Aggression, Altruism, and Moral Development When parents are asked what moral qualities they hope their children will learn, the following things were marked as primary: 1) Avoid hurting others (control aggression) 2) Prosocial concern for others (altruism) 3) Sense of personal values and ethics Aggression-behavior done for the purpose of harming another. So intent defines an act as aggressive, not the ultimate consequences. 1) Hostile aggression-main goal is to inflict harm or injury on the victim. 2) Instrumental aggression-goal is to gain access to something desirable. 3) Retaliatory aggression-aggression in response to real or imagined provocation. 4) Relational aggression-social exclusion, withdrawing acceptance, sabotaging or gossiping about another in order to damage self-esteem, power, or friendships. More common for girls. Trends over time- 1 yr-olds will act aggressively if someone is frustrating their goal toward an object, but the act seems to merely be an effort to get what they want. 2 yr-olds will more likely attempt to negotiate with another to get the desired object. Parents can be effective in training children at this young age to share and ask. Temper tantrums for 2 yr-olds give way to active aggression in service to anger or frustration by age 3. By age 5 physical aggression is replaced by name-calling, tattling, and verbal aggression. Older children learn to assess intention in order to decide if retaliatory aggression is okay. Gender differences-males are more physically and verbally aggressive except in 1 – 2 yr. olds. By age 3 gender differences emerge in a consistent way. It’s not that girls aren’t aggressive, it’s that they are more covert in their aggression. Since girls are more motivated by needs for relationships and acceptance by others, this is a significant area to display hostility towards others. So exclusion and rumor-mongering can be extremely hurtful to other girls. Older girls are actually engaging in more physical aggression today. Aggression is a relatively stable attribute over time. Types of aggressors 1) proactive aggression-see aggression as a successful means to an end- to get what they want. They may actually display happy emotions as they exhibit aggression toward others, since they expect a payoff. 2) reactive aggression-suspicious of others’ motives, seeing others as adversaries who deserve their aggressive treatment. Social Information-Processing Theory of Aggression suggests that the child’s past experiences and mental state set up expectancies of others which they use to assess others’ intentionality in ambiguous circumstances. If one’s expectation is victimization and believing others have a hostile intent, his response to normal bickering, etc. is more likely to be aggressive- to protect himself (hostile attributional bias). Unfortunately this creates resentment and hostility on the part of peers, isolating and rejecting the angry child. Bullies vs. whipping boys-bullies have often witnessed violence at home but rarely been the target. They learn that violence pays off, so they use it with peers. They see victims as easy marks to use to get what they want (proactive aggressors). (Christmas Story). This does not cause other children to protect the victims – they are generally disliked by others. 1) Passive victims-withdrawn, physically weak, reluctant to fight back, do little to invite the aggression. May have had overprotective mothers. 2) Provocative victims-oppositional, hottempered who seem to deliberately irritate peers, fight back unsuccessfully. Often physically abused at home and learn to see others as adversaries and threats. Victimized children are at risk: for loneliness, anxiety, depression, poor self-esteem, avoidance of school. They need social skills training and even self-defense training to enhance their selfconfidence. Cultural influences on aggression-different cultures have different values regarding the acceptability of aggression. The US is tolerant of aggression in service of problem-solving. The male culture in urban, lower SES groups are more aggressive. Aggression thrives when there is low chance of paying a significant personal price. Coercive Home Environments-commonalities in families that raise aggressive children are bickering, needling, deliberate irritations of others. When these techniques work to get another to leave them alone, they are reinforcing. Parents rarely use positive reinforcement for loving behavior or prosocial behavior. In some families the only attention the kids get is for acting out. Even negative attention is attention. In the absence of other attention, kids will take the negative attention if necessary. But they learn to perceive others as hostile, coming out of such a punitive environment. It also results in children who are resistant to punishment. Nothing makes an impression on them. Eventually this contributes to poor impulse control, academic problems, and peer problems. They gravitate to the marginalized in the school, finding acceptance in gangs and antisocial peers. Once they commit to a deviant peer group, academics are devalued, aggression embraced, dropping out seems more acceptable. Antisocial boys are likely to pair up with antisocial girls, creating abusive relationships and early parenthood. Interventions need to target the parents’ skills in managing conflict and learning to respond more effectively to their children’s needs. Other interventions target the child having academic problems to keep them from getting so frustrated in school that they give up. Methods of Controlling Aggression 1) Creating nonaggressive Environmentsreduce access to aggressive toys, video games, guns. Expand other play activities. 2) Eliminating the Payoffs for Aggressionrequires alertness and assertiveness in reacting by the parent in order to short-circuit early attempts to force compliance on another. a. Incompatible response techniqueignoring unacceptable behavior and giving attention to positive behaviors, sharing, cooperation that are incompatible with aggression (a slowly effective technique that most parents can’t tolerate) b. Time-out Technique-removing the hostile child from the setting until they calm down. Only works if the child is being removed from a desirable place to a place with no rewards. 3) Social-cognitive interventions-coaching aggressive children in empathizing with others, generating milder solutions to conflicts, and seeing others’ behavior as less hostile. 4) Preventing violence at school-programs entail preventive strategies: reducing payoffs for aggression, teaching prosocial responses, learning self-control of emotions, negotiations to find solutions to conflict. These programs have resulted in significantly less violence on campus as well as less prejudice. Altruism-selfless concern for the welfare of others that is shown by prosocial acts, such as sharing, cooperating and helping. Origins of altruism-even 2 yr-olds show signs of sympathy toward others and compassion toward those in distress. More inhibited children may not attempt to help, but they show signs of distress when another is upset. Most significant in training compassion is the parents’ responses to distress. Parents of compassionate toddlers used affective explanations when dealing with misdoing. “How would that make you feel if Joe did that to you?” Toddlers will share if instructed, but spontaneous self-sacrifice doesn’t happen at this age. Prosocial behavior increases with age. Girls are no more empathic or helpful than boys, although it is displayed differently. Prosocial Moral Reasoning-how we decide whether to help another, share with them or comfort others, particularly if there is a cost to ourselves. Preschoolers make these decisions based on selfserving interests. Maturity shows in willingness to help someone you don’t like if they were in need. This displays a child’s ability to empathize with others. Empathy is a person’s ability to experience the emotions of other people. We come into the world with it, but it can be suppressed or enhanced by environment. Social concern is modeled by parents in their concern and response to the child’s needs. Parents who actually talk about their feelings as they act sympathetically develop sensitivity in their children toward others. (Children whose pain is reacted to abusively by a parent actually show disintegration of a significant structure of the brain related to social concern and empathy. These children are at risk of becoming sociopaths with no feelings toward their victims.) Children become more effective in helping others as they mature in their cognition, allowing them to think more creatively and abstractly in the face of another’s need. Felt-Responsibility hypothesis-empathy may promote altruism by causing one to reflect on norms of social responsibility and feel some obligation to help another. Now the child feels guilty if s/he ignores personal responsibility in helping another. This link becomes stronger with age. Cultural influences on altruism-less industrialized, more closely knit cultures develop more altruistic children. These cultures train children to contribute to the family well-being by helping in the home, field. In US, most children don’t have significant tasks that benefit the family, but those who do have routine chores that benefit others do develop more prosocial skills. Western culture also emphasizes individual achievement and competition, over group goals. So in collectivist societies, helping others is not seen as a choice but a social expectation. Not so in Western culture. Altruism can be verbally reinforced and once focused, children will tend to try to live up to positive expectations that have been set for them. Modeling charitable work is powerful, especially if the child has a warm relationship with the model. Also it emphasizes that people practice what they preach. Effective parents have a warm relationship with their children and they act in concert with their stated principles. Also these effective parents respond quickly and effectively to children’s harm to others. They do not ignore these behaviors. They expect children to display a sense of personal responsibility for their behaviors. They also give children means of undoing their transgressions. Morality1) capacity to distinguish right from wrong 2) act on this distinction 3) experience pride in virtuous conduct and guilt over acts that violate one’s standards Internalization- adopting the standards of other people as your own. Shifting control of behavior from external controls (risk of arrest) to internal control (my own ethical standards). Components of morality 1) Affective 2) Cognitive 3) Behavioral Theories of Moral Development- different theories focus on the different components. Psychoanalytic focus on affect- we act morally to feel good about ourselves and avoid guilt and shame. Cognitivedevelopmental theories focus on cognitive aspects, how we reason about moral behavior which changes as we develop cognitively. Social learning and information-processing people focus on actual behavior- resisting temptation, inhibiting immoral impulses. Psychoanalytic – Freudian theory is based on the concept that people behave ethically to feel good about themselves and avoid guilt. But coercive parents don’t raise more moral children. Boys don’t show greater moral development than girls. Even toddlers show distress with others, before they would have experienced the Oedipal conflict. Toddlers actually show greater compassion to others when they are more securely attached to a parent, and that relationship is mutually responsive not punitive. Aloof or insensitive parents promote situational compliance- compliance based on a parent’s power to control the child and impose punishment. Cognitive-Developmental theory-examine the development of moral reasoning as they assess what is right and wrong. a. Piaget’s theory-developed by playing marbles with children & asking about the rules for play. He also gave them moraldecision stories to interpret. Premoral period- preschoolers show little concern for rules. They don’t play to win, they make up their own rules and play to have fun (what a concept!) Heteronomous morality-ages 5 – 10 children develop strong respect for rules, thinking they are immutable, moral absolutes. They also believe in immanent justice- that all rule violators will be punished. Justice for all. Autonomous morality- by 10 –11 children understand rules are arbitrarily created and can be changed, can be violated for a higher good. Now intent is very important in judging an act. They favor reciprocal punishments- punishments that fit the act. This development requires a decline in egocentric thinking and an ability to take others’ perspectives. As older children develop a sense of respect for peers and self, it decreases their reliance on external arbiters of behavior and they internalize autonomy in morality. Partly this is a response to parents relinquishing control as children mature. Overcontrolling parents inhibit development of self-control. As in other areas, Piaget is underestimating b. Kohlberg’s theory- a stage theory of moral development i. Moral dilemmas-using fictional moral dilemmas, children were asked to decide what the right solution was and then asked why they picked that. ii. Preconventional morality-rules are imposed by society and the child conforms to avoid punishment or obtain personal rewards 1. Punishment and Obedience Orientation 2. Naïve hedonism iii. Conventional morality-obeying the rules to gain others’ approval or maintain social order 1. Good Boy or Good Girl 2. Social Order Maintenance iv. Postconventional morality-right and wrong is defined in terms of principles of justice, higher ethics 1. Social Contract Orientation 2. Morality of Individual Principles These stages generally follow development, but few grow to the level of Stage 5 and 6. It does relate to cognitive development as Piaget described. v. Cultural and gender bias-it does reflect a Western and male ideal of justice, so it doesn’t measure communal reasoning (or female) as accurately. Gilligan showed that socialization made a difference in how we reason. Men reason from a morality of justice while women reason from a morality of care. Recent studies show little difference in reasoning between M/F. Doctrine of specificity- the idea that moral affect, behavior are dependent on the situation. Moral behavior may not seem consistent. An important aspect of moral growth is ability to resist temptation. When children can do this, even when alone and possibility of detection is remote, they have internalized that moral norm. Why do some children internalize values and others not? 1. If a warm adult has reinforced compliance. 2. Punishment used is firm, immediate, consistent and explained to the child. This gives the child information about why the punished act is wrong & why they should feel guilty. It also helps if the adult reinforces that the child is moral and kind, so s/he wouldn’t do this thing. Combined with praise for self-control, this establishes inhibitory control. 3. Modeling of moral behavior is important. Parents need to do what they ask children to do. What do successful parents do that result in moral children? The following are common discipline techniqueswhich ones are effective? 1) Love withdrawal-adult withholds attention, affection or approval to control a child. 2) Power assertion-spankings or withholding privileges to control child. 3) Induction-adult explains why a child’s behavior is wrong and emphasizes his/her behavior’s effects on others. Power assertion relates more to moral immaturity, love withdrawal has little effect, & induction relates to moral maturity. Induction also relates to development of empathy for others. This is not entirely a top-down process. Acting out, fearless children influence parents in the direction of coercion. Fearful, easily upset children induce parents to use gentler forms of discipline. Children prefer inductive discipline and are more motivated to accept influence from an inductive adult whom they respect. Aggression, Altruism, and Moral Development When parents are asked what moral qualities they hope their children will learn, the following things were marked as primary: Avoid hurting others (control aggression) Prosocial concern for others (altruism) Sense of personal values and ethics Forms of Aggression: Hostile aggression Instrumental aggression Retaliatory aggression Relational aggression Trends over time Gender differences Genetic vs. familial influences Types of aggressors: proactive aggression reactive aggression Social Information-Processing Theory of Aggression (hostile attributional bias) Bullies vs. whipping boys: Passive victims Provocative victims Cultural influences on aggression Coercive Home Environments Methods of Controlling Aggression: Creating nonaggressive Environments Eliminating the Payoffs for Aggression a. Incompatible response technique b. Time-out Technique Social-cognitive interventions Preventing violence at school Altruism-selfless concern for the welfare of others that is shown by prosocial acts, such as sharing, cooperating and helping. Origins of altruism Prosocial Moral Reasoning Empathy Felt-Responsibility hypothesis Cultural influences on altruism Moralitycapacity to distinguish right from wrong willingness to act on this distinction experience pride in virtuous conduct and guilt over acts that violate one’s standards Internalization- adopting the standards of other people as your own. Components of morality Affective Cognitive Behavioral Theories of Moral Development Psychoanalytic – Freudian Cognitive-Developmental 1. Piaget’s theory Premoral period Heteronomous morality Autonomous morality 2. Kohlberg’s theory Moral dilemmas Preconventional morality Conventional morality Postconventional morality Cultural and gender bias What do successful parents do that results in moral children? Love withdrawal Power assertion Induction Children prefer inductive discipline and are more motivated to accept influence from an inductive adult whom they respect.