) structured (Abdolmohammadi 1991). If this view is prevalent throughout the audit firms, it assists in explaining the lack of reliance on certain decision aids related to unstructured tasks, such as fraud evaluation aids (e.g. Hansen et al. 1996). Given the diversity of disparate tasks required to complete an audit, and the sensitivity to task structure, one may reasonably conclude that auditor reliance on the outcome of a decision aid is correlated with the type of task being performed. 10 Indeed, Bonner et al. (1996) find that decision aid use leads to better decision quality when a mismatch between task structure and auditor knowledge structure exists. They also point out that decision aids that improve decision quality in one task may not improve it in others. Recall from the discussion in section 3 that the appropriateness of a decision aid depends on the task structure. This brings into question what portion of non-reliance may be attributable to how the decision aid relates to task structure. Despite this important aspect of auditor reliance on decision aids, there is a dearth of research related to differentiated tasks. However, much of the current research is performed relative to one specific task (e.g. fraud, client acceptance, etc…). The weakness in this approach is that no individual study provides evidence on how specific factors influence auditor reliance on decision aids in general. Although many studies focus on finding ways to ensure auditor reliance on decision aids, few examine cases where auditor non-reliance is correct with respect to decision aid outcome. Within a fraud context, Nieschwietz et al. (2000) provide examples from extant research of six decision models compared to auditor findings. These results show that decision aids are significantly better at minimizing errors occurring from a rejection of fraud when fraud is actually present, while auditors are significantly better at minimizing errors occurring from a conclusion that fraud exists when in reality it does not. Despite making type II errors more than half of the time when fraud is present, the ultimate overall accuracy, measured by the number of times the existence of fraud is correctly identified as present or not present, is comparable between total self-reliance and decision aid reliance. The reason for that outcome is that incidents of actual fraud are perceived to be infrequent. Despite the above findings, the costs of type I and type II errors are a relevant comparison, and differ by task. The potential cost of concluding there is no occurrence of fraud 11 when in fact fraud exists is higher than concluding that fraud is present when it is not (Hansen et al. 1996). Because of this assertion, much of the literature has focused on type II errors. In the Hansen, et al. model, type II errors are weighted at 10 times more costly than type I errors to reflect this cost. The prevailing thought is that while there are inefficiency costs associated with increased work, and potential damage in the auditor-client relationship from type I errors, the potential governmental sanctions and litigation costs resulting from type II errors far outweigh those costs (e.g. Enron). Alternatively, a type I vs. type II sample size determination error may not have the same differential cost impact as compared to the fraud scenario. Hanson et al. (1996) find that when fraud does not exist, the auditor is rarely ever wrong, while decision aids have a much higher frequency of false positive errors. However, when fraud does exist, auditors rarely conclude that fraud exists, causing a costly type II error. Because auditors are trained to use professional skepticism, they may be predisposed to become increasingly cynical as they see more errors made by a decision aid. When auditors see a substantial number of type I errors from decision aids, they may be more apt to distrust the aid. When type I and type II errors have different costs, and the decision aid is a better predictor of the more costly error, this distrust may become problematic.3 The implication is that decision aid design research might also need to focus on reducing each type of error, rather than focusing on the more costly type, so that auditors will rely more optimally on them. 3 When type I and type II errors have different costs, the bias of the decision aid may be calibrated so that the cost of making an error is reduced at the expense of overall accuracy. For example, if a type II error is more expensive than a type I error, the decision aid may be biased in a way that it is more (less) likely to register a false alarm (miss). This could have the effect of lowering the cost of making an error with the decision aid, but reducing the overall accuracy of the aid. 12 4. Auditor Decision Aid Reliance Research 4.0 Organizing Framework Due to the multitude of factors that make both auditors and their tasks uniquely different from others, it is difficult to classify extant research on the factors themselves. However, major differences do exist between studies, particularly between those that examine factors causing over- or under-reliance decisions and those focusing on usability or decision performance. To organize the literature under review, I use the framework in figure 2 (below) which is slightly modified, but based on Rose (2002) in which differing perspectives are used. Under-reliance, over-reliance, usability, and performance perspectives are used as the organizing perspectives. Figure 2: Organizing Framework for Auditor Decision Aid Reliance Decision Aid Auditor Making Decision 1, 2 3 3 Decision Process 4 Decision Outcome 1 – Over-reliance 2 – Under-reliance 3 – Usability 4 – Performance These perspectives are based on the core determinants of the decision making outcome. Studies that focus on over or under-reliance by the auditor on the decision aid are characterized as over or under-reliance papers within table 1 (Panel A and B, below). Usability may affect the decision process through the decision aid or directly from the decision maker to the process. Research that has reliance implications based on either of these effects is classified as usability 13 research within table 1 (Panel C, below). Also, some studies focus directly on the decision outcome and evaluate the performance of the decision process. These studies are classified as performance papers within table 1, (Panel D, below). 4.1 Synthesis of Current Literature Under-reliance Statistical models tend to generate relatively more reliable outcomes than do individual decision makers (Kleinmuntz 1990). Despite this, auditors often avoid reliance on decision aids that are based on statistical models (Ashton 1992; Boatsman et al. 1997). While only three of the ten studies appear in this section, most of the other studies also take the perspective that increasing reliance usually equates to better decision quality. Boatsman, et al. (1997) encountered interesting problems with decision aid reliance. Auditors whose first reaction was to rely on a decision aid often shifted their final judgments away from decision aid outcomes. Some users appeared to ignore the aid altogether. Boatsman et al. (1997) point out that intentional shifting appears to be subject to the same factors as in Ashton’s pressure-arousal-performance model. This supports a combination of incentives, feedback, and pressure resulting in lower reliance within an auditing / fraud determination context. Eining, et al. (1997) provide a possible solution for the ignoring problem, through constructive dialogue. Combining active involvement and continuous feedback elements, constructive dialogue may lead to higher reliance. 14 Table 1 -- Empirical Studies -- Auditor Reliance/Non-reliance on Decision Aids (1995-Present) Panel A: Under-reliance Study Research Issue(s) Context Data / Participants Outcome Boatsman, Moeckel, and Pei (1997) Will users change to rely, ignore, intentionally shift, or remain in agreement with a decision aid under different decision consequences? Fraud determination Told that decision aid outperforms firm experts Auditor makes initial judgment, has to justify answers within the aid, then makes a final determination Five cases each— one warm up, two fraud, and two nofraud Electronic use of decision aid with 24 yes/no red flag questions Auditors split into five different treatment groups and one control group. 118 audit seniors in an international accounting firm As a whole, when the initial judgment is no fraud, and the aid predicts fraud, ignoring the aid is significant Does predictive ability information influence decision makers differently depending on their locus of control? Bond rating predictions Mechanical Decision Aid Similar experiment to Ashton (Ashton 1990; Ashton 1992) Laboratory experiment 91 auditors from a big six audit firm, each with approximately three years of experience (predictive ability) 61 masters level business students (decision maker involvement) Internal control locus participants demonstrated significantly less reliance on the decision aid than did external control locus participants. Kaplan, Reneau, and Whitecotton (2001) Does decision maker involvement in the decision aid’s development affect reliance by users based on their locus of control? 15 As a whole, when the initial judgment is fraud, and the aid predicts fraud, intentional shifting is significant Internal locus of control participants, who were involved with the decision aid’s development, demonstrated significantly higher reliance on the decision aid. Decision Aid Reliance Implications Ignoring decision aids remains a large problem. There was no clear reason for ignoring the aid. Adds credence to Ashton’s (1990) pressure-arousalperformance hypothesis. Understanding how to address individual differences may lead to greater decision aid reliance Allowing decision makers to stay involved with a decision aid’s development may increase reliance for internal locus of control individuals Panel A continued Study Research Issue(s) Lowe and Reckers (2000) Will auditors provided with negative outcome information about a firm provide greater inventory obsolescence judgments about the company? If cued about potential legal scenarios, will auditor’s assessments of inventory obsolescence be different? If an auditor is required to generate multiple potential outcomes, will that affect inventory obsolescence assessments? Panel B: Over-reliance Anderson, Will auditors rate Moreno, and decision aid Mueller explanations more (2003) reasonable than client explanation when both are insufficient to account for the full fluctuation? Context Data / Participants Outcome Publicly traded manufacturing company with a recent decline in performance Financial statement ratios slightly above debt covenants Potential obsolescence of one of the firm’s highest selling items Participants asked for likelihood of adjustments needed to issue an unqualified opinion Design consists of four groups. Foresight groups were further manipulated via outcome knowledge (none, single, and multiple) with the final group consisting of a hindsight group. 131 audit seniors from a big 5 firm with an average of 3.3 years experience The decision aid prompting auditors to consider potential legal scenarios significantly affected auditor judgment of inventory obsolescence relative to those receiving no cue, but not from those in the hindsight condition. Medium size manufacturing client Perform analytical procedures Lab experiment 51 randomly assigned big 5 auditors with a mean of 74 months audit experience Auditors receiving explanations from decision aids rated the explanations as more sufficient than those receiving client explanation 16 Decision Aid Reliance Implications Foresight decision aids may have the ability to influence reliance by prompting auditors to consider potential scenarios that nonforesight aids do not. Prompting auditors to consider multiple potential outcomes appeared to have a marginal incremental effect relative to cuing one possible outcome. Inappropriate sufficiency judgments about decision aid information, possibly due to its perceived objectiveness as a source Potential for ending the information search too early in the analytical procedure stage of an audit Panel B Continued Study Research Issue(s) Swinney (1999) Do auditors over rely on expert systems when they are incorrect? Context Auditor evaluation of loan loss reserves by a financial institution Do auditors rely more heavily on negative outcomes than positive outcomes? Panel C: User Preference Dilla and What are the Stone (1997) differential effects on auditors of using numeric cues and responses relative to linguistic cues and responses? Do the cue and response types affect cognitive effort? Computer-based experiment with custom software, with risks based on professional literature Participants obtain hidden cue information by prompting the system, and respond by making risk assessments. Data / Participants Outcome Cases distributed to one control and two treatment groups Results suggest an over reliance on expert systems 29 practicing auditors from three national accounting firms Decision Aid Reliance Implications Due to the sophistication of the decision aid, auditors may over rely on incorrect expert systems outcomes Auditors relied on negative information from the expert system to a greater degree than positive information from the expert system Auditors rely more heavily on negative outcomes from an expert system than positive outcomes 2 x 2 betweenparticipants crossed design (cues x responses) balanced by firm 71 auditors (60 from big 6 firms, and 11 from smaller national firms; 5 staff, 36 seniors, 24 managers and 6 partners) Linguistic cue representation increases agreement on cue weights, but not judgment consistency. Numeric response representation increases judgment consistency, but not agreement on cue weights. Linguistic (numeric) cue (response) representation increases time spent per cue acquired 17 The differential cognitive demands by cue type may have implications tied to the technology acceptance model ‘perceived ease of use’ construct, ultimately affecting reliance. Panel C continued Study Research Issue(s) Eining, Jones, and Loebbecke (1997) Will auditors rely to a different degree on checklists, logit models, and expert systems? Will constructive dialogue within expert systems increase auditor reliance? Messier, Kachelmeier, and Jensen (2001) Do auditors work backward from a desired outcome to justify their decisions? Will the use of a supplemental worksheet help circumvent the working backward problem? Context Data / Participants Outcome Fraud determination Expert system provided constructive dialogue Computerized Laboratory experiment Four groups—one control, one checklist, one logit, and expert system Three total cases— one low, one medium, and one high risk 93 auditors from a big six accounting firm averaging 3.2 yrs. experience and 23 audit engagements Control group and checklist group did not significantly differ. Between-participants design with two experimental factors (AICPA nonstatistical sample guidance [four levels] and population size [two levels]) 149 auditors (44 staff, 82 seniors, and 20 managers) with an average of 3.6 years experience from two big six firms Working backward phenomenon exists causing auditors to choose sample sizes significantly less than what is sufficient Sample size determination based on strong internal controls Supplies inventory from a small consumer appliance manufacturer 18 Members in the logit group relied on the decision aid to a greater degree than did the control group. Members in the expert system group performed better than the logit group. Sample size was increased and consistency enhanced, but the working backward problem was not solved Decision Aid Reliance Implications The design of a decision aid can ultimately increase auditor reliance in the aid Use of an expert system with constructive dialogue can allow auditors to make decisions regarding additional audit actions required by the evaluation of risk As auditors perceive a decision aid outcome to be counter-intuitive, they are likely to find a solution they find intuitive and work backward through the aid to justify their decision. Design of decision aids should take into account this problem to make outcomes seem more intuitive Panel D: Performance Study Research Issue(s) Bedard and Graham (2002) Does the orientation of a decision aid (positive vs. negative) affect auditor judgments of the risk factors for an existing client? Context Data / Participants Outcome Identification of specific risk factors related to auditors’ existing clients. Risks are evaluated and audit planning decisions made. Cases distributed to matched pairs of auditors who had prior experience working together. Decision aid manipulated by positive and negative orientation (wording). 46 big 5 auditors (19 seniors, 25 managers, and two partners) 6.9 years avg. experience Results suggest that the framing of the decision aid influences auditors. Computerized decision aid Auditors perform an analytical review, where the client’s inventory turnover substantially decreased from the prior year. Two-way betweenparticipants design, where client risk is either high or low, and goal framing is inclusive or exclusive respective to potential explanations. 65 big 5 auditors (19 staff, 20 senior, 20 managers, 2 partners, and 4 did not indicate) An inclusive goal frame (high-risk client) lead to a significantly larger number of potential explanations. Will auditors plan an audit differently as a result of using a decision aid with positive relative to negative orientation? Mueller and Anderson (2002) Does a higher client risk level (or inclusive goal framing) lead to a greater number of potential explanations during an analytic procedure task? Is there an interaction effect between client risk and goal framing on the number of potential explanations during an analytic procedure task? 19 Negative framing influences auditors to recall more risks about existing clients than positive framing. This leads to a greater amount of audit tests chosen in the planning phase. No interaction effect was found between goal framing and client risk. Decision Aid Reliance Implications Framing did not affect the auditors’ initial inherent/control risk assessments. It is possible that this is evidence of a form of non-reliance on the decision, though other causes cannot be dismissed. Auditors appear to be relying on the aid by implementing more tests under negative framing, though auditor inputs differ based on their framing condition. Biases built into a decision aid may influence reliance, but make the decision aid less effective/efficient, depending on the task and error costs. Another factor that may lead to greater reliance is how differences in individual decisionmakers’ personalities may affect decision aid reliance. Kaplan, et al. (2001) show that auditors with an internal locus of control were more likely to under-rely on decision aid outcomes. Thus, we may be able to now focus on internal locus of control auditors and design decision aids to increase their reliance, since those with an external locus of control demonstrate significantly higher reliance. Over-reliance The differences between type I and type II reliance errors in the current studies highlight the importance of understanding the precise factors leading to both under and over-reliance, and the conditions under which they obtain. Certain consequences not yet discussed have been identified in the current research regarding both over and under reliance. Anderson et al. (2003) find that auditors may view decision aids as more objective than the client as a source of information. Because of this, client explanations were rated lower than those generated by the decision aid were. Overrating decision aided explanations may improperly divert auditors away from investigating possible irregularities (Anderson et al. 2003). As previously stated, most decision aid studies are motivated by emphasizing improving decision quality through increasing reliance. A few studies, however, have begun examining over-reliance issues. Early studies in this realm indicate that in certain contexts, auditors may in fact over-rely on decision aids – specifically when using expert systems (Swinney 1999) and during the analytical procedures phase (Anderson et al. 2003). Swinney’s finding that auditors over-rely on an expert system in a loan loss reserve context could is a prime example of a mismatch between auditor task experience and sophistication of a decision aid. This type of reliance is suboptimal however, as users are not supplementing decision aid outcomes with their 20 professional knowledge. Under this scenario, the decision effectively becomes automated, and the user is contributing little to the decision process. Further research must be done in this area to understand the implications of this type of reliance, and how to avoid it. Usability The usability of a decision aid can also affect user reliance. Both usability and performance variables have direct implications on decision aid design. In some cases, rather than under-rely on a decision aid, auditors may manipulate or misuse decision aids to justify their intuitive decisions. The Messier et al. study (2001) shows that there is a “working backward” phenomenon, first found by Kachelmeier and Messier (1990). In their study, Messier et al. (2001) found that during a sample size determination task, auditors choose an intuitive sample size and then work backward through the decision aid to justify their choice. This dysfunctional behavior relates to the auditor not understanding the statistical reasons for the aid’s results. To remedy this problem, Messier et al. (2001) suggest statistical training, or auditor involvement in an internal decision aid design. It is notable that the idea of involvement with the aid’s development can increase reliance is consistent with the findings of Kaplan et al. (2001). Another way of handling the working backward problem is through the type of decision aid used. For example, Bamber et al. (1998) point out that group support systems are less likely to have working backward problems. Although system designs that discourage working backward can mitigate that problem, they may be potentially less intuitive to auditors who may prefer the transparency of seeing how the decision aid works. In other words, instead of remedying the working backwards problem, there is a potential to transform it into an underreliance problem. Still, current studies continue to show that the design of decision aids, including such features as constructive dialogue, can increase reliance (e.g. Eining et al. 1997). 21 Another study that may have preference implications is Dilla and Stone (1997). In their study, they found that linguistic (numeric) cues (response) increased the time spent per cue acquired relative to numeric (linguistic) cues (response). In light of this finding, it is possible that users notice the cue or response type difference, and perceive some relative disparity in ease of use. However, since Dilla and Stone found differences in judgment consistency based on cue or response type, a perceived usefulness construct may also have an impact. To determine if auditor reliance implications exist based on numeric and linguistic differences, more research must be done in this area. Performance The performance dimension refers to those studies that have reliance implications based on certain features within the decision aid or between the aid and task. The studies within this subsection tend to be less direct in terms of evaluating reliance, but have implications for it nonetheless. Many of the studies are based on the framing of linguistic cues within a decision aid. One factor often studied within the literature is the effect of framing a decision aid in different ways. Within these studies, it is common to examine the impact of how information is presented on the actions of the user. Lowe and Reckers (2000) examined a decision aid prompting auditors to consider the possibility of potential legal scenarios if an inventory obsolescence problem exists and is not found by the audit team. They found that framing the decision aid to prompt auditors to think about their legal liability if errors were made had a significant impact on the auditors’ judgments. Other framing studies have either found mixed results, or cautioned against the potential costs of differing aid structures. Mueller and Anderson (2002) examine a decision aid used 22 during analytical review to determine which alternative explanations to test. They find that the decision aid with an inclusive goal frame resulted in a larger number of potential explanations in an analytic procedure task relative to an exclusive goal frame.4 Bedard and Graham (2002) found that while framing did not affect auditors’ initial inherent/control risk assessments, it did affect the number of tests implemented during the planning phase. Within the context of making multiple decisions, framing effects on reliance at the individual level versus the aggregate decision level should be evaluated. Although these sorts of biases may be built-in for reliance purposes, the task and error costs must be considered to assess the cost/benefit of instituting such factors into the aid. 5. Conclusion and Future Research Implications 5.0 Conclusion and Future Research Questions The biggest drawback to this area of research is the lack of uniform theory. Various studies have highlighted different factors that either do or do not influence auditor reliance within different contexts, yet we still have no set theory of decision aid reliance. Recently, Arnold and Sutton (1998) have submitted the Theory of Technology Dominance (TTD), which models the general factors leading to reliance. Audit decision aid research should continue moving in this direction and empirically test these types of models to establish underlying theory as to what factors influence reliance. Throughout the course of this review, many determinants of decision aid reliance have been found in addition to those identified by both the Technology Acceptance Model and TTD (see figure 3 below). 4 The goal frames differ on whether the auditor should choose to test alternative explanations from a long list of alternatives (inclusive) or should not (exclusive). Mueller and Anderson point out that deciding which to include or exclude are logically identical, however the framing may cause auditors to process the decision differently. 23 5.1 Technology Acceptance Model and the Theory of Technology Dominance At its core, the Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) (Davis 1989) establishes a causal relationship between potential users of some technology, and their eventual acceptance or rejection of that technology. TAM posits that the user’s intention directly determines whether the individual will use the technology or not. This behavioral intention is driven by the user’s perceptions about both the usefulness and ease-of-use of the technology. Figure 3 Factors shown to increase or have no effect/decrease reliance on decision aids5 Increase reliance No effect or decrease reliance Active Involvement (Davis and Counter-Intuitiveness (Messier et al. 2001) Kottemann 1994) Continuous Feedback (Davis and Internal Locus of Control (Kaplan et al. 2001) Kottemann 1994) Constructive Dialogue (Eining et al. Incentives, Feedback, and Pressure (combined) 1997) (Ashton 1990) Ease of Use (Davis 1989) Increasing Task Difficulty (Brown and Jones 1998) Face Validity (Ashton 1990) One-Time Outcome Feedback (Arkes et al. 1986) Framing Effects (Lowe and Reckers Pattern Matching (Hoch and Schkade 1996) 2000; Mueller and Anderson 2002) Intuitiveness (Kachelmeier and Self-Confidence (Arkes et al. 1986; Ashton 1990; Messier 1990; Messier et al. 2001) Whitecotton 1996) Perceived Usefulness (Davis 1989; High degree of Task Experience (Whitecotton Venkatesh and Davis 2000; 1996) Venkatesh et al. 2003) Previous Decision Aid Experience (Whitecotton 1996; Whitecotton and Butler 1998) Relative Source Objectiveness (Anderson et al. 2003) Rule Description (Davis and Kottemann 1995) Sophistication of the Decision Aid (Swinney 1999) 5 This is a summary of outcomes found and explained throughout the course of this review. Also, note that I include research that is not specific to auditors, or did not use auditor participants, in italics. 24 The Theory of Technology Dominance (TTD) (Arnold and Sutton 1998) was designed in part to explain a user’s tendency toward or away from reliance on intelligent decision aids. When task experience is low, TTD hypothesizes that there will be a tendency toward reliance. This type of reliance is suboptimal because the user does not have personal task knowledge to factor in with the decision aid suggestion. When task experience is high, and task complexity, aid familiarity and cognitive fit are all high, TTD hypothesizes that there will be a tendency toward reliance.6 This type of reliance is optimal. However, when task experience is high but task complexity, aid familiarity, or cognitive fit are low, there will be a tendency toward nonreliance. This non-reliance is suboptimal because the decision aid is not being used to supplement personal task knowledge. TTD further theorizes that over the course of time, a decision maker with high task experience using an intelligent decision aid will eventually lose skill related to that task. 5.2 Combining TTD, TAM, and Extant Research Although much research has been done in the area of decision aids, we still lack a uniform theory. Auditors have shown a general under-reliance tendency on decision aids. Now, as technologically advanced decision aids continue to gain acceptance, recent studies show instances of over-reliance problems as well. Each of these problems presents potential benefits if solved, and potentially tremendous consequences that remain largely unexplored. Continuing to identify and understand the factors and consequences under which auditor reliance on decision aids is influenced remains an important task. TAM includes usability and performance related constructs that are applicable to decision aid reliance research. TTD includes a model of factors hypothesized to influence reliance on Arnold and Sutton define cognitive fit as “the degree to which the cognitive processes used with the decision aid to complete or solve a task match the cognitive processes normally used by an experienced decision maker” (p. 180 1998) 6 25 decision aids. These factors include the task experience of the user, the complexity of the task, familiarity of the user with the decision aid, and the cognitive fit between the processes of the decision aid and those required by the task. The determinants featured in TTD are consistent with many of the audit based decision aid literature included in this review. TTD and TAM do not explain all factors found in extant research, however. Absent from these theories are certain factors that may be found at the individual level, audit firm level, within different tasks, or at the decision aid level, such as locus of control, which have been demonstrated to affect auditor reliance (see Kaplan et al. 2001). Other decision aid related factors affecting auditor reliance, such as framing effects (see Lowe and Reckers 2000; Mueller and Anderson 2002), may be a function of firm culture, which is not captured in either of these theories. Accountability to the firm as well as the public creates a strong incentive to avoid errors. Reminding auditors about the potential consequences to the firm if certain decision errors are made can influence reliance. The decision to include such framing effects or language in decision aids is likely determined by the firm culture. Firms that encourage, or make it tougher to disagree with decision aid suggestions, are likely able to influence auditor reliance. In light of these theoretical limitations, I present a model that draws on extant research and both of the above theories – TTD and TAM in figure 4 below. The model intends to capture the major constructs parsimoniously and in a manner that can be empirically tested. TTD, TAM, and the preceding literature imply certain propositions related to each factor listed. Those implications, which have been discussed throughout the review, include the following. Expected determinants of increasing decision aid reliance include a moderate degree of task familiarity, an external locus of control, firm culture with explicit training and expectations for decision aid 26 usage, high decision aid familiarity, strong cognitive fit, high degree of justification, and salient user perception of decision aid usefulness. As discussed previously, certain combinations of factors might deter decision aid reliance – particularly those discussed within TTD. Figure 4: Proposed Model of Auditor Decision Aid Reliance Decision Aid Auditor* 1, 4 2, 7 3, 6 Making Decision 1 Decision Process 1 – Task Familiarity 2 – Locus of Control 3 – Firm Culture 4 – Decision Aid Familiarity 5, 7 Decision Outcome 5 – Cognitive Fit 6 – Justification 7 – Perceived Decision Aid Usefulness * – Firm culture and the justification requirement make auditors unique from other decision makers While literature has begun addressing auditor reliance on decision aids, opportunities for further research exist. The lack of a unifying theory currently plagues this line of research; however, TTD encompasses a promising model of decision aid reliance. By fitting TAM and the reliance model of TTD together, and finding where unrelated factors found in extant literature fit in, we can move toward developing a universal theory of auditor reliance on decision aids. It is important to continue refining our understanding of user, task, and decision aid characteristics that interact to cause reliance judgments. 27 Abdolmohammadi, M. and C. Usoff (2001). "A longitudinal study of applicable decision aids for detailed tasks in a financial audit." International Journal of Intelligent Systems in Accounting, Finance and Management 10(3): 139. Abdolmohammadi, M. J. (1991). Factors affecting auditors' perceptions of applicable decision aids for various audit tasks. Contemporary Accounting Research, Canadian Academic Accounting Association. 7: 535-548. Abdolmohammadi, M. J. (1999). "A comprehensive taxonomy of audit task structure, professional rank and decision aids for behavioral research." Behavioral Research in Accounting 11: 51. Anderson, J. C., K. K. Moreno and J. M. Mueller (2003). "The effect of client vs. decision aid as a source of explanations upon auditors' sufficiency judgments: A research note." Behavioral Research in Accounting 15: 1. Arkes, H., R. Dawes and C. Christensen (1986). "Factors influencing the use of decision rule in a probabilistic task." Organizational Performance and Human Performance: 93-110. Arnold, V. and S. G. Sutton (1998). The Theory of Technology Dominance: Understanding the Impact of Intelligent Decision Aids on Decision Maker's Judgments. Advances in Accounting Behavioral Research 1, JAI Press: 175-194. Ashton, R. H. (1992). "Effects of Justification and a Mechanical Aid on Judgment Performance." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 52(2): 292. Ashton, R. H. H., Vicky (1990). Pressure and Performance in Accounting Decision Settings: Paradoxical Effects of Incentives, Feedback, and Justification. Journal of Accounting Research, Blackwell Publishing Limited. 28: 148. Bamber, E. M., Mary Callahan Hill, Richard T. Watson (1998). Audit groups and group support systems: a framework and propositions for future research. Journal of Information Systems, American Accounting Association. 12: 45. Bedard, J. C. and L. E. Graham (2002). "The effects of decision aid orientation on risk factor identification and audit test planning." Auditing 21(2): 39. Bell, T. B., J. C. Bedard, K. M. Johnstone and E. F. Smith (2002). "KRisk(SM): A computerized decision aid for client acceptance and continuance risk assessments." Auditing 21(2): 97. Boatsman, J. R., C. Moeckel and B. K. W. Pei (1997). "The Effects of Decision Consequences on Auditors' Reliance on Decision Aids in Audit Planning,." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 71(2): 211-247. Bonner, S. E., R. Libby and M. W. Nelson (1996). "Using decision aids to improve auditors' conditional probability judgments." The Accounting Review 71(2): 221. Brown, D. L. and D. R. Jones (1998). Factors that influence reliance on decision aids: a model and an experiment. Journal of Information Systems, American Accounting Association. 12: 75. Davis, F. D. (1989). "Perceived Usefulness, Perceived Ease Of Use, And User Acceptance." MIS Quarterly 13(3): 319. Davis, F. D. and J. E. Kottemann (1994). "User perceptions of decision support effectiveness: Two production planning experiments." Decision Sciences 25(1): 57. Davis, F. D. and J. E. Kottemann (1995). "Determinants of Decision Rule Use in a Production Planning Task." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 63(2): 145-157. 28 Dilla, W. N. and D. N. Stone (1997). "Representations as decision aids: The asymmetric effects of words and numbers on auditors' inherent risk judgments." Decision Sciences 28(3): 709. Eining, M. M., D. R. Jones and J. K. Loebbecke (1997). "Reliance on Decision Aids: An Examination of Auditors' Assessment of Management Fraud." Auditing: a Journal of Practice and Theory 16: 1-19. Hansen, J. V., J. B. McDonald, W. F. Messier Jr. and T. B. Bell (1996). A generalized qualitative-response model and the analysis of management fraud. Management Science, INFORMS: Institute for Operations Research. 42: 1022. Hoch, S. J. and D. A. Schkade (1996). A psychological approach to decision support systems. Management Science, INFORMS: Institute for Operations Research. 42: 51. Kachelmeier, S. J. and W. F. Messier (1990). "An Investigation Of The Influence Of A Nonstatistical Decision Aids." The Accounting Review 65(1): 209. Kaplan, S. E., J. H. Reneau and S. Whitecotton (2001). "The effects of predictive ability information, locus of control, and decision maker involvement on decision aid reliance." Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 14(1): 35-50. Kleinmuntz, B. (1990). "Why We Still Use Our Heads Instead of Formulas: Toward an Integrative Approach." Psychological Bulletin 3(107): 296-310. Leech, S. A. and A. Sangster (2002). Expert Systems in Accounting Research: A Design Science Perspective. Researching Accounting as an Information System Discipline. V. Arnold and S. G. Sutton. Sarasota, FL, American Accounting Association Information Systems Section: 65-79. Lowe, D. J. and P. M. J. Reckers (2000). "The use of foresight decision aids in auditors' judgments." Behavioral Research in Accounting 12: 97. Messier, W. F. (1995). Research in and Development of Audit Decision Aids. Judgment and Decision-Making Research in Accounting and Auditing. R. H. Ashton and A. H. Ashton. New York, NY, Cambridge University Press: 207-228. Messier, W. F. and J. V. Hansen (1987). Expert Systems in Auditing: The State of the Art. Auditing, American Accounting Association. 7: 94. Messier, W. F., S. J. Kachelmeier and K. L. Jensen (2001). "An experimental assessment of recent professional developments in nonstatistical audit sampling guidance." Auditing 20(1): 81. Messier, W. F., S. J. Kachelmeier and K. L. Jensen (2001). An Experimental Assessment of Recent Professional Developments in Nonstatistical Audit Sampling Guidance. Auditing, American Accounting Association. 20: 81. Mueller, J. M. and J. C. Anderson (2002). "Decision aids for generating analytical review alternatives: The impact of goal framing and audit-risk level." Behavioral Research in Accounting 14: 157. Nieschwietz, R. J., J. J. Schultz Jr. and M. F. Zimbelman (2000). "Empirical research on external auditors' detection of financial statement fraud." Journal of Accounting Literature 19: 190. Rohrmann, B. (1986). Evaluating the Usefulness of Decision Aids: A Methodological Perspective. New Directions in Research and Decision Making. B. Brehmer, H. Jungermann, P. Lourens and G. Sevon. Amsterdam, The Netherlands, North Holland Publishing Company: 363-381. 29 Rose, J. M. (2002). Behavioral Decision Aid Research: Decision Aid Use and Effects. Researching Accounting as an Information Systems Discipline. V. Arnold and S. G. Sutton. Sarasota, FL, American Accounting Association: 111-133. Solomon, I. and M. D. Shields (1995). Judgment and decision-making research in auditing. Judgment and decision-making research in accounting and auditing. R. H. Ashton and A. H. Ashton. Cambridge, England ; New York, Cambridge University Press: xvi, 294. Sutton, S. G. and V. Arnold (2002). Foundations and Frameworks for AIS Research. Researching Accounting as an Information Discipline. V. Arnold and S. G. Sutton. Sarasota, FL, American Accounting Association: 3-10. Swinney, L. (1999). "Consideration of the social context of auditors' reliance on expert system output during evaluation of loan loss reserves." International Journal of Intelligent Systems in Accounting, Finance and Management 8(3): 199. Venkatesh, V. and F. D. Davis (2000). A Theoretical Extension of the Technology Acceptance Model: Four Longitudinal Field Studies. Management Science, INFORMS: Institute for Operations Research. 46: 186. Venkatesh, V., M. G. Morris, G. B. Davis and F. D. Davis (2003). "User acceptance of information technology: Toward a unified view." MIS Quarterly 27(3): 425. Whitecotton, S. M. (1996). "The effects of experience and confidence on decision aid reliance: A causal model." Behavioral Research in Accounting 8: 194. Whitecotton, S. M. and S. A. Butler (1998). Influencing Decision Aid Reliance Through Involvement in Information Choice. Behavioral Research in Accounting, American Accounting Association. 10: 182. 30