Condition and spatial distribution The condition of individual bush-crickets influence their spatial distribution DAG Ø. HJERMANN Department of Biology, Division of Zoology, PO Box 1050 Blindern, N-0316 Oslo, Norway Abstract Males of many species of grasshoppers and crickets (Orthoptera) form aggregations when calling for females. However, to our knowledge it has not been reported that the males' spatial position within aggregation correlates with mating success, such as in many species of lekking birds and mammals. Males of the wart-biter Decticus verrucivorus, a bush-cricket (Tettiginiidae), are attracted by other males' song and so have an innate tendency to aggregate. They also provide no parental care. Their mating system therefore resembles a lek (but not a "classical" lek, since males hold floating territories). In this experiment, I "created" animals of good and poor condition by rearing approximately half of the animals under suboptimal conditions. The animals were released in equal density in eight experimental patches of three sizes (20, 40 and 80 m2), and subsequently tracked by recording their positions four times per day during three weeks. Males that were raised under suboptimal conditions moved less, sang less and had lower song amplitude than the other animals, confirming that they were of poorer condition. The movement patterns revealed that in medium-sized patches, poor-condition animals of both sexes stayed more in the edge zone (< 1 m from habitat edge) than expected by chance, while good-condition animals used the edge zone and interior equally much. The same tendency was found in the only small patch with good-condition animals during most of the experiment. During the peak hours of male singing, however, poor-conditioned females (but not poor-conditioned males) moved towards the center of the patch, where predominantly good-condition males were advertising. In the large patches, however, animals used the interior and the periphery equally much regardless of condition. The results indicate that in the small and medium patches, poor-condition males are repelled by males of better condition because they latter sing louder or more frequently. The spatial segretation between good- and poor-condition females was surprising; the most plausible explanation appears to be that females compete for access to attractive males. Wartbiter males may only mate once a day under ideal conditions, and more infrequently under Norwegian climatic conditions. The lack of differences in the edge-interior use in the large patches may be caused by larger habitat area (less need or higher cost of defending the interior) or by larger population size (the optimal behavioral strategy depends on population size). The interaction between condition and patch size is interesting in the light of the excessive habitat fragmentation of many habitats, including the wart-biters' natural habitat. 1 Paper III Introduction In many species of grasshoppers and crickets (Orthoptera), the males form aggregations when calling for females (e.g., Campbell and Clark 1971; Schatral 1984). In some species, males seem to clump mainly because the preferred microhabitat, usually tall plants, is clumped, i.e., there is no need to invoke an innate tendency to clump to explain the observed distribution (Arak and Eiriksson 1992). In these species, clumping may reduce the ability of males to attract females (Arak et al. 1990), so when the area of suitable microhabitat is limited, males may be forced to settle more densely than optimal. In contrast, males of the wart-biter Decticus verrucivorus are attracted to other singing males (postive phonotaxis; Schatral et al. 1985; Keuper et al. 1986). This, together with the impression that wart-biter males congregate in smaller areas than the preferred vegetation, lead Weidemann et al. (1990) to propose that the wart-biter mating system is a resource-based lek, i.e., that positive phonotaxis leads to a denser aggregation than one would expect from the distribution of microhabitat alone. The male does not provide parental care (Wedell and Arak 1989; Wedell 1993), so it indeed appears likely that wart-biters actually display a lek in the relative broad sense ("an aggregated male display that females attend primarily for the purpose of mating"; Höglund and Alatalo 1996:6). In many species of lekking birds and mammals, "dominant" males tend to stay in the middle of the male aggregation (Tab. 3.1 in Höglund & Alatalo 1995). However, this has to my knowledge never been reported in Orthoptera. In the current study, I studied the spatial movement patterns of wart-biters that were manipulated so they were of either good or poor condition. The animals were individually marked and their movements were followed during an extended period of time in experimental patches of different sizes. I specifically tested whether "dominant" males tended to stay in the middle of the male aggregation, and whether the spatial orginazations of the animals was affected by habitat or population size. Methods Study species and experimental design The wart-biter (Decticus verrucivorus L.) is a relatively large bush-cricket (length 34 cm); it has wings, but most individuals are not capable of flying more than a couple of meters (Ander 1947). In Norway, adult males are largely found in July and August. Adult males emerge before females, probably to avoid sperm competition by mating with virgin 2 Condition and spatial distribution females (Wedell 1992). The diet of the wart-biter is a combination of insects (especially acridid grasshoppers) and plant food. Both instars and adults are extremely heat-loving, and the habitat (in Scandinavia) is low meadow or grassland vegetation, preferably including microhabitats with tall vegetation (male singing perches) and vegetation-free spots (where females prefer lay their eggs; Ingrisch and Boekholt 1982; Cherrill and Brown 1990; Cherrill et al. 1991). Adults sing mostly between 1000 and 1200, on hot days also around 1500. Males move frequently around during these periods, singing for a few minutes from each perch. Females approach the male when they want to mate; mating is very short (1-2 minutes). The experiment was performed during the summer of 1997 at Oslo University’s experimental station at Evenstad, SE Norway. Eight habitat patches were made by mowing parts of a continuous area of seminatural pasture. Because the available area was shaped as a narrow rectangle, all habitat islands were 4-6 m wide, while their lengths varied from 4 to 16 m. Two islands were relatively large (ca. 80 m2), two were medium (ca. 40 m2), and four were small (ca. 20 m2) (Fig. 1). Since wart-biters prefer a habitat mosaic of open ground and vegetation (Cherrill and Brown 1990), I created a number of vegetation-free spots within each patch. The patches were protected from bird predation using nets suspended 1.5 - 2 m above the ground (the wart-biters could move freely through it). To the north and south, adjacent patches were separated by 20-25 metres of short-cut lawn. To the western side there was 80100 cm of earth, then a 60-cm fence of transparent plastic seaparating the study area from a corn field. On the eastern side, there was ca. 2 m short-cut lawn between the patch and a 60 cm high fence of solid metal. There were no wart-biters on the study site in advance. The animals were caught in semi-natural meadows and roadsides in five locations in SE Norway. Some were caught as 2.-3. instars and reared in the laboratory until they were adult, while Wire fence Corn field Habitat patches Bare ground Metal fence 10 m 50 m North Figure 1. Map of the experimental area. The experimental patches are arranged in a row between a corn field in the west and another experimental area in the east. Between the patches was extremely short-cut grass. 3 Paper III others were caught as adults shortly before the experiment. In the laboratory, the insects were kept individually in plastic boxes (measuring 16 x 8 x 5.5 cm) with a bit dry vegetation (to climb on to prior to moulting) and fed with cabbage leaves and ground dried catfood. Partly because there was not enough space for the final moult, the laboratory conditions were clearly suboptimal, as evidenced by small physical deformities and lower song performance and movement activity in males (see Results). I therefore refer to laboratory-reared and adultcaugh animals as the groups "poor condition" and "good condition", repectively. The animals were marked by gluing one end of a red, numbered 80 x 4 mm plastic tape to the top of the pronotum, cooled down and released in the experimental habitat patches at about 5 p.m. to avoid escape reactions because of handling. On June 24 at 0500, adults were released in equal densities in each of the eight patches (4, 8 and 16 in the small, medium and large ones, respectively) in approximately even sex ratios (male:female number was 5:3 in one medium patch and 9:7 in the large ones) and with approximately even proportions of good- and poorcondition animals. However, 23 of the animals migrated between patches during the study, especially from the small patches; migration rate was especially high in good-condition males. The populations in the small patches were therefore biased towards poor-conditioned females at the end of the study. Animals were usually tracked four times a day on almost every day from day 4 to day 23 of the experiment and on two days thereafter. In each tracking session, I measured the position of each individual relative to a 25 x 25 cm grid which was marked in the patches using small poles. During tracking, the animals were usually not disturbed so much that they moved. I also noted activities such as singing, mating and egg-laying. For each male, I assessed the song volume on a scale from 0 (not audible) to 3 (very loud). Temperatures were recorded (usually 20 - 30 times/day) using an ordinary ethanol thermometer lying sun-exposed on ground level leaves in order to mimic the body temperatures of basking wart-biters. Movement activity Movement activity or speed was analysed by linear regression, based on the distance between two subsequent recordings on the same day (r) and the time interval (t) between the two recordings. The transformation loge(r2) was used as the response variable, while the predictor variables were ln(t) together with covariates (these transformations are suitable to describe random walks and similar movement patterns; Johnson et al. 199x, Hjermann 2000). I used the following covariates: the identity number of the animal (ind) and the mean temperature during the time interval between recordings (temp, calculated from temperature 4 Condition and spatial distribution measurements smoothened out using 2nd degree polynomial regression), and temp2. I tested whether any of the covariates could be excluded from the model using Mallow's Cp. On the individual level I tested the effect of patch size, sex, catch location (the geographic origin of the animals), and condition (lab-reared vs. wild-caught) on movement activity, using each individual’s estimate of the ind parameter as an indicator of movement activity. Use of the edge zone vs. the patch interior As a condensed measure of choice of microhabitat within each patch, I calculated the use of edge zones vs. the patch interior for each individual. The edge zone of the patch was defined as the part of the patch lying within 1 m of the patch edge. Because the patches were relatively small, I assumed the entire patch to be equally available to the animals. I defined the selection index SI following Manly (1974, eq. 3): SIedge = ratioedge/(ratioedge + ratiointerior), where ratio = (proportion of use)/(proportion of area). SIedge was calculated for each patch*animal combination with at least five observations. SIedge can vary from 0 to 1. Animals outside the patch were not included when SIedge was calculated. A rough measure of the range area of each animal was calculated from the median as well as the 10th and 90th percentiles of the x- and y-coordinates (xmed, x10, x90, etc.). The range area was calculated as the area of the irregularly diamond-shaped area between the points (x10, ymed),(xmed, y90), (x90, ymed),(xmed, y10). I did this for all animal-patch combinations with at least eight observations. Results Movement and song activity The animals moved quite much on sunny days, but some of them could spend several days within a limited area (Fig. 2). The optimal model (as measured by Mallow's Cp) to explain movement length between subsequent observations on the same day (r) included all the variables considered: the time between observations (t), the average temperature during that time interval (temp), temp2, and the identity of the individual (ind): log(r2) = 1.047 log(t) + 0.72 temp - 0.0109 temp2 + ind + 5 Paper III All factors were significant with P < 0.001. The random component had an estimated variance of 3.82 and appeared normally distributed. In the case of a random walk, the coefficient of log(t) is theoretically expected to equal one (Johnson 1992); thus, the value of 1.047 (SE: 0.147) indicates that the wart-biters' movement paths were close to a random walk on this scale. The temp and temp2 coefficients indicate that the movement length increased with the temperature (measured in the sun) for temperatures up to 28-30 C and flattened out thereafter. Lastly, the significance of the ind parameter (P < 0.0001) indicates A 4 (la b -re a re d m a le ) A 5 (la b -re a re d fe m a le ) 22 A ug 8 A ug 1 Sept 8 A ug 6 A ug b Mark f ound 2 8 J u ly 5 A ug 1 3 m f r o m th e 2 8 J u ly 3 1 J u ly p a tc h o n 1 8 A u g 1 A ug 4 A ug 4 A ug a 4 A ug b 2 7 J u ly 8 A ug 3 0 J u ly S I e d g e = 0 .5 3 a 14 A ug 5 7 A ug S I e d g e = 0 .7 0 A 2 (a d u lt-c a u g h t m a le ) A 3 (a d u lt-c a u g h t fe m a le ) 1 Sept 8 A ug 6 A ug 12 A ug 22 A ug 7 A ug 3 1 J u ly a 3 A ug 8 A ug 2 7 J u ly 4 A ug 2 7 J u ly 15 A ug 7 A ug 14 A ug 5 A ug 12 A ug 13 A ug S I e d g e = 0 .5 8 b b 12 A ug 14 A ug a 3 0 J u ly S I e d g e = 0 .4 9 Fig. 2. Four representative examples (one for each "type" of animal) of animal's movements within a medium patch (4x8 m). Some dates are marked to give an impression of the temporal scale. The border between the edge zone and the interior is marked with a broken line. The edge selection index SIedge for each individual is note expected to be 0.5 when the edge zone and the interior is used equally much. (Note that this index does not reflect the area use of the A5 female very well, partly because observations outside the patch are ignored.) 6 Condition and spatial distribution P o o r-c o n d it io n m a le s 0,7 G o o d -c o n d it io n m a le s E g g -la y in g , fe m a le s 0,6 0,14 0,5 0,1 0,4 0,08 0,3 0,06 0,2 0,04 0,1 Proportion egg-laying Proportion singing 0,12 0,02 0 0 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 Hour of da y Fig. 3. Proportion of observations where males sang (squares, on the left axis) and where females laid eggs (crosses, right axis) during the day. that movement activity differed between individuals. Movement activity, as indicated by each individual's estimate of ind, did not depend on patch, patch size, catch location or sex (ANOVA, P > 0.40). There was an interaction between sex and condition (F1,50 = 4.76, P = 0.034): males caught as adults (males in good condition) were significantly more active than males caught as instars and reared in the lab (males in poor condition; t21 = 5.68, P < 0.001). The same tendency prevailed for females but was not statistically significant (t29 = 2.10, P = 0.16). As expected, singing activity was highest in from ca. 10 a.m. to about noon and declined thereafter (Fig. 3). In addition to display higher movement activity, males in good condition also tended to sing 3-4 times more often than males in poor condition (song frequency per individual; t21 = 5.89, P < 0.0001; Fig. 3). When they sang, good-condition males tended to sing much less loud than poor-condition males (means 1.08 vs. 2.79 on a scale from 0 to 3; t21 = 4.00, P < 0.001). Egg-laying was observed only 22 times (mostly just outside the patches); it was most frequently observed in the afternoon and evening (Fig. 3). There was no difference in egg-laying frequency with regard to condition (Fisher's exact twosided test, P = 0.64). 7 Paper III Table 1. Tests of effects of sex, rearing (lab-bred or adult-caught) and patch size on the use of edgezones, measured by the overall habitat use and by habitat selection within days. Single factors were tested using one-way anova, while the tests for interaction effects are type III tests from two-way anovas (tests of the main effects are not shown). Overall habitat use Selection within days Effect d.f. F P d.f. F P Sex 1,59 0.01 0.97 1,53 0.01 0.91 Sex x Rearing 1,57 0.03 0.87 1,51 0.25 0.62 Sex x Patch size 2,55 1.41 0.25 2,49 1.12 0.33 Patch size x Rearing 2,55 3.59 0.034 2, 49 3.84 0.028 Patch size (among lab-reared animals) 2,40 6.36 0.004 2, 37 6.68 0.003 Patch size (among animals caught as adults) 2,15 0.76 0.49 2, 12 0.97 0.41 Use of edge zone vs. patch interior Use of the edge zone as measured by SIedge did not differ among the sexes, but differed among patches for animals reared in the lab. In the medium-sized patches, poorcondition animals stayed significantly more in the edge zone than in the interior (selection indices > 0.5, Fig. 4, Tab. 1). In the small and large patches, the selection indices indicate that Edge se le ction inde x poor-condition animals on average used edges and interiors equally much. This effect of P o o r c o n d itio n 0 ,7 G o o d c o n d itio n 0 ,5 0 ,3 0 ,5 S m a ll 1 ,5 M e d iu m 2 ,5 L a rg e 3 ,5 P a t c h s iz e Fig. 4. Average edge selection index for poor- and good-condition animals on patches of three sizes. The indicated standard errors (1SE) were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. 8 Condition and spatial distribution P o o r- c o nd it io n f e ma le s 4 Dis ta n c e to p a tc h c e n te r Go o d - c o nd it io n f e ma le s P o o r- c o nd it io n ma le s 3,5 Go o d - c o nd it io n ma le s 3 2,5 2 1,5 - 1000 1000 - 1330 - 1330 1500 1500 - Tim e o f d a y Fig. 5. Each individual's average distance to the patch center during the day, based on the medium patches only. One standard error is indicated; standard errors were calculated using each wart-biter observation as one statistical observation. patch size was not found among good-condition males, and the interaction between patch size and condition was significant (Tab. 1). However, because most good-condition animals left the small patches early in the experiment, the estimates of good-condition animals in these patches are based on only four animals, of which two were were male immigrants who stayed for a short time. In the only small patch were the two groups coexisted for an extended period, the two good-condition animals (one male, one female) stayed more in the patch interior (SIedge = 0.32 and 0.37) than the three poor-condition animals (SIedge = 0.45, 0.62 and 0.69). Thus, this patch shows the same tendency as the medium patches: poor-condition animals of both sexes used the edge more than good-condition animals. To further explore the social processes in the medium patches, I calculated the distance of each observation to the patch center for different times of the day. I found that although the overall habitat use of the sexes is quite similar, poor-condition females approach the center of the patch in the period between 1000 and 1230, in the period when male singing is most intense (Fig. 5). Thus, females of both poor and good condition were spatially associated with adult-caught males in this period. As expected, range areas were smallest in the small patches, but ranges were not larger in the large patches relative to the medium-sized ones (Fig. 6). Range areas did not 9 Paper III F e m a le s R ange are a M a le s 16 F e m a le s M a le s Are a (sq. m ) 12 8 4 0 0 ,5 S m a ll 1 ,5 M e d iu m 2 ,5 L a rg e 3 ,5 P a tc h si z e Fig. 6. Area of the wart-biter ranges, calculated as explained in the text. There were no significant effect of neither sex nor the condition treatment, but a significant effect of patch area (F2,53 = 6.26, P=0.004). The indicated standard errors were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. differ between the sexes or between lab-reared and wild-caught animals, nor were there any significant interactions. As expected, range areas were smallest in the small patches, but ranges were not larger in the large patches relative to the medium-sized ones (Fig. 6). Range areas did not differ between the sexes or between lab-reared and wild-caught animals, nor were there any significant interactions. Discussion The results shows that in patches of medium size, both males and females of poor condition stay more in the edge zone than expected by random usage of the patch. The same tendency can be seen in the only small patch with a stable population including animals of both condition types. Thus, in these patches, animals were to some degree segregated with respect to condition. Also, good-condition males moved more than poor-condition males. Because I chose to study behaviour of several patches somewhat extensively rather than one patch more intesively, I have no evidence that good-condition males have the highest mating success. However, it would be highly surprising if this was not the case. High-quality males sang about double as often as low-quality males; this alone should attract more females (quiet males do not attract females at all). 10 Also, they sang much more loudly in the Condition and spatial distribution frequencies heard by humans, and probably also in the ultrasound frequencies. Song intensity is generally strongly related to attractiveness (e.g., Farris et al. 1997 and references therein) and can override other characteristics of the song (e.g., Snedden and Greenfield 1998). Finally, Keuper et al. (1995) and Kalmring et al. (1990) suggested that male wart-biters move so frequently during display because its song does not reach very far (the wart-biters song has dominant frequencies of 12 kHz and a complex temporal structure, and is strongly reduced and distorted in grassland and meadow). Thus, a male that moves more when singing can be expected to attract more females. The range areas did not differ with coindition, however, which indicates that good-condition males moved more back and forth. Finally, during the song period, females of both condition stayed closer to the center than poor-condition males, indicating that good-condition males indeed have higher mating success. Although the data from this study are not suitable to reveal the proximate and ultimate causes for the observed differences in space use, I find it interesting to discuss possible causes of the observed pattern. First, for males the most important proximate mechanism is likely to be acoustic interactions between calling males. Keuper et al. (1986) found that at long distances, wart-biter males are attracted by other males' song, but at short distances, the song has a repellent effect. When two singing males come close together, they sing in in unison for a few minutes until one of them, or both, draw back (Keuper et al. 1986; personal observation). In addition, Weidemann et al. (1990) found that within aggregations, males had a tendency to be distributed with equal nearest-neighbor distances. Thus, males appear to "defend" a circle using the song. In this study, poor-condition males may be driven out in the periphery by asymmetric song interactions. Whether song frequency (e.g., when one male sings but the other does not) or song amplitude is most important in this asymmetry is not known. The mechanism behind the females' patterns of habitat use is much less certain. It may involve response to the males' song. I have also observed physical aggression between females, including once in this experiment when one female attacked and kicked another. Since I did not observe the animals continuously, such agonistic encounters might have happened regularely without my notice. In contrast I have never observed male-male physical aggression. There are at least two possible ultimate (or evolutionary) causes for space partitioning. The first hypothesis is that the correlation between condition and patch position is analogous to the situation commonly encountered in vertebrate leks, where the dominant and successful males are centrally placed in the lek (e.g., black grouse ; Höglund and Alatalo 11 Paper III 1996:122-136). The motivation for this hypothesis is that the mating system of wart-biters at least superficially appears to have many of the characteristics of leks. First, males put on a display while clumping together in excess of what can be expected from habitat variation (Weidemann et al. 1990). Second, there are ritualized contests between males. Third, there seem to be no parental investment on part of the males (Wedell and Arak 1989). In contrast to "classical" vertebrate leks (e.g., Bradbury 1985), males have no fixed territories, but sing for short periods from each perch and move intermittently. Fixed territories, however, may be a widespread consequence of lek systems rather than a crucial component of it. E.g., "classically" lekking species such as black grouse (Tetrao tetrix) have mobile aggregations similar to the wart-biters when they lek on ice-covered lakes, where there are no landmarks (Hovi et al. 1996). However, much more information about the wart-biter mating system is needed to confirm or reject that they lek. For instance, I have not demonstrated that the central, high-quality males actually have greater mating success than the peripheral males. The other hypothesis is that dominant males seek to minimize predation risk by avoiding the patch edges. Increased predation and parasitation close to the habitat edge is well-known from some bird communities (e.g., Brittingham and Temple 1983). In experimentally fragmented habitats, the adult and litter survival of root voles Microtus oeconomus decreases sharply with increasing edge utilisation due to bird predation (Hovland et al. 1999, Gundersen and Andreassen 2000). In concordance with the predation hypothesis, subordinate individuals of both sexes (such as nonreproductive individuals and individuals with low body mass) used the habitat edges more frequently than dominant individuals (Hovland et al. 1999, Gundersen and Andreassen 2000). As mentioned, we cannot only speculate around the question of which ultimate cause that is most probable. None of the hypotheses is inconsistent with the belief that asymmetric acoustic interactions are the proximate mechanism for the space partitioning between males. In the case of the predation hypothesis, the high edge use of poor-condition males may simply be a side effect of their instinct of keeping a distance to the dominant males. However, the fact that male wart-biters clump also in non-fragmented habitats (Keuper et al. 1986) may indicate that position relative to conspecifics is at least as important as position relative to the patch geometry (i.e., avoiding the edges). The finding that also females exhibit space partitioning, may be more consistent with the predation hypothesis than the lek hypothesis. However, Pie (1988) observed contests between females in the lekking fly Setellia sp., but they were less ritualized than male-male 12 Condition and spatial distribution contests, which in most cases were presentations rather than physical fights. Females may compete simply to have fast access to any male (which assumes that limited by access to fresh sperm), or they may compete for the best males. The former appears less likely; e.g., Reinhardt et al. (1999) found that although females of the grasshopper Chorthippus parallelus mate every 4.5 days if they have access to males, the fertility of females that were allowed to mate only once did not decline over a period of about a month. The latter In the wart-biter, there is pronounced sexual selection on the postcopulatory level. Wedell (1991) found that sperm competes numerically and not by last-male precedence, as is common in insects with multiple-mating females (Davies 1991), and that the size of the spermatophylax influences sperm competition by determining how much sperm that enters the female. There is therefore ample potential for Fisherian selection for preferring males that offers large spermatophylaxes. For instance, in the field cricket Gryllus bimaculatus, sons of males that were chosen by females obtained double as many matings as sons of males that were not chosen (Wedell and Tregenza 1998). The finding that females seem to move inwards form the edges at mating time does not yield support to either of the hypotheses; they may be attracted by the best singers, facilitating quick mate choice, or decreasing predation risk during mating. Why was there no space partitioning in the large patches? The cause may be the size of the patch, or the size of the population, or a combination. The size of the patch is the most plausible explanation. First, the proportion of interior areas is higher in larger patches, which means less individuals per interior area. For males, this effect may be exaggerated by their tendency to clump, at the same time as ranges did not increase from medium to large patches. Thus, there may be more available space where poor-quality males can use and still keep an acceptable distance to high-quality males. However, population size per se influence the mating success of high-ranking males relative to low-ranking males (Widemo and Owens 1995) as well as the females' success in finding suitable males (Kokko 1997). Consequently, behavioural strategies (including movement) may change with popualtion size. Of course, we cannot tease the effects of population size and patch area in this study, since they in principle are perfectly correlated. Regardlessof the ultimate cause for space partitioning, the observed effect of patch/population size on wart-biter behaviour is interesting in the light of the excessive fragmentation of the wart-biter's natural habitat. A few decades ago, this species was common in Norwegian semi-natural, unfertilized meadows, commonly with areas of several thousand 13 Paper III m2. At present, there are almost no such habitats left (due to fertilization, forest plantation, etc.). The wart-biter still appears to be fairly common, but almost all populations are in roadsides and other very small remnant habitats (usually <1000 m2; Hjermann and Ims 1996). The present paper indicates that such a shift in average habitat area may have other and more subtile behavioural effects in addition to the well-known effects of on genetic and population dynamic processes. Such behavioural effects of habitat size and form may have unexpected consequences on the viability of populations, e.g. by altering mate choice, mating skew and the effective population size. Reference List Ander, K. (1947). Flygförmågan hos våra hopprätvinger [Flight ability of our Orthoptera]. Fauna Och Flora 42: 210-221. Arak, A. and Eiríksson, T. (1992). Choice of singing sites by male bushcrickets (Tettigonia viridissima) in relation to signal propagation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30: 365-372. Bradbury, J. W. (1985). Contrasts between insects and vertebrates in the evolution of male display. Fortschritte in Zoologie 31: 273-289. Brittingham, M.C. and Temple, S.A. (1983). Have cowbirds caused forest songbirds to decline? BioScience 33: 31-35. Campbell, D. J. and Clark, D. J. (1971). Nearest neighbour tests of significance for nonrandomness in the spatial distribution of singing crickets (Teleogryllus commodus (Walker)). Animal Behaviour 19: 750-756. Cherrill, A.J. and Brown, V.K. (1990). The habitat requirements of adults of the wart-biter Decticus verrucivorus (L.) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) in southern England (UK). Biological Conservation 53: 145-158. Cherrill, A.J., Shaughnessy, J., and Brown, V.K. (1991). Oviposition behavior of the bushcricket Decticus verrucivorus (L.) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Entomologist 110: 37-42. Davies, N. B. (1991). Mating systems. In: J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies (eds.): Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell, Oxford. Farris, H.E., Forrest, T.G., and Hoy, R.R. (1997). The effects of calling song spacing and intensity on the attraction of flying crickets (Orthoptera: Goryllidae: Nemobiinae). Journal Of Insect Behavior 10: 639-653. 14 Condition and spatial distribution Gundersen, H. and Andreassen, H.P. (2000). Space use and diel activity of root voles: Individual attributes and fitness consequences. In: Andreassen, H.P.: Movement ecology of root voles in patchy habitats. Ph. D. thesis, University of Oslo, Oslo. Hovi, M., Alatalo, R.V., and Rintamaki, P.T. (1996). Habitat differences and variability in the lek mating system of black grouse. Behaviour 133: 561-578. Hovland, N., Andreassen, H.P. and Ims, R.A. (1999). Foraging behaviour of the root vole Microtus oeconomus in fragmented habitats. Oecologia 121: 236-244. Höglund, J. and Alatalo, R.V. (1995). Leks. Princeton Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Ingrisch, S. and Boekholt, I. (1982). Zur Wahl des Eiablageplatzes durch mitteleuropäische Saltatoria. Zoologische Beiträge 28: 33-46. Johnson, A.R., Milne, B.T., and Wiens, J.A. (1992). Diffusion in fractal landscapes: Simulations and experimental studies of tenebrionid beetle movements. Ecology 73: 19681983. Kalmring, K., Keuper, A., and Kaiser, W. (1990). Aspects of acoustic and vibratory communication in seven European bushcrickets. In: W. J. Bailey and D. C. F. Rentz (eds.): The Tettigoniidae: Biology, Systematics and Evolution. Crawford House, Bathurst, Australia. Keuper, A., Kalmring, K., Schatral, A., Latimer, W., and Kaiser, W. (1986). Behavioural adaptations of ground living bushcrickets to the properties of sound propagation in low grassland. Oecologia 70: 414-422. Kokko, H. (1997). The lekking game: Can female choice explain aggregated male displays? Journal of Theoretical Biology 187: 57-64. Major, R.E., Christie, F.J., Gowing, G., and Ivison, T.J. (1999a). Age structure and density of red-capped robin populations vary with habitat size and shape. Journal of Applied Ecology 36: 901-908. Major, R.E., Smith, D., Cassis, G., Gray, M., and Colgan, D.J. (1999b). Are roadside strips important reservoirs of invertebrate diversity? A comparison of the ant and beetle faunas of roadside strips and large remnant woodlands. Australian Journal of Zoology 47: 611-624. Manly, B.F. (1974). A model for certain types of of selection experiments. Biometrics 30: 281-294. Pardo, M.C., López-León, M.D., Hewitt, G.M., and Camacho, J.P. (1995). Female fitness is increased by frequent mating in grasshoppers. Heredity 74: 654-660. 15 Paper III Pie, M.R. (1998). Lek behavior as the mating strategy of Setellia sp (Diptera: Richardiidae). Journal Of Insect Behavior 11: 823-832. Reinhardt, K. and Kohler, G. (1999). Costs and benefits of mating in the grasshopper Chorthippus parallelus (Caelifera: Acrididae). Journal Of Insect Behavior 12: 283-293. Reinhardt, K., Kohler, G., and Schumacher, J. (1999). Females of the grasshopper Chorthippus parallelus (Zett.) do not remate for fresh sperm. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 266: 2003-2009. Schatral, A., Latimer, W., and Broughton, B. (1984). Spatial dispersion and agonistic contacts of male bush-crickets in the biotope. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 65: 201-214. Snedden, W.A. and Greenfield, M.D. (1998). Females prefer leading males: relative call timing and sexual selection in katydid choruses. Animal Behaviour 56: 1091-1098. Wedell, N. (1992). Protandry and mate assessment in the wartbiter Decticus verrucivorus (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 31: 301-308. Wedell, N. (1993a). Evolution of Nuptial Gifts in Bushcrickets. Ph. D. thesis, University of Stockholm, Stockholm. Wedell, N. (1993b). Mating effort or paternal investment - incorporation rate and cost of male donations in the wartbiter. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32: 239-246. Wedell, N. and Arak, A. (1989). The wartbiter spermatophore and its effect on female reproductive output (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae, Decticus verrucivorus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 24: 117-125. Wedell, N. and Tregenza, T. (1999). Successful fathers sire successful sons. Evolution 53: 620-625. Weidemann, S., Stiedl, O., and Kalmring, K. (1990). Distribution and population density of the bushcricket Decticus verrucivorus in a damp-meadow biotope. Oecologia 82: 369-373. Widemo, F. and Owens, I.P.F. (1995). Lek size, male mating skew and the evolution of lekking. Nature 373: 148-151. 16 Condition and spatial distribution Table 1. Tests of effects of sex, rearing (lab-bred or adult-caught) and patch size on the use of edgezones, measured by the overall habitat use and by habitat selection within days. Single factors were tested using one-way anova, while the tests for interaction effects are type III tests from two-way anovas (tests of the main effects are not shown). Overall habitat use Selection within days Effect d.f. F P d.f. F P Sex 1,59 0.01 0.97 1,53 0.01 0.91 Sex x Rearing 1,57 0.03 0.87 1,51 0.25 0.62 Sex x Patch size 2,55 1.41 0.25 2,49 1.12 0.33 Patch size x Rearing 2,55 3.59 0.034 2, 49 3.84 0.028 Patch size (among lab-reared animals) 2,40 6.36 0.004 2, 37 6.68 0.003 Patch size (among animals caught as adults) 2,15 0.76 0.49 2, 12 0.97 0.41 17 Paper III Fig. 2. Four representative examples (one for each "type" of animal) of animal's movements within a medium patch (4x8 m). Some dates are marked to give an impression of the temporal scale. The border between the edge zone and the interior is marked with a broken line. The edge selection index SIedge for each individual is note expected to be 0.5 when the edge zone and the interior is used equally much. (Note that this index does not reflect the area use of the A5 female very well, parlty because observations outside the patch is ignored.) Fig. 3. Proportion of observations where males sang (on the left axis) and where females laid eggs (right axis) during the day. Fig. 4. Average edge selection index for poor- and good-condition animals on patches of three sizes (1, 2 and 3 denote small, medium and large; the large patches were double the size of the medium). The indicated standard errors (1SE) were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. Fig. 5. Each individual's average distance to the patch center during the day, based on the medium patches only. One standard error is indicated; standard errors were calculated using each wart-biter observation as one statistical observation. Fig. 6. Area of the wart-biter ranges, calculated as explained in the text. There were no significant effect of neither sex nor the condition treatment, but a significant effect of patch area (F 2,53 = 6.26, P=0.004). The indicated standard errors were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. 18 Condition and spatial distribution F e m a le s R ange are a M a le s 16 F e m a le s M a le s Are a (sq. m ) 12 8 4 0 0 ,5 S m a ll 1 ,5 M e d iu m 2 ,5 L a rg e 3 ,5 P a tc h si z e Fig. 6. Area of the wart-biter ranges, calculated as explained in the text. There were no significant effect of neither sex nor the condition treatment, but a significant effect of patch area (F 2,53 = 6.26, P=0.004). The indicated standard errors were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. Fig. 4. Average edge selection index for poor- and good-condition animals on patches of three sizes (1, 2 and 3 denote small, medium and large; the large patches were double the size of the medium). The indicated standard errors (1SE) were calculated using each individual (not each wart-biter observation) as one statistical observation. 19 Paper III P o o r- c o nd it io n fe ma le s 4 Dis ta n c e to p a tc h c e n te r Go o d - c o nd it io n fe ma le s P o o r- c o nd it io n ma le s 3,5 Go o d - c o nd it io n ma le s 3 2,5 2 1,5 - 1000 1000 - 1330 - 1330 1500 1500 - Tim e o f d a y Fig. 5. Each individual's average distance to the patch center during the day, based on the medium patches only. One standard error is indicated; standard errors were calculated using each wart-biter observation as one statistical observation. 20