G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs IMPLICIT CAUSALITY IN VERBS: THE ROLE OF COGNITIVE PROGRAMS Guido Peeters Catholic University of Leuven Center for Social and Cultural Psychology (CSCP) Technical Report August 2011 Abstract. Some students of causality in verbs have concluded that ES (ExperiencerStimulus) verbs such as "liking" and "admiring" involve perceived causality in the object (e.g., Rudolph & Försterling, 1994).. However, students of person perception have observed that personality inferences from ES verbs are attributed to the subject and regarded as causal dispositions accounting for the verbs. In this respect Semin & Marsman (1994) have distinguished between instigating causes (in the object) and dispositions (in the subject). In the present study, both types of perceived causality are shown to reflect the operation of two clearly distinct cognitive programs for the processing of relational information including interpersonal verbs. Experimental evidence is derived from previous research completed with a new study showing that inferences regarding personality and instigating causes differ in a way consistent with the operation of distinct cognitive programs. 1 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs "Mary likes Joan because of the kind of person she is." Brown & Fish (1983) observed that when people are presented with similar sentences and asked who is referred to by "she", most of them answer "Joan" rather than "Mary." Apparently, given the sentence: "Subject likes Object", people are inclined to localize the cause of the "liking" in the object rather than the subject. Other verbs, however, elicit causality localized in the subject. For instance, presented with the sentence: "Mary dominates Joan because of the kind of person she is", people tend to identify "she" with "Mary." This phenomenon has been called "implicit causality in verbs." Subsequent research has involved the establishment of models such as the "Linguistic Category Model" (Semin & Fiedler, 1991) and the related "Action State differentiation Model" (Rudolph & Försterling, 1997). For instance, the latter model distinguishes four relevant verb categories based on semantic roles: (1) ES (Experiencer Stimulus) verbs. Examples are: "A likes B" and "A admires B." The object (B) is perceived as the stimulus that elicits the experience (liking) of the subject (A). Hence causality is localized in the object (B). This verb category corresponds to the State Verbs (SV) of the Linguistic Category Model. (2) SE (Stimulus Experiencer) verbs. For instance: "A impresses B." The subject (A) is the stimulus that produces the experience (being impressed) in the object (B). This verb category corresponds to the State Action Verbs (SAV) of the Linguistic Category Model. (3) AP (Agent Patient) verbs. Examples are: "A (agent) kicks B (patient)" and "A (agent) helps B (patient)." Causality is attributed to the agentic subject (A). (4) AE (Agent Evocator) verbs. For instance: A (agent) punishes B (evocator of the punishment). Causality is attributed to the object (B). AP verbs and AE verbs are "Action Verbs". Also the Linguistic Category Model considers "Action Verbs" (AV) but they are subdivided differently into "Interpretative Action Verbs" (IAV, e.g.: A helps B) and "Descriptive Action Verbs" (DAV, e.g.: A kicks B). However, this discrepancy between models is ignored in the present study that deals exclusively with the first of the above categories: ES verbs (or SV "State Verbs") such as liking and admiring. As already mentioned, ES verbs elicit attribution of causality in the object. Given that A "likes" 2 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs or "admires" B because of the kind of person he is, perceivers associate 'he" with B assuming that B has done something good or admirable that triggers positive feelings in A. Referring to evidence of that sort, Rudolph & Försterling (1997) developed an explanation in terms of the classic covariation principle of causal attribution (Kelley, 1967). However, that interpretation was problematic. Semin & Marsman (1994) observed that ES verbs could elicit causal attributions to the subject/experiencer as well. Those attributions concern predominantly the personality of the subject as a cause of the state communicated by the verb (for a review and additional evidence, see: Hoorens, Maier & Maris, 2011). Beyond the field of causality in verbs, the subject-verb-object approach to social cognition (Gollob, 1974; see also Wyer, 1974) and impression formation research on the role of relational stimulus information (Peeters, 1983, 2010) have yielded overwhelming evidence that, for instance, sentences such as "A likes B" elicit inferences of likable personality traits in the subject A (Peeters, 1983, 2010). Meanwhile, it has been shown that also the attributions to the subject fit the covariation principle if it is taken into account that perceivers go beyond the information given consistent with well-established cognitive schemata and related propensities such as balance repair (Hoorens, et al., 2011; Peeters, 2010; Rudolph & von Hecker, 2006). To summarize, evidence on classic verb causality effects seems inconsistent with evidence on trait inferences from verbs and interpersonal relations. Specifically, trait inferences from verbs seem to affect sentence subjects more than sentence objects, even when verbs are used that in classic verb causality studies yield strong causality inferences in the object. In search for an explanation, Semin & Marsman (1994) suggested that two sorts of perceived causality are involved. The classic verb causality effect would be limited to inferences of immediate event instigating causes and would not extend to deep lasting dispositional properties such as personality traits. In the same way, an apple falling from a tree involves immediate instigating causes such as a gust of wind, and lasting dispositional causes such as gravity that, like personality, is a deep or distal property, a conceptual matter that is not directly observable. The idea that traits inferred from verbs are perceived as causal factors has not only face validity. It has been supported by experimental evidence showing that personality inferences from verbs are drawn in a way as if participants would be instructed to handle personality as the cause of the states communicated by the verbs (Peeters, 1976). More indirect evidence may be found in some ancillary effects reported by Hoorens et al. (2011, Study 1 and 2). Participants were asked to draw subject inferences and object inferences (target conditions). Two "inference conditions" were manipulated between participants: some participants were 3 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs asked to rate how much the verb would "say about the personality" of the subject/object (personality inference), and other participants were asked to rate how much what the verb communicated would be "caused by" the subject/object (causality inference). The experiment (study 1) was replicated (study 2) and both studies yielded a main effect of inference indicating that causality inference was rated higher than personality inference. This could mean that the when making causality ratings participants considered both personality traits and instigating causes as causal factors that, added together, scored higher than personality alone. The main effect of inference was qualified by an interaction with target represented in fig.1 of Hoorens et al.'s paper. It shows that personality inference ratings regarding the object are prominently lower than personality ratings regarding the subject and also lower than causality ratings regarding both the subject and the object. This pattern can be interpreted as the combined effect of two tendencies established in the literature reviewed above: (1) a tendency to deal with personality as a causal factor in the subject rather than in the object, and (2) a tendency to deal with instigating cause as a causal factor in the object rather than in the subject. The highest score was obtained for causality attributions to the subject. However the score for personality attributions to the subject was almost as high, which indicates that participants may have conceived causality in the subject as a matter of the subject's personality. The negligibly small difference between causality and personality attributions might either be neglected or be accepted as a possible indicator of some minimal attribution of instigating causality to the subject as well. The lowest score was obtained for personality attributions to the object. However, causality attributions to the object scored much higher, which suggests that causality attributed to the object may not be limited to personality but includes instigating causes that boosted the score. Provided that causal inferences from verbs involve two sorts of causalities--dispositions and instigating causes-- it may surprise that it took so long before researchers arrived at dealing with both together, as if researchers dealing with one type were blind for the other. A possible explanation may be that the two sorts of causality are processed, by what we may call, different cognitive programs. There is dramatic anecdotic evidence that even experienced researchers (Robert Wyer and myself) who were fully aware of the distinct cognitive programs, ran into a failed experiment because they did not realize that participants would interpret the instructions using a different cognitive program than the program they had in mind (Peeters & Wyer, 1985). The cognitive programs in question are the SO-program (SelfOther program) and the 3P-program (Third-Person program). They have been derived from linguistic and related cognitive universals (Peeters, 1983) and they have been shown to 4 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs account for a wide variety of psychological and even philosophical observations. As an extensive review is available elsewhere (Peeters, 2004, and Peeters & Hendricks, 2007), I shall restrict to a striking example (Peeters, 1987) that clearly illustrates the selective use of the cognitive SO- and 3P-programs. A scene from the Benny Hill show goes as follows. At a reception, Benny and his friend are offered a piece of cake. Actually two pieces are left: a small one and a big one. The friend politely lets Benny go first and Benny takes a piece: the bigger one. The friend is upset and yells: "If you had let me go first, I would have taken the smaller piece!" Whereupon Benny puts on an aggrieved expression and says: "You got it!" The pun of the joke is based on a switch between cognitive programs. Benny's friend, and most of us who find the scene funny, use the SO-program. For instance, when asked why Benny's friend would have taken the smaller piece, the common answer would be: "Because one should take the smaller piece for the self and leave the bigger piece to the other out of politeness" However, Benny suddenly makes us aware of the alternative 3P-program that ignores the distinction between self and other looking exclusively at "Who does what to whom". Benny leaves the small piece to him labelled "the friend", and "the friend" says that he would have taken it anyway. So what? There is strong evidence that perceivers are biased to process personality inferences in the way of Benny's friend, which means: consistent with the SO-program (Peeters, 1983, 1991, 2011). In the same way as the audience of the Benny Hill show did not realize Benny's 3Pshaped view as an alternative of the common SO-shaped view of his friend, students of personality inferences may not have realized the role of instigating causes because information concerning personality would be processed consistent with the SO-program and instigating causality consistent with the 3P-program. The other way around, students of causality in verbs may have been blind for the role of personality as a causal factor because they were locked in by the 3P-program. The aim of the present paper is to test the validity of that explanation by demonstrating that personality inferences from ES verbs are processed consistent with the SO-program while inferences of an instigating cause consistent with the 3P-program. Thereby I was fortunate to find serendipitous evidence in a former paper (Peeters, 1991, experiment 1) of which an extended and updated version has been made available recently (Peeters, 2011, experiment 1). In the following I review the old serendipitous evidence and then report a more recent experiment that was explicitly designed to test the hypothesis that deep personality inferences tend to be processed consistent with the SO-program, and inferences about event instigating causes consistent with the 3P-program. 5 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs . Serendipitous Evidence: The 1991 Study The Benny Hill scenario gives scope for highlighting the role of the 3P-program in the processing of instigating causes. Let us return to Benny and his friend at the reception and assume that the friend grows rigid with fear when he notices the hostess approaching with her self-baked cakes that he detests. He knows the hostess is quick to take offence of any sign of depreciation regarding her culinary achievements. Hence he feels obliged to take a piece. Fortunately, there's Benny who likes the cake and knows about his friend's distaste. So he decides to relieve his friend's pain by taking the big piece. The friend has a sigh of relief and as soon as the hostess has disappeared from view, he exclaims: "If you had let me go first, I would have taken the smaller piece!" Whereupon Benny winks at his friend and whispers: "You got it!" This version of the story may be a pleasant anecdote but not exactly our idea of great fun worth to be performed as a farce. The reason is that the surprising effect of the switch from SO- to 3P-program is absent because additional information has been provided that sets the audience for the 3P-program from the beginning on. When asked why Benny's friend would have taken the smaller piece, the common answer may not be "Because he wants to be polite" but "Because he dislikes the cake and knows that Benny does not care about getting the bigger piece." Notice that the conceptual distinction between self and other is no longer required as it is for the right understanding of particular rules of etiquette. What matters is not that the big piece goes to the other and the small piece to the self, or vice versa, but that the big piece goes to him or her who likes the cake and small piece to him or her who dislikes the cake. In the latter example, the sizes of the pieces of cake combined with the actors' tastes or preferences may be perceived as the instigating causes that bring about Benny's choice of the larger piece and his friend's remark that he would have taken the smaller piece. And these instigating causes are handled by the 3P-program. Beyond the present thought experiment, more convincing evidence may be found in the results of a full-fledged experiment that was designed to investigate possible conditions that would make perceivers to switch from the SOprogram to the 3P-program (Peeters, 1991, experiment 1, resumed in Peeters, 2011). It is henceforth referred to as the 1991 Study. In the 1991 study, cognitive programs were diagnosed using the method of the relation pattern model. Participants were presented with items describing how two target persons (A 6 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs and B) were relating to each other and to themselves. Each description involved four relations: (1) AB or how A related to B, (2) BA or how B related to A, (3) AA or how A related to him/her-self, and (4) BB or how B related to him/her-self. Each relation could be either positive or negative, which enabled for 16 (two raised to the fourth power) descriptions. Participants were asked to provide similarity ratings of A and B across the 16 descriptions. The 16 similarity ratings of a participant enabled to compute the exact amounts of rating variance that were consistent with particular cognitive programs such as the above SO- and 3P-programs. The following example offers a simplified demonstration of the suitability of the AB similarity ratings to discriminate between cognitive programs. Let us consider the following descriptions of items: (1) A and B like each other and also themselves (2) A and B dislike each other but they like themselves (3) A and B like each other but they dislike themselves In description (1) all four of the relations (AB, BA, AA and BB) are positive. According to the SO-program, this implies high similarity of A and B. Indeed both A and B are similar in that both like the other person and both like the self. However, also according to the 3Pprogram, A and B are alike in that both like the same people and both are liked by the same people. Indeed both like (and are liked by) A, and both like (and are liked by) B. Hence, high AB similarity ratings are expected, both the SO- and the 3P-programs predicting high similarity, and high similarity has effectively been found in experimental studies. Description (2) is composed of two negative relations (AB and BA), and two positive relations (AA and BB). As in description (1), the information provided about A is similar to that provided about B according to the SO-program: both A and B dislike the other and like the self. However, according to the 3P-program the information provided about A is the complete opposite of the information provided about B. The information about A tells that he or she likes A and dislikes B; however the information about B tells that he or she dislikes A and likes B. Thus the one likes a person the other dislikes, and vice versa. Hence participants who rate A and B as more different from each other in description (2) than in description (1) can be assumed to interpret the descriptions using the 3P-program. It could be objected that 7 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs the difference attributed to A and B in description (2) may not be due to the use of the 3Pprogram. It may just follow from a simple rule of thumb associating liking with similarity and disliking with dissimilarity, as suggested by the proverb: "Birds of a feather flock together." The application of that proverb might be considered as a third possible cognitive program accounting for inferences of similarities and dissimilarities from relations between A and B. Similar alternative cognitive programs could be checked for by obtaining similarity ratings for additional relational patterns such as the one implemented by description (3). If participants apply the rule "birds of a feather flock together", then they should attribute more similarity to A and B in description (3) than in description (2). However, if they use the 3P-program they should rate A and B in description (3) as different from each other as in description (2), and, of course, less different from each other in description (1) than in descriptions (2) and (3). Using the above method it was generally found that personality inferences from liking relations were drawn almost exclusively consistent with the SO-program. A small amount of variance was accounted for by the birds-of- a-feather-flock-together rule, but the 3P-program seemed switched off completely. Participants assigned high similarity to the personalities of A and B in descriptions (1) and (3), and some less similarity in description (2). The same outcomes were obtained when participants were not asked to compare personalities of A and B, but attitudes of A and B. For instance, being asked to provide similarity ratings of A and B's "tastes and preferences", the dominance of the SO-program persisted. It persisted also for similarity ratings of A and B's personalities in a condition where A and B were presented as musicians and the liking and disliking relations were specified as evaluations of the music they produced. Thus in description (1bis) A and B were presented as musicians who liked their own music and each other's music as well. In description (2bis) they were presented as musicians who liked their own music but disliked each other's music. In description (3bis) they were said not to like their own music but to like each other's music, and so forth. Similarity ratings of A and B's personalities yielded once more SO-program dominated outcomes. However a dramatic shifts towards the 3P-program was observed for the similarity ratings of A and B's tastes and preferences. For instance, in description (3bis) A and B were assumed to have contrasting tastes and preferences. For instance, A might feel sick of the light traditional compositions that are the best he can, and admire the sophisticated avant-garde compositions of B. At the same time B would feel disappointed with his technical avant-garde tours de force that lack the spontaneity he thinks to recognize in A's naive compositions. In the latter condition, A and B's musical compositions function as instigating causes eliciting A and B's liking. It's true that participants did not provide similarity ratings of A and 8 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs B's music. However they provided similarity ratings of A and B's musical "tastes and preferences", which may be regarded as correlates of A and B's music. If two people have different musical tastes and preferences, then they are assumed to like different music, and so be appealed to by different music. Apparently the 1991 experiment provides evidence that instigating causes are inferred consistent with the 3P-program, while dispositional "personality" inferences are drawn consistent with the SO-program. In addition, the interpretation of the results seems to reveal an aspect of instigating causality that may deserve some attention. It concerns the observation that instigating causes attributed to the object may involve correlates in the subject. For instance, if A likes B because of his music (instigating cause in the object), it is implied that B's music fits A's musical taste (correlate in the subject). If A likes B because B helps him out of his problems (instigating cause in the object), it is implied that A has problems (correlate in the subject). In this way, instigating causes in the object may imply a certain state of readiness in the subject that is complementary to the cause in the object. That state of readiness is part and parcel of the perceived causal instigation process associated with the verb. Hence presumed causality in the object may be a more complex phenomenon involving also a complementary causal factor in the subject such as a certain readiness. Anyway, this complication of the instigating cause in the object may be interesting for its own sake and deserve attention. At present it is only important to note that this complication does not detract from the finding that instigating causality is processed consistent with the cognitive 3P-program. It could be objected only that the present evidence is still very limited, only one verb pair (like/dislike) and one instigating cause (music) being investigated. Hence the following experiment was set up to have an additional check of the hypothesis that implicit causality in an ES verb (admiring) involves dispositional personality inferences processed consistent with the SO-program, and instigating causality consistent with the 3P-program. The New Study General Overview, Design, and Predictions Participants were asked to provide difference ratings of two target persons, A and B, who were said to admire each other's achievements. Two within-subjects conditions were realized 9 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs by manipulating information about the ways A and B were relating to the self (self-relation conditions). (a) In the positive self-relation condition A and B were said to admire their own achievements as well, which resulted in a description of A and B analogous to the above description (1), the only difference being that "admiring" was substituted for "liking." (b) In the negative self-relation condition A and B were said not to admire themselves, which resulted in a condition analogous to the above description (3). Two other within-subjects conditions concerned the nature of the AB differences to be rated, which was specified in a way as to provide a potential cause of the admiration.. (a) In the personality condition participants rated how different they expected A and B's personalities would be. (b) In the achievement condition participants rated how different they expected the nature of A and B's achievements would be, "achievement" being regarded as an instigating cause of admiration. Notice that the above self-relation and cause conditions were presented to all of the participants but counterbalanced across participants. Finally, the experiment was replicated across four groups of participants, which constituted four between-subjects "replication" conditions. Expecting that judgments about personality would be underlain by the SOprogram, and judgments about achievement by the 3P-program, it was predicted that in the negative self-relation condition more difference would be attributed to A and B than in the positive self-relation condition and that this effect would be obtained only, or at least most prominently, when attributed differences concern the nature of A and B's achievements rather than A and B's personalities. Method Participants. Four groups of Dutch-speaking psychology undergraduate students of the university of Leuven volunteered in different places and at different points of time to complete a questionnaire presented as part and parcel of a study on the informational value of verbs. One group consisted of first-year students who participated in an experimental questionnaire session as part of a course requirement. The three other groups consisted of 10 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs second-year psychology students who attended working sessions connected with an introductory course of social psychology. The number of participants amounted to 193 (36 men and 157 women). They were spread about equally over the four groups that were handled as four replications. Questionnaire. Participants were presented with a brief questionnaire (two pages) that was anonymous, only gender being asked for on the top of the first page. The questionnaire contained only four experimental questions, being four similarity-difference ratings of two target persons (A and B) presented in the four conditions of the 2 by 2 design that was obtained by combining the factors cause (personality vs. nature of achievement) and selfrelation (positive vs. negative). The presentation order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants, about half of the participants getting the positive self-relation condition before the negative self-relation condition, and about half of the participants rating personality before nature of achievement. The rating scales ranged from 0 (similar) to 8 (different), higher values indicating more difference. To make it all clear, an English translation of one questionnaire brochure is presented in APPENDIX. in which the positive self-relation condition precedes the negative self-relation condition, and the personality as cause condition precedes the nature of the achievement as cause condition. For the sake of clarity of the present text, names of conditions have been inserted between brackets in italics. Results and Discussion of the New Study The difference ratings were subjected to a 2(Presentation order of self-relation conditions: positive first vs. negative first) X 2(Presentation order of cause conditions: personality first vs. nature of achievement first) X 4 (Replications: 1, 2, 3, 4) X 2(Self-relation: positive vs. negative) X 2(Cause: personality vs. nature of achievement) MANOVA with self-relation and cause as within subjects variables. The hypothesis predicts an interaction effect of self-relation and cause: more perceived difference is expected between positive and negative self-relation conditions when the difference is about the nature of the achievements of A and B than when the difference is about the personalities of A and B. The predicted interaction is significant F(1,177) = 4.20 p < .05 MSE = 3.19. The mean difference ratings, presented in Table 1, show that the interaction is indeed as predicted. The difference ratings of A and B's personalities are nearly constant irrespective of whether the relations of A and B with the self are positive or negative 11 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs (p > .99 by Tukey HSD for unequal N) while all of the other differences between values displayed in Table 1 are significant (p < .01 by Tukey HSD for unequal N), and so is the greater difference attributed to the nature of A and B's achievements when A and B's relations with the self are negative than when their relations with the self are positive. Table 1. Mean difference ratings between A and B in the Cause and Self-relation conditions ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Relation with Self -------------------------------Positive Negative ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Personality 3.20 3.24 Nature of Achievement 4.62 5.19 Cause ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- In addition to the predicted effect, there is a significant main effect of cause, overall more difference being attributed to the nature of A and B's achievements (M = 3.22) than to their personalities (M = 4.91). Also when relations with the self are positive, achievements are expected to be more different than personalities, the difference between 3.20 and 4.62 in Table 1 being significant, as mentioned. Hence the baseline difference of achievement may be nearer to the psychological ceiling of perceived difference than the baseline difference of personality, which may have masked the greater expected difference in the negative selfrelation condition to some degree. This may explain why the effect of self-relation, though significant, is not as overwhelming as we might have wanted. It is worthwhile that the latter effect of cause is qualified by an interaction with order of self-relation conditions and order of cause conditions F(1,177) = 7.77 p < .01 MSE = 4.88. It means that presentation order produced effects, but those effects did not involve the predicted interaction effect of cause and self-relation. This argues for the robustness of the predicted effect, and so does the absence of any effect of the replication factor. For the rest, no more effects reached statistical significance. 12 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs General Discussion The results of the new experiment confirm the conclusions drawn from the 1991 study: causality in ES verbs, such as liking and admiring, is inferred through the SO-program if it involves personality, and it is inferred through the 3P-program if it involves instigating causes such as appealing music or the showing of impressive achievements. In this way, the present data add to the psychological contrast between dispositions and instigating causes stressed by Semin & Marsman (1994). In addition, they may explain why researchers who focused on instigating causality have overlooked the role of dispositional causality, and vice versa. Indeed, as mentioned in the introduction, perceivers tend to stick either to the SO-program or to the 3P-program, without mixing up both programs. It should be mentioned that neither the 1991 study, nor the new study, enable to determine whether the inferred causality is located in the subject or in the object. However, as it has been explained in the introductory section, there's convincing evidence that personality inferences are drawn to the subject. If A likes B, perceivers infer a disposition in A to like others in general, which is translated into so-called "other profitable" (Peeters, 1982) personality traits such as likableness, friendliness, tolerance, etc. In terms of Kelley's (1967) ANOVA covariation model of causal attribution, it means that personality as a causal factor in the subject is inferred consistent with the low consensus (combined with low distinctiveness) rule: A might be assumed to like others in general (low distinctiveness), even nasty others that are disliked by most people (low consensus). As to instigating causality in ES verbs, according to the current literature it belongs to the object. However, as explained in the section on the 1991 study, it may involve a component in the subject as well. If A likes B's music, the causality accounting for A's liking does not only involve B's music as an instigating cause, but also a certain susceptibility in A to the sort of music B produces. This means that, according to Kelley's ANOVA model, instigating causality would not only be a matter of high distinctiveness (not anybody's music--only B's-is liked) but also of low consensus (not everybody--only people with A's taste--like the music). It could be objected that in the music case, A's susceptibility can be defined as "musical taste," which is a well-established concept, indeed, but no such well-established concept may define susceptibility that sets for admiring a particular achievement. Everybody may be assumed to admire great achievements, in which case instigating causality may only be a matter of high distinctiveness (combined with high consensus), limiting causality to the 13 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs object (B) .However, the lack of a well-established concept defining a particular susceptibility does not exclude the existence of the susceptibility in question. If A admires B's soccer performances, it may be implied that A shows at least some interest in soccer. If instigating causality in ES verbs implies some correspondent susceptibility or readiness in the subject, one could wonder if that readiness should not be regarded as a dispositional property that is part and parcel of the subject's personality. In this respect we may distinguish between, at least, two sorts of dispositions: traits and attitudes. The specific readiness that makes a person susceptible to a specific instigating stimulus may be attitude-like rather than trait-like. So the question arises if attitudes should be regarded as part and parcel of personality. Obviously, the answer to that question depends on how personality is defined. Experts may debate on a definition, but what's at stake here is not the expert's explicit definition but the implicit definition that underlies the lay's social perception and judgment. Five revelatory experiments yielded evidence that the implicit personality concept is handled consistent with the SO-program (Peeters, 1991, and extended version in 2011). In the experiments no personality "traits" were involved. Participants were only questioned about A and B's "personalities" without any specification of personality attributes. Taking a purely logical perspective, the questions could be dealt with equally well using the 3P-program as using the SO-program. However, participants relied almost exclusively on the SO-program as if the simple word "personality" raised an insurmountable barrier preventing access to the 3Pprogram. Even hints intended to push them towards the 3P-program failed to open the participants' eyes for the possibility to draw personality inferences consistent with the 3Pprogram. Instead the hints created only confusion. Only when the experimenter demonstrated a simple line of reasoning for drawing personality inferences consistent with the 3P-program, participants were surprised that they had failed to discover that obvious line of reasoning in spite of the hints they got. Apparently, perceivers handle an SO-program bound implicit personality concept that, moreover, has been found consistent with a current two-dimensional model of implicit personality theory. Since Rosenberg's seminal study (Rosenberg, Nelson, & Vivekanathan, 1968), perceived personality traits have been found organized consistent with two dimensions (Abele, et al., 2008) that have showed up also in personality trait inferences based on the SO-program (Peeters, 1983). Considering that the same dimensions have been found underlying interpersonal language in non-Western cultures where personality trait labels are not in use (White, 1980), we may conclude that there is an universal implicit socialcognitive structure generated by the SO-program that in modern Western culture has been made explicit as a person's "character" or "personality." 14 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs Provided that there exists a universal SO-programmed implicit personality concept associated with the trait dimensions of implicit personality theory, we are still left with the question: What about attitudes? If the implicit personality concept does not only involve traits but also attitudes, then attitudes should be confined to the SO-program as well. Experiments involving inferences about concrete attitudes (Peeters, 1983) as well as about unspecified attitudinal concepts such as tastes and preferences in general (Peeters, 1991, resumed in 2011), showed that participants are primarily set to process attitudinal information following the SO-program, indeed, but switch readily to the 3P-program. For instance, in the above 1991 study, participants used the SO-program to draw inferences about tastes and preferences from liking relations, but switched readily to the 3P-program when the "liking" was specified as "liking of music." Hence, attitude seems a somewhat equivocal category that can be handled either as part and parcel of personality close to the essence of the person, or as a more superficial feature close to external physical properties. Instigating causality in verbs, then, may be concluded to belong to the latter, more superficial--more impersonal--realm of social cognitive content. REFERENCES Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A. J. C., Judd, C.M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y., Eds (2008). Fundamental dimensions of social judgment. Special Issue. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38-7, 1063-1224. Brown, R., & Fish, D. (1983). Are there universal schemas of psychological causality? Archives de Psychologie, 51, 145-153. Gollob, H. F. (1974). The subject-verb-object approach to social cognition. Psychological Review, 81, 286-321. Hoorens, V., Maier, J., & Maris, S. (2011). When words speal louder: The effect of verb abstraction on inferences from interpersonal events. Submitted manuscript, 63 pp. 15 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp.192-238). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Peeters, G. (1976). In search for schemata underlying the inference of traits from interpersonal relations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 191-205. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/128992/1/1976EJSP_Traits_From_Relations.pdf Peeters, G. (1983). Relational and informational patterns in social cognition. In W. Doise & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Current issues in European Social Psychology (pp. 201-237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/166030 Peeters, G. (1987). The Benny Hill Effect: Switching cognitive programmes underlying subjective estimations of the outcomes of bargains concerning distributions of rewards. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 465-481. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/128088/1/1987EJSP_BENNY_HILL_EF FECT.pdf Peeters, G. (2004). Thinking in the third person: A mark of expertness?. Psychologica Belgica, 44, 249-267. http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/acad/psyb/2004/00000044/00000004/art00002 http://docserver.ingentaconnect.com/deliver/connect/acad/00332879/v44n4/s2.pdf?expires=1 304507149&id=62582301&titleid=11972&accname=KU+LEUVEN+UNIVERSITY&checks um=94B91F2BF53AF0A70627308C37044B54 https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/113693/1/2004PsBELexpertness.pdf . Peeters, G. (2010). Causal or dispositional inferences from verbs/relations: Overview of some published and unpublished material relevant for the issue of implicit causality in verbs. Internal Report. K.U.Leuven, Center for Social and Cultural Psychology. 20 pp. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/269446 https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/269446/1/2010_Rep_CAUSAL+INFERENCE S+VERBS.doc 16 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs Peeters, G. (2011). Relational information processing and the implicit personality concept: An updated and extended draft. Internal Report. K.U.Leuven, Center for Social and Cultural Psychology, 35 pp. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/307610 https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/307610/1/2011_Rep_RELATIONAL+INFO+ PR.+EXTENDED.doc Peeters, G., & Wyer, R. (1985). The selective activation of cognitive programs in the processing of relational information: The role of subjective orientation, priming, and grammatical form. (Internal Report No 1). Leuven, Belgium: K.U.Leuven, Laboratorium voor Experimentele Sociale Psychologie. 53 pp. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/176052 https://lirias.kuleuven.be/bitstream/123456789/176052/1/1985REPORT_RPM_WYER.p df Peeters, G., & Wyer, R. (1985). The selective activation of cognitive programs in the processing of relational information: The role of subjective orientation, priming, and grammatical form. (Internal Report No 1). Leuven, Belgium: K.U.Leuven, Laboratorium voor Experimentele Sociale Psychologie. 53 pp. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/retrieve/17577/1985REPORT_RPM_WYER.pdf Rosenberg, S., Nelson, C., & Vivekanathan, P.S. (1968). A multidimensional approach to the structure of personality impressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 283294. Rudolph, U., & Försterling, F. (1997). The psychological causality implicit in verbs; A review. Psychological Bulletin, 121,192-218. Rudolph, U., & von Hecker (2006). Three principles of explanation: Verb schemas, balance, and imbalance repair. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 25, 377-405. Semin, G. R., & Fiedler, K. (1991). The linguistic category model, its bases, application and range. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.),European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 491508. 17 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs Semin, G.R., & Marsman, J.G. (1994). "Multiple inference-inviting properties" of interpersonal verbs: Event instigation, dispositional inference, and implicit causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 836-849. White, G.M. (1980). 'Conceptual Universals in Interpersonal Language'. American Anthropologist 82:759-781. Wyer, R. (1974). Cognitive organization and change: An information processing approach. New York: Wiley. APPENDIX Copy of a questionnaire (translated from Dutch) in which the positive self-relation condition precedes the negative self-relation condition, and the personality as cause condition precedes the nature of the achievement as cause condition. Note that names of conditions [added between brackets and in italics] were not present in the original questionnaire. --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------THE INFORMATIONAL VALUE OF VERBS Look at the following situation: [positive self-relation condition] A and B admire each other's achievements. They admire also their own achievements. We want to how that situation is best imagined. Hence we want to submit two questions [personality as cause condition] First, how could the personalities of A and B be imagined? One possibility is that A and B's personalities are similar. Another possibility is that the one's personality differs from the other's personality. Mark your answer on the scale below. If you see A and B as two people with very similar personalities, then you mark 0. If you think of them as different personalities, then you mark a higher number. You mark 8 if you think that the personality of the one is very different from the personality of the other. PERSONALITIES OF A AND B SIMILAR 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 DIFFERENT 18 G. Peeters / Causality in Verbs and Cognitive Programs [nature of achievement as cause] The second question concerns how the nature of the achievements of A and B are imagined. The nature of their achievements can be very similar. This is the case when both perform, for instance, as soccer players. Another possibility is that their performances concern very different activities. For instance, A may perform well as a soccer player, while B may not play soccer at all but do well as a student. NATURE OF A AND B's ACHIEVEMENTS SIMILAR 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 DIFFERENT Now still follows another set of similar questions, but now they concern a different situation involving two other people (called again A and B). Imagine the following situation: [negative self-relation condition] A and B admire each other's achievements, but they do not at all admire their own achievements. [personality as cause condition] How could the personalities of A and B best be imagined in that case? PERSONALITIES OF A AND B SIMILAR 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 DIFFERENT [nature of achievement as cause] And how could the nature of A and B's achievements best be imagined? NATURE OF A AND B's ACHIEVEMENTS SIMILAR 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 DIFFERENT -----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------The questionnaire ended with a note of thanks and information about where and how participants could obtain information about the study they had participated in. 19