Three-dimensional shape coding in grasping circuits: a comparison between the Anterior Intraparietal area and ventral premotor area F5a Tom Theys1, 2, Pierpaolo Pani1, Johannes van Loon2, Jan Goffin2, Peter Janssen1 Laboratorium voor Neuro- en Psychofysiologie1 and Afdeling Experimentele Neurochirurgie en Neuroanatomie2, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven Herestraat 49 B-3000 Leuven Belgium Corresponding author: Peter Janssen MD PhD Laboratorium voor Neuro- en Psychofysiologie, Leuven Medical School Herestraat 49, bus 1021 B-3000 Leuven Belgium Phone: +32 16 34 57 45 Fax: +32 16 34 59 93 Email: peter.janssen@med.kuleuven.be Abstract Depth information is necessary for adjusting the hand to the three-dimensional shape of an object in order to grasp it. The transformation of visual information into appropriate distal motor commands is critically dependent on the Anterior IntraParietal area (AIP) and the ventral premotor cortex (area F5), particularly the F5p sector. Recent studies have demonstrated that both AIP and the F5a sector of the ventral premotor cortex contain neurons that respond selectively to disparity-defined three-dimensional (3D) shape. To investigate the neural coding of 3D shape and the behavioral role of 3D-shape selective neurons in these two areas, we recorded single-cell activity in AIP and F5a during passive fixation of curved surfaces and during grasping of real-world objects.Similar to those in AIP, F5a neurons were either first- or second-order disparity selective, frequently showed selectivity for discrete approximations of smoothly-curved surfaces that contained disparity discontinuities, and exhibited mostly monotonic tuning for the degree of disparity variation. Furthermore, in both areas, 3D-shape selective neurons were co-localized with neurons that were active during grasping of real-world objects. Thus area AIP and F5a contain highly similar representations of 3D shape, which is consistent with the proposed transfer of object information from AIP to the motor system through the ventral premotor cortex. Introduction Binocular disparity based on differences in the horizontal positions of retinal images provides an important cue for three-dimensional (3D) object recognition and manipulation. Indeed, binocular disparity information is used for object manipulation while the hand is preshaping to adapt to the intrinsic 3D properties of an object (Watt & Bradshaw, 2003). The Anterior Intraparietal area (AIP) and the ventral premotor cortex are considered critical nodes in the visual control of grasping in non-human primates based on single-cell, inactivation and functional imaging results (Rizzolatti et al., 1988; Taira, Mine, Georgopoulos, Murata, & Sakata, 1990; Sakata & Kusunoki, 1992; Gallese, Murata, Kaseda, Niki, & Sakata, 1994; Jeannerod, Arbib, Rizzolatti, & Sakata, 1995; Sakata, Taira, Kusunoki, Murata, & Tanaka, 1997; Fogassi et al., 2001; Fluet, Baumann, & Scherberger, 2010; Nelissen & Vanduffel, 2011). Human imaging studies have reported grasp-related activations in the human AIP and the ventral premotor cortex (Faillenot, Toni, Decety, Gregoire, & Jeannerod, 1997; Binkofski et al., 1998; Jacobs, Danielmeier, & Frey, 2010). In line with early behavioral work on grasping (Jeannerod, 1981), a recent human fMRI study demonstrated activations in the human AIP and ventral premotor cortex elicited by intrinsic rather than extrinsic object properties (Cavina-Pratesi et al., 2010). We previously demonstrated that neurons in area AIP and area F5a, which is adjacent to F5p, provide a robust representation of 3D shape (Srivastava, Orban, De Maziere, & Janssen, 2009; Theys, Pani, van Loon, Goffin, & Janssen, 2012). The presence of 3D-shape selective neurons in F5a does not indicate how these neurons represent disparity-defined 3D shapes, i.e. which aspects of the stimuli are primarily encoded by the neurons. Previous studies have shown that the coding of 3D shape in area AIP is fast (i.e. short latency), metric (i.e. largely monotonic tuning for the degree of the disparity variation in curved surfaces) and coarse (i.e. selectivity is retained for discrete approximations (Srivastava et al., 2009)). In contrast, 3D shape coding in the inferior temporal cortex (ITC) is slower (longer latencies), more categorical, and highly sensitive to disparity discontinuities such as sharp edges and disparity steps (Janssen, Vogels, & Orban, 2000b). Similar coding differences between dorsal and ventral stream areas have been suggested by human fMRI studies (Preston, Li, Kourtzi, & Welchman, 2008). These differences in the neural representation of 3D shape between ITC and AIP are consistent with lesion studies in humans (Goodale & Milner, 1992), which have proposed a dichotomy in the primate visual system between the ventral stream supporting perception/object recognition and the dorsal stream supporting action (Dijkerman, Milner, & Carey, 1996; Marotta, Behrmann, & Goodale, 1997): the very detailed representation of 3D shape in ITC is suitable for object recognition and categorization, whereas the coarser representation of 3D shape in AIP is suitable for grasping. We have also recently shown that the 3D-shape representation in AIP is more boundary-based (i.e. weak influence of surface dots and robust selectivity for stimuli with disparity varying along the boundaries of the shape) compared to that in ITC (Theys, Srivastava, van Loon, Goffin, & Janssen, 2012). However, it is unknown whether the 3Dshape representation in F5a is more similar to the one in AIP or that in the ITC. Given that F5a is strongly connected to AIP but not directly with ITC (Matelli, Camarda, Glickstein, & Rizzolatti, 1986; Gerbella, Belmalih, Borra, Rozzi, & Luppino, 2011) we hypothesized that the neural coding of 3D shape in F5a should be more similar to that in AIP than in ITC. Recent studies have also begun to investigate the behavioral role of 3D-shape selective neurons in ITC and F5a. Consistent with the properties of individual ITC neurons (Janssen et al., 2000b), electrical microstimulation of ITC clusters during 3D-shape categorization exerts a profound and predictable influence on both perceptual choices and reaction times (Verhoef, Vogels, & Janssen, 2012). In the F5a sector of ventral premotor cortex, in contrast, most 3D- shape selective neurons were also active during visually-guided grasping of real-world objects (Theys et al., 2012), suggesting a close relationship between 3D-shape selectivity and visuomotor transformations for grasping. However, no data exist concerning the possible behavioral role of 3D-shape selective sites in AIP. First-order disparity coding has been demonstrated in areas V4 (Hinkle & Connor, 2002) and ITC (Janssen, Vogels, & Orban, 1999) in the ventral stream, as well as in areas MT/V5 (Nguyenkim & DeAngelis, 2003), CIP (Shikata, Tanaka, Nakamura, Taira, & Sakata, 1996; Taira, Tsutsui, Jiang, Yara, & Sakata, 2000; Katsuyama et al., 2010) and AIP (Srivastava et al., 2009) in the dorsal stream. Second-order disparity selectivity has been demonstrated in ITC (Janssen et al., 2000b) and AIP (Srivastava et al., 2009). The first goal of the present study was to investigate the neural representation of 3D shape in F5a to compare this representation with that of AIP. We tested whether F5a neurons respond to first-order (as in tilted planar surfaces) or second-order disparity (curved surfaces). We also tested the coarseness of the 3D-shape coding in F5a by comparing the selectivity for smoothly curved surfaces to that for discrete and linear approximations of these surfaces containing disparity discontinuities, and by examining the sensitivity of F5a neurons for small differences in the degree of the disparity variation. Finally, to complement our previous study of 3D-shape selectivity and grasping activity in F5a (Theys et al., 2012) we also recorded from 3D-shape selective sites in AIP during visually-guided grasping, and trained both animals in a perceptual 3D-shape categorization task, to be able to relate interindividual differences in the neural representations of 3D shape to behavioral differences in discrimination performance. To allow a direct comparison between F5a and area AIP, we recorded in both areas (in counterbalanced order) in the same animals using the same stimulus sets and tasks. We found that the 3D-shape representation in F5a was highly similar to that in AIP. Conversely, 3Dshape selective sites in AIP were also frequently active during object grasping, as in F5a. Thus, the parietofrontal grasping circuit contains two almost identical representations of the depth structure of objects that are both closely related to the visual guidance of the hand during grasping. Materials and Methods Subjects, surgery and recording procedure Two rhesus monkeys served as subjects for extracellular micro-electrode recordings. The surgical and recording procedures were identical to previous experiments in ITC and AIP (Janssen et al., 1999; Janssen, Vogels, & Orban, 2000a; Janssen et al., 2000b; Srivastava et al., 2009). Under isoflurane anesthesia, a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)compatible head fixation post and recording wells were implanted. Implantation of the recording chambers was centered on the inferior limb of the arcuate sulcus (area F5) and the lateral bank of the anterior intraparietal sulcus (AIP) stereotactically guided using preoperative MRI. All surgical techniques and veterinary care were performed in accordance with the NIH Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the local ethical committee of the KU Leuven. The animals were trained to perform a passive fixation task keeping the gaze of both eyes inside a 1-degree fixation window. After a 400-ms fixation period, the stimulus was presented at the fixation point for 600 ms, and if fixation had been maintained, a drop of juice was given as a reward. Stimuli were presented dichoptically using ferroelectric liquid crystal shutters (Displaytech) operating at a frequency of 60 Hz each and synchronized with the vertical retrace of the display monitor (VRG) operating at 120 Hz. As previously reported (Srivastava et al., 2009), no crosstalk was measured between the images presented to the two eyes. Horizontal and vertical eye movements were recorded using an infrared-based camera system sampling at 500 Hz (EyeLink II; SR Research). Stimuli and tests The basic stimulus set used in the search test was identical to the one used in previous experiments (Janssen et al., 2000b; Srivastava et al., 2009) and consisted of disparity-defined 3D shapes generated by combining a two-dimensional contour and a depth profile. We used thirty-two 3D shapes portraying curved surfaces, in which four disparity profiles were imposed over eight 2D shapes filled with a 50% density random-dot pattern (stimulus size: 5.5 deg). For all subsequent tests, stimuli with opposite curvatures were created by interchanging the monocular images between eyes (concave surfaces become convex and vice versa). All neurons were therefore tested with pairs of 3D shapes (concave-convex) composed of the same monocular images, but with right and left images interchanged. Recordings in area AIP and F5a were performed according to previous experiments (Srivastava et al., 2009; Verhoef, Vogels, & Janssen, 2010; Theys et al., 2012). In the search test - which was identical to that used in our previous studies - we presented the basic stimulus set consisting of 32 stimuli (eight 2D contours combined with 4 depth profiles, degree of the disparity variation, i.e. the disparity difference between the center and the edge of the shape: 0.65 deg) at the fixation point during passive fixation. On the basis of responses obtained in this test, the optimal 2D contour was selected for all subsequent tests. As in our previous studies, we first verified that the selectivity for 3D shape could not be accounted for by a selectivity for the monocular images per se (disparity test, see (Theys et al., 2012)): if the response difference between the two members of a pair of 3D shapes (composed of the same monocular images) was significant (t-test p < 0.05) and at least three times greater than the difference in the sum of the monocular responses (i.e. the difference in the summed responses to the monocular presentations of each of the two members of a pair of 3D shapes), the neuron was judged to be disparity selective. When stereo selectivity was present, we assessed that selectivity for higher-order disparity in the position-in-depth test (for further details, see (Janssen et al., 2000b)) in which concave and convex 3D stimuli were presented at five positions in depth, ranging from - 0.50° (near) to + 0.50° (far). If a neuron in area F5a was judged to be higher-order disparity-selective, the cell was further tested in the disparity order test to assess its selectivity for first- and second-order disparity using various approximations of the original 3D shapes. This stimulus set has been described in Janssen et al. (Janssen et al., 2000b) and Srivastava et al.(Srivastava et al., 2009). Briefly, the first-order stimulus approximations consisted of planar surfaces inclined in depth (linear variation in disparity over the vertical axis of the shape), to approximate either the top or the bottom part of the original three-dimensional shape (Fig. 1B). The linear approximations of the concave and convex 3D shape pairs were derived from the original 3D shapes as least-squares approximations of disparity profiles in the original surfaces, and consisted of two tilted planes (linear disparity variations) with a sharp disparity discontinuity in the center of the stimulus. For the inclined 3D shape pair (see example neuron in Figure 2) the linear approximation was a single inclined surface and therefore identical to the first-order approximation. We tested linear approximations only for the cosine and Gaussian depth profiles, as in Srivastava et al. (Srivastava et al., 2009). Three different discrete approximations were constructed by dividing the stimulus into three parts with a central region of varying size. These three parts were presented at three positions in depth (disparity amplitude, 0.65°). In the disparity sensitivity test, the optimal 2D contour was presented with two depth profiles (e.g. concave and convex) and six degrees of disparity variation (i.e. the disparity difference between the center and the edge of the shape) ranging from 1.3 to 0.03 deg. Because our goal was to compare the representation of 3D shape in F5a with that in AIP, all stimuli were presented in the center of the display at the fixation point and in the fixation plane. M2 was also used in the AIP study of Srivastava et al. (Srivastava et al., 2009). To allow direct comparison between AIP and F5a we also recorded from higher order disparityselective neurons in the sensitivity test in area AIP of monkey M1. To investigate the selectivity of F5a neurons for 3D shapes in which the disparity variation was confined to the boundary or the surface of the stimulus, we presented the same stimuli as in previous studies (Janssen, Vogels, Liu, & Orban, 2001; Theys et al., 2012). Different Gaussian depth profiles were created by varying the disparity along the surface of the stimulus, along the boundary, or along both surface and boundary (boundary-surface test). In the surface stimuli the depth profile consisted of a 2D-gaussian or a 13° x 13° square with the maximum disparity in the center of the shape smoothly approaching zero towards the boundaries. A subset of 3D-shape selective neurons was tested in a visually-guided grasping task (described in Theys et al (Theys et al., 2012)). In this task, the monkey was seated in the dark and had to place its right hand in a resting position for 500 ms, after which an LED was illuminated at the bottom of an object. Fixation upon the LED (keeping the gaze inside a 2.5degree fixation window throughout the trial until the object was lifted) for 500 ms was followed by the illumination of a 3D object. One of six different objects (a small cylinder, a small cube, a large cylinder, a large sphere, a large cube and a cylinder with groove) was pseudorandomly presented on a custom-built, vertically rotating carousel at a viewing distance of 28 cm. The dimensions (width, length and height) of the small and large objects were 15 and 35 mm, respectively. After a variable period of 500 to 1000 ms of object fixation, an auditory GO-cue instructed the monkey to reach, grasp, and lift the object. After a correctly completed trial, a juice reward was given. The resting position and the lifting phase were monitored by fiber-optic cables. Eye movements were recorded using an infrared- sensitive camera system (EyeLink II; SR Research). Eye position signals, neural activity, and photocell pulses were digitized and processed with a digital signal processor (DSP) at 20 kHz (C6000 series; Texas Instruments, Dallas, TX, USA). Recording procedure For extracellular recordings, tungsten micro-electrodes (FHC, MicroProbes) were inserted through a guide tube placed in a standard grid (Crist Instruments). Magnetic resonance imaging (resolution 0.6 mm isotropic) using glass capillaries filled with a 1% copper sulfate solution inserted into key grid positions confirmed correct positioning of the electrode in the depth of the posterior bank of the inferior limb of the arcuate sulcus (area F5a) and in the lateral bank of the anterior intraparietal sulcus (area AIP). For area F5a, we reconstructed the electrode penetrations based on the anatomical MRI using BrainVISA (http://brainvisa.info). Because of the inclination of the recording cylinder, we first traversed area 45B on the anterior bank of the inferior arcuate sulcus, then passed through the sulcus and finally entered the posterior bank of the inferior arcuate sulcus near the fundus. The patterns of active and silent zones during the recordings were highly consistent with the reconstructed electrode paths. The center of our recording area was located within the fMRI activation elicited by curved surfaces reported by Joly et al. (Joly, Vanduffel, & Orban, 2009). The recording positions in F5a were identical to the ones in our previous study (Theys et al., 2012); microstimulation in these recording positions did not elicit any overt behavioral response. The AIP recordings were performed in posterior AIP, in a region where Sakata et al. reported hand-manipulation neurons (Sakata, Taira, Murata, & Mine, 1995). Data analysis Matlab (Mathworks) was used for data analysis. Net neural responses were calculated by subtracting the mean activity in the 400 ms interval preceding stimulus onset from the mean activity between 50 and 450 ms after stimulus onset. In the position-in-depth test, neurons were considered responsive to the spatial variation of disparity (i.e. higher-order disparity selective) if the response to the non-preferred shape did not significantly (Student’s t-test) exceed any response to the preferred shape at any position in depth (Janssen et al., 2000b). In the disparity-order test, neurons were classified as first order if the selectivity for the first-order stimuli was not significantly smaller than the selectivity for the original smoothly curved surfaces, as evidenced by a nonsignificant interaction between threedimensional structure (concave-convex) and stimulus type (original vs first-order stimulus, ANOVA p > 0.05). Second-order neurons were significantly more selective for the smoothly curved surfaces compared to the first-order approximations (ANOVA p < 0.05). Discrete neurons showed a significant (t-test, p < 0.05) selectivity for at least one of the discrete approximations. Depending on the tuning for 3D shape as determined in the sensitivity test, neurons were classified as monotonic, broadband or tuned as in Janssen et al., (2000b). Monotonic neurons showed the strongest response to the largest disparity variation or a response statistically indistinguishable (t-test) from the response to the second-largest disparity variation, and a decline in response for smaller disparities. Broadband neurons responded equally well to all disparity variations (no significant effect of degree of disparity variation in a one-way ANOVA of the responses to the six disparity variations of the preferred 3D shape). If an optimal disparity variation was found, with a significant decrease in response on either side of this optimal magnitude, the neuron was classified as tuned. In the grasping task, neural activity (MUA) was aligned to the onset of the illumination above the object, to the time of the go-signal and to the time of the object lift. Results All F5a neurons reported here (N = 131) were higher-order disparity selective, i.e. showed significant selectivity in the disparity test (consisting of presentations of concave and convex surfaces and monocular presentations) that could not be accounted for by the monocular responses and preserved this selectivity across positions-in-depth. Binocular eye position traces recorded during the position-in-depth test showed only marginal deviations (averaging 0.1 degree of vergence) between the nearest and the farthest position in depth, much smaller than the range of disparities in the position-in-depth test (1 deg). Although visual responses could be recorded over an extended range in and around the arcuate sulcus, higher-order disparity selective neurons were concentrated in area F5a, in line with previous fMRI results (Joly et al., 2009). Disparity order test To determine whether F5a neurons encode first- or second-order disparities, we tested 71 F5a (M1: N = 35; M2: N = 36) neurons in the disparity-order test, in which the original 3D shapes were presented together with various approximations of these stimuli. The example neuron in Fig. 2A was highly selective for the inclined depth profile (first column), but was equally selective for a simple first-order approximation of the original 3D shape pair (second column, ANOVA with factors 3D profile and stimulus type, interaction ns). This example neuron was therefore encoding primarily first-order variations in disparity, as present in tilted planes. Furthermore, the discrete approximations (three rightmost columns) also elicited significant selectivity (t-tests on the net responses, p < 0.01 for all three approximations), indicating that the coding of 3D shape was relatively coarse. Across our population of F5a neurons, 55% (39/71; M1: N = 14/35; M2: N = 25/36) showed selectivity for first-order stimuli equal to or greater than that for second-order stimuli, and were therefore considered first-order neurons, in line with previous studies (Janssen et al., 2000b; Srivastava et al., 2009). The average responses in the disparity-order tests of these first-order neurons are illustrated in Fig. 2B. Clearly both the first-order and the discrete approximations evoked strongly selective responses in this population of first-order neurons (t-test on the net responses to the preferred and nonpreferred depth profiles, p < 0.01 for every approximation). However, we also observed strong second-order selectivity in F5a, as illustrated by the example neuron in Fig. 3A. This neuron was strongly selective for convex versus concave depth profiles and for their linear approximations, but not for the first-order approximations (rightmost column; ANOVA with factors 3D shape and stimulus type, interaction p < 0.05). Two out of three discrete approximations also elicited significant selectivity (t-test, p < 0.01), which again indicates that the representation of 3D shape in F5a is relatively coarse. Neurons showing significantly less selectivity for the first-order stimuli compared to the second-order stimuli (ANOVA, interaction between 3D profile and stimulus type p < 0.05) were deemed to be second-order disparity selective. In the average response of our population of second-order disparity selective F5a neurons (N = 32/71, 45%) (Fig. 3B), no selectivity was present for the first-order stimuli. As in the example neuron in Fig. 3A, two out of three discrete approximations yielded significant selectivity across the population (t-test p < 0.05), and 19 neurons (59%) showed selectivity for at least one of the discrete approximations. Interestingly, the average response to the preferred linear approximation differed significantly from the response to the preferred smoothly-curved 3D shape (t-test, p < 0.01; M1: p < 0.001; M2: p < 0.05), and 12 neurons discriminated reliably between the preferred smoothly-curved 3D shape and its linear approximation. Therefore the neural coding of 3D shape in F5a is, on the one hand, relatively coarse (in view of the frequent selectivity for discrete approximations), but at the same time sensitive enough to signal the small differences in 3D profile between the smoothly curved surface and its linear approximation, which constitutes a least-squares approximation of the smoothly curved surface. Note that we also encountered 17 neurons that were selective for the full-period sine depth profile, consisting of combined convex and concave profiles (see Fig. 2 in Theys et al.,(Theys et al., 2012)) and that therefore represents a third-order disparity stimulus. However we did not perform the disparity-order test for these neurons. To illustrate the responses and the neural selectivity for first- and second-order stimuli we plotted the average net responses and response differences for the original preferred 3D shape against the responses and response differences for the first-order stimuli (Fig. 4A), for the first-order and second-order neurons independently. Although second-order neurons frequently fired strongly to first-order stimuli (left panel in Fig. 4A), the response differences were much smaller for first-order stimuli than for second-order stimuli (right panel in Fig. 4A). Similarly, discrete approximations can evoke strong responses in second-order F5a neurons (left panel in Fig. 4B), but the degree of selectivity was weaker for these discrete approximations than for the smoothly curved 3D surfaces (right panel in Fig. 4B). Finally and in contrast to area AIP, a substantial proportion of F5a neurons (38%) discriminated reliably between a smoothly curved surface and its linear, least-square approximation (see data points below the diagonal in Fig. 4C). Overall, F5a neurons can encode zero-, first-, second- and possibly third-order disparities, similar to those in AIP and ITC. The neural coding of 3D shape in F5a shares many features with that in area AIP (e.g. selectivity for discrete approximations), but a subset of the neurons in F5a appear to be more sensitive to subtle differences in the depth profile compared to AIP neurons. Sensitivity to the degree of disparity variation To investigate the precision of the 3D-shape representation in area F5a, we measured the sensitivity of F5a neurons to differences in the degree of the disparity variation within the stimulus (an amplitude of 1.3 deg corresponded to a highly-curved surface, whereas an amplitude of 0.03 deg was almost flat). In the disparity sensitivity test, 131 neurons (M1: 61; M2: 70) were tested with disparity variations in the stimulus ranging from 1.3 to 0.03 deg. Fig. 5 shows the responses of three example neurons (positive numbers on the X-axis indicate the preferred depth profile). The most frequent response pattern in both monkeys (72%; M1: 79%, M2: 66%; z-test, ns; Table 1) was a monotonic profile, with the maximal response to the largest disparity variation and a monotonic decline in the response for smaller disparity variations (green line in Fig. 5). A small proportion of the neurons (8%; M1: 13%, M2: 4%; ztest, ns; Table 1) were significantly tuned to a particular disparity variation: the maximal response was observed for one of the smaller disparity variations, and this response differed significantly (t-test p < 0.05) from the response to the largest disparity variation (blue trace in Fig. 5). Finally 20% of the neurons (M1: 8%; M2: 30%; Table 1) were broadband (red trace in Fig. 5) since no significant difference was observed between responses in the preferred range of disparity variations (ANOVA, ns). The proportion of broadband neurons was significantly greater in M2 than in M1 (z-test, p < 0.05). However, the strongest decline in the response was more frequently (38% of the broadband neurons) seen within the nonpreferred range of disparity variations (as illustrated by the example neuron in Fig. 5) than at the change in the sign of the curvature (11%), in contrast to what has been reported in ITC (Janssen et al., 2000b). Although most F5a neurons showed a monotonic response pattern in the sensitivity test, a fraction of these neurons displayed significant selectivity for small differences in the depth profiles of the stimuli: more than 20 % of F5a neurons (28 % in M1 and 23 % in M2) were significantly selective for the smallest differences in the depth profile (+0.03 vs -0.03 deg, t-test p < 0.05) and 18 out of 131 neurons tested (15%, M1: 18%, M2: 11%) showed the largest response difference at the change in the sign of the disparity curvature (i.e. between the two smallest disparity variations, -0.03 vs +0.03 deg). Our population of 131 F5a neurons reliably discriminated between the two smallest disparity variations (paired t-test of the average responses to the +0.03 and -0.03 deg disparity variations, p < 0.0001 for both monkeys combined; M1: p < 0.001; M2: p = 0.12). Thus, F5a neurons can signal very small differences in the depth profiles of curved surfaces. The average normalized responses in the disparity sensitivity test are illustrated in Fig. 6A for both monkeys independently. In both monkeys, the F5a population showed a largely monotonic response pattern, but in M1 a more pronounced drop in the response was present between the two smallest disparity variations (t-test comparing the difference in the normalized response differences between the -0.03 and +0.03 deg disparity variations between the two monkeys, p < 0.001). To directly compare the representation of 3D shape in F5a with that in AIP, we also recorded the responses of 71 higher-order AIP neurons in the same animals (N = 38 in M1, N = 33 in M2). The average normalized responses of AIP neurons are illustrated in Fig. 6B. Overall the average response in AIP was highly similar to that of F5a. Furthermore, the proportions of monotonic, broadband and tuned neurons were highly similar in AIP and F5a in both animals: also, monotonic neurons predominated in AIP (M1: 74%; M2: 85%), whereas tuned (M1: 10%; M2: 9%) and broadband (M1: 16%; M2: 6%) neurons represented much smaller fractions of the neurons. In both monkeys the normalized difference between responses to the -0.03 and +0.03 deg disparity variations did not significantly differ in the two areas (t-test, M1: p = 0.46; M2: p = 0.82). Therefore the results of the sensitivity test indicate that the neural representation of 3D shape in F5a was highly similar to that in AIP. To determine whether the difference between the neural representations of 3D shape in our two monkeys was related to interindividual differences in the quality of stereoscopic vision, we trained both animals in a 3D-shape discrimination task (Verhoef et al., 2010; Verhoef, Vogels, & Janssen, 2011; Verhoef et al., 2012) In this task, either a convex or concave 3D surface (disparity varied along both the vertical and the horizontal axis, no disparity on the boundaries (Theys et al., 2012) was presented at the fixation point, and the animal was required to make an eye movement to the left when the stimulus was concave and to the right when the stimulus was convex. The disparity coherence was always 100%. After 6 training sessions, M1 reached a performance level of 82% correct, whereas M2 still performed at chance level (50% correct) after 12 training sessions. The chance performance of M2 was not due to an inability to learn the task rule, since this animal had learned a simple shape discrimination task (saccade to the left for a square and to the right for a triangle) in 5 sessions (92 % correct). Given the presence of large numbers of disparity-selective neurons in F5a and AIP (Srivastava et al., 2009) in M2, it is unlikely that this animal was stereoblind, but behavioral testing indicated that the quality of its stereoscopic perception was in all likelihood weaker than that of M1. Therefore the differences we observed between our two animals in the disparity sensitivity test were likely related to differences in stereoscopic perception. Selectivity for surfaces and boundaries in depth We previously demonstrated that 3D-shape selective AIP neurons encode both disparity variations along the boundary and along the surface of the shape, and that for the great majority of AIP neurons, boundaries in depth (lacking 3D surface information) are sufficient for evoking selectivity (Theys et al., 2012). We tested 18 higher-order F5a neurons with the same stimuli as in our previous study (Theys et al., 2012): concave and convex curved surfaces with a disparity variation on both the surface and the boundary of the shape (vertical 3D shape), on the boundary of the shape but not on the surface (silhouettes and outline stimuli), and on the surface but not on the boundary (restricted and large 3D surfaces). In a manner very similar to those of AIP, most (78%) F5a neurons were selective for at least one of the curved boundaries, whereas a smaller proportion (55%) was selective for the 3D surface stimuli (data not shown). Although the low number of neurons precludes a detailed comparison between AIP and F5a in terms of 3D boundary selectivity, the proportions of neurons in F5a were highly comparable to those previously reported for AIP (67% boundary neurons, 53% surface neurons). 3D-shape selectivity and grasping activity As a final comparison of 3D-shape selective sites in F5a and AIP, we recorded multiunit activity (MUA) in 3D-shape selective AIP sites during delayed visually-guided object grasping after assessing higher-order disparity selectivity in the position-in-depth test, as in our previous study of F5a (Theys et al., 2012). We found that the great majority of the 3Dshape selective sites (13/16, 81%) also responded during the fixation and grasping of realworld objects. Because our AIP recordings consisted of MUA, we cannot infer that the same AIP neurons were both 3D-shape selective and active during grasping. Furthermore we did not test whether 3D-shape selective AIP sites remained active during grasping in the dark (visuomotor activity), as in F5a. However, at the very least these data demonstrate that 3Dshape selectivity was co-localized with grasping activity in AIP, as it is in F5a(Theys et al., 2012). In Figure 7, the average population response during visually-guided grasping is plotted as a function of time for area AIP (top panel). For comparison we also plotted the average grasping-related activity of 98 higher-order disparity selective MUA sites in F5a (bottom panel). The visual response in the first 100 ms after light onset appeared stronger and faster in AIP in comparison to F5a (Fig. 7 left panels), but because the latency of the population response strongly depends on the number of recording sites, we cannot directly compare the two areas in this respect. At the moment of object lift ([-100, 200 ms] around object lift) the average activity in AIP declined and the average F5a activity became stronger than in AIP. A mixed-design ANOVA with brain area (AIP – F5a) and epoch ([-100, 200 ms] around object lift and [-500, -200 ms] before object lift) as independent factors revealed a significant interaction (F(1,108) = 8,18, p = 0.005). Bonferroni post-hoc analysis showed a significant difference (p < 0.002) between neural activity in the [-100, 200 ms] epoch around the lift and that in the epoch [-500, -200 ms] before the lift (respectively 39.9 ±8.3 spikes/sec vs 9.46 ±7.6 spikes/sec) for AIP, while no difference was observed between these epochs for F5a (24.3 (±3.3) vs 18.75 (±3.0)). Although the low number of AIP sites warrants a degree of caution in interpreting these results, they may suggest that 3D-shape selective AIP sites are most active during the visual analysis of the object, whereas 3D-shape selective F5a sites are more strongly active during the execution of the grasping movement. Future studies will have to determine to what extent 3D-shape selective AIP neurons remain active during grasping in the dark, i.e. exhibit visuomotor or motor-dominant activity. Discussion We investigated the coding of 3D shape selective neurons in area F5a and compared their properties with AIP neurons. We found that F5a neurons could be either first-order or second-order disparity selective. Furthermore, 3D shape coding in area F5a was fast, robust, largely metric and coarse, similar to area AIP. In both areas, 3D-shape selective neurons were embedded in clusters of neurons that also fired during grasping. The coding of 3D shape information in AIP and F5a is most likely important for translating 3D object properties into the appropriate motor commands for grasping. This study is the first detailed comparison between the object representation in AIP and F5a. It is remarkable that neurons in the ventral premotor cortex provide exceedingly detailed visual information about the 3D-structure of objects (e.g. the selectivity for very small disparity variations and the differences between smoothly curved surfaces and their linear approximation). Our data also demonstrate that at least some F5a neurons encode not only relative disparity (i.e. the disparity difference between the center and the edge of the shape) but also first- and second-order disparities (curvature). Hence the motor system has access to a robust visual 3D description of objects, even though this information may not necessarily determine the grip type. Murata et al. (Murata, Gallese, Luppino, Kaseda, & Sakata, 2000) and Raos et al. (Raos, Umilta, Murata, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2006) compared the object representations in AIP and F5 using multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis, and concluded that AIP furnishes a visual object description whereas F5 represents objects in motor terms (i.e. determined by the grip type used to grasp the object). However, the study of Raos et al. (Raos et al., 2006) focused on the F5p sector in ventral premotor cortex and most likely did not include F5a neurons. It is noteworthy that the 3D-shape stimuli we used did not resemble the objects that the monkeys had to grasp; nevertheless the same ensembles of neurons were active in F5a and AIP during 3D-shape presentation and during object grasping. At least part of this overlap in neural preference for these seemingly disparate stimulus classes may arise from the relatively broad tuning for objects in AIP and F5a, since most neurons in these areas respond to many objects (Pani et al., unpublished observations). Moreover, in addition to the 3D profile, AIP and F5a neurons also encode the 2D contour of objects, which may be based on relatively simple shape features that are shared between many objects. Also in the ITC, it is frequently difficult to identify a single object or shape feature that activates the neuron, and many neurons appear to respond to seemingly unrelated shape features, even in the 3D domain (Janssen et al., 2001). We have previously investigated the neural coding of 3D shape in the ITC and in AIP using the same stimuli (Janssen et al., 2000b; Srivastava et al., 2009). The neural representation of 3D shape information was very similar in areas F5a and AIP. The premotor area F5a contained many neurons for which a discrete approximation was sufficient to evoke selectivity, and was similar to area AIP in this regard (Srivastava et al., 2009). The sensitivity to the degree of the disparity variation with an overall monotonic coding and the selectivity for minute disparity differences in a small fraction of the neurons was also very similar to AIP. However, we observed a tendency towards a more elaborate and detailed representation in area F5a since in both monkeys, neurons were signaling subtle distinctions in depth profiles such as the difference between a linear approximation (a wedge-shape stimulus) and a smoothly-curved 3D shape, which was not observed in AIP (Srivastava et al., 2009). This refinement suggests that additional processing occurs between the output of AIP and the output of F5a before visual information connects to the motor system. Finally, in both F5a and AIP, 3D-shape selective neurons were embedded in clusters of neurons that were also active during visually-guided grasping. The latter observation highlights the probable behavioral role of 3D-shape selective neurons in the parietal and premotor cortex, i.e. to provide a visual 3D description of objects for the purpose of programming the grasp. Consistent with this idea, reversible inactivation of 3D-shape selective sites in AIP causes a marked deficit in grasping but not in the perceptual discrimination of 3D-structure (Verhoef, Vogels and Janssen, unpublished observations). However, we also observed similarities between ITC, AIP and F5a in 3D-shape coding. We observed zero-order (position in depth), first-order (tilt/slant) and second-order disparity selectivity (curvature) in all three areas. In contrast, earlier visual areas such as V4 contain zero- and first-order neurons (Hinkle & Connor, 2002) but no second-order neurons (Hegde & Van Essen, 2005). Hence the properties of neurons in lower-tier areas appear to be reiterated at the highest levels in dorsal, ventral and premotor areas. Furthermore, in a manner similar to neurons in area AIP (Theys et al.) and area IT (Janssen et al., 2001) F5a cells showed selectivity for three-dimensional surfaces and boundaries. The anatomical connectivity between areas AIP and F5a (Gerbella et al., 2011), the response latency difference between AIP and F5a (approximately 10 ms, (Theys et al., 2012)), and the strong resemblance in functional properties (fast and coarse coding, grasping-related activity) suggest a hierarchical parietofrontal 3D processing network for the control of grasping, distinct from the ventral stream 3D-shape representation in IT. Whether the selectivity in area F5a depends exclusively on input from area AIP needs further confirmation through combined recording-inactivation experiments. The current physiological and anatomical evidence suggests that AIP input could be processed in area F5a and translated into a more motoric code for the F5p neurons which project to M1 and the spinal cord (Gerbella et al., 2011). Consistent with this hypothesis, virtual lesions of the human AIP using theta-burst Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) disrupt the normal PMv-M1 interactions during grasp preparation (Davare, Rothwell, & Lemon, 2010), suggesting that these PMv-M1 interactions depend on the object information provided by AIP to the premotor cortex. Human fMRI studies indicate that part of the PMv is also activated more strongly by curved surfaces than by flat surfaces at different positions in depth (Georgieva, Peeters, Kolster, Todd, & Orban, 2009), which could be homologous to the F5a region of the monkey. On the other hand, the homology between the human AIP (hAIP) (Culham & Kanwisher, 2001; Culham et al., 2003; Frey, Vinton, Norlund, & Grafton, 2005; Begliomini, Wall, Smith, & Castiello, 2007; Cavina-Pratesi, Goodale, & Culham, 2007) and the monkey AIP may be more questionable. We recorded in the posterior part of AIP (Srivastava et al., 2009), in which strong visual (3D-shape selective) and grasping responses can be measured but no somatosensory responses are found (Pani, Theys and Janssen, unpublished observations). A range of fMRI studies in humans and monkeys (reviewed in (Orban, 2011) suggest that posterior AIP in the monkey may correspond more to the DIPSA region in the human, which is located in the IPS posterior to the hAIP and is also activated by 3D shape defined by disparity (Durand, Peeters, Norman, Todd, & Orban, 2009). The hAIP, in contrast, is more activated during grasping than during reaching (Culham et al., 2003), responds even to somatosensory stimulation (Bodegard, Geyer, Grefkes, Zilles, & Roland, 2001), but is not (or only weakly) activated by disparity-defined 3D shape (Durand et al., 2009), similar to the more anterior part of AIP. [Note that DIPSA may also be activated during grasping, but not more than during reaching because of the strong visual responses in this region.] Furthermore Culham et al. (Culham et al., 2003) reported that the hAIP is not activated by images of objects, whereas we observed strong and selective responses to images of objects in the macaque AIP (Romero, Van Dromme, & Janssen, 2012). These apparently conflicting results between human fMRI and single-cell studies may have been caused by the homology between the macaque posterior AIP and a more posterior region in the human IPS (DIPSA) resulting from the expansion of the human IPS areas compared to the monkey. A similar reasoning may apply for the homology between the human Lateral Occipital Complex (LOC (Malach et al., 1995; Grill-Spector, Kourtzi, & Kanwisher, 2001) and monkey ITC. The question remains as to why two separate but very similar visual representations of 3D shape should exist in both AIP and F5a for the control of grasping. One explanation could involve differences in neural properties that we did not test for, such as different receptive field sizes. Another possibility is that F5a neurons could respond to larger object parts or more complex shape features than AIP neurons, comparable to the V4 – TEO – TE hierarchy in the ventral stream (Kourtzi & Connor, 2011). The observation that F5a neurons, but not AIP neurons, signal the difference between a smoothly curved 3D shape and its linear approximation suggests that the 3D-shape representation in F5a may be more refined than in AIP and that additional processing is required before connecting to the motor system. Extensive receptive field mapping and a systematic variation of shape features to determine their critical features in each of these two areas could answer such questions. Multi-unit recordings in F5a revealed that 3D-shape selective visual-dominant neurons are co-localized with visuomotor neurons that were active during grasping in the light and in the dark (Theys et al., 2012). Furthermore F5p/c neurons showed sluggish and nonselective responses to our 3D-shape stimuli (Theys et al., 2012), and previous studies have demonstrated that objects are encoded in motor terms rather than in visual terms in F5p (Raos et al., 2006). Therefore we hypothesize that the main reason for an intermediate area (F5a) between AIP and F5p may be that the connection between visual information and motor commands occurs in these clusters of visual-dominant and visuomotor neurons in F5a which then project to F5p. In this interpretation the grasping-related activity we observed in 3D-shape selective clusters of AIP would be entirely different in nature: motor-related grasping activity in AIP may arise as a corollary discharge from visuomotor and motor-dominant F5p neurons projecting back to AIP for online visual control (Rizzolatti & Luppino, 2001), a hypothesis that deserves experimental testing. We also observed interindividual differences between the two animals in the disparity sensitivity test: the average F5a and AIP population response in M1 showed a larger decline at the point where the disparity curvature changes sign (between the 0.03 deg preferred amplitude and 0.03 deg non-preferred amplitude) compared to monkey M2. The observed difference in neural sensitivities was most likely related to a difference in the quality of stereoscopic vision in the two animals, since M1 learned to discriminate disparity-defined curved surfaces in a limited number of training sessions whereas M2 did not. However, M2 was not stereoblind, since we recorded large numbers of disparity-selective neurons in this animal’s AIP and F5a. Furthermore, we measured a normal stereo visually-evoked potential (VEP) over V1 with disparity stimuli in this animal (Janssen, Vogels, & Orban, 1998; Srivastava et al., 2009). In humans also, a wide range of stereoscopic capacities and stereoanomalies can be observed (Howard & Rogers, 2002). Individual differences in neural sensitivity that we observed suggest that differences between cortical areas have to be interpreted cautiously if the data are not acquired in the same animals. However, our two monkeys were quite comparable in the proportions of zero-, first- and second-order neurons, as well as in the proportion of neurons showing selectivity for at least one of the discrete stimuli. Furthermore, in both animals the average AIP population response in the sensitivity test was largely monotonic. Previous monkey fMRI studies found stronger activations for curved surfaces compared to flat surfaces at different positions-in-depth which were highly localized in F5a (Joly et al., 2009), but very extensive and comprising a large part of AIP and the anterior region of LIP (Durand et al., 2007) in the lateral bank of the IPS. However in our single-cell recording experiments, the extent of the recording area that contained 3D-shape selective neurons was highly similar in AIP and F5a, matching closely the F5a activation described in Joly et al. (Joly et al., 2009). Future studies will have to investigate the neural basis of the difference between F5a and AIP in the hemodynamic response to curved surfaces. Acknowledgments We thank Piet Kayenbergh, Gerrit Meulemans, Stijn Verstraeten, Marc Depaep, Wouter Depuydt and Inez Puttemans for assistance, and Steve Raiguel for comments on the manuscript. Grants This work was supported by Geconcerteerde Onderzoeksacties (GOA 2005/18, 2010/19), Fonds voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek Vlaanderen grants (G.0495.05, G.0713.09), Excellentiefinanciering (EF05/014), Programmafinanciering (PFV/10/008) and ERC-StG260607. Disclosure The authors report no conflicts of interest. Reference List Begliomini, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., & Castiello, U. (2007). Differential cortical activity for precision and whole-hand visually guided grasping in humans. Eur.J.Neurosci., 25, 1245-1252. Binkofski, F., Dohle, C., Posse, S., Stephan, K. M., Hefter, H., Seitz, R. J. et al. (1998). Human anterior intraparietal area subserves prehension: a combined lesion and functional MRI activation study. Neurology, 50, 1253-1259. Bodegard, A., Geyer, S., Grefkes, C., Zilles, K., & Roland, P. E. (2001). Hierarchical processing of tactile shape in the human brain. Neuron, 31, 317-328. Cavina-Pratesi, C., Goodale, M. A., & Culham, J. C. (2007). FMRI reveals a dissociation between grasping and perceiving the size of real 3D objects. PLoS.One., 2, e424. Cavina-Pratesi, C., Monaco, S., Fattori, P., Galletti, C., McAdam, T. D., Quinlan, D. J. et al. (2010). Functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals the neural substrates of arm transport and grip formation in reach-to-grasp actions in humans. J.Neurosci, 30, 10306-10323. Culham, J. C., Danckert, S. L., DeSouza, J. F., Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Visually guided grasping produces fMRI activation in dorsal but not ventral stream brain areas. Exp.Brain Res., 153, 180-189. Culham, J. C., & Kanwisher, N. G. (2001). Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Curr.Opin.Neurobiol., 11, 157-163. Davare, M., Rothwell, J. C., & Lemon, R. N. (2010). Causal connectivity between the human anterior intraparietal area and premotor cortex during grasp. Curr.Biol., 20, 176-181. Dijkerman, H. C., Milner, A. D., & Carey, D. P. (1996). The perception and prehension of objects oriented in the depth plane. I. Effects of visual form agnosia. Exp.Brain Res., 112, 442-451. Durand, J. B., Nelissen, K., Joly, O., Wardak, C., Todd, J. T., Norman, J. F. et al. (2007). Anterior regions of monkey parietal cortex process visual 3D shape. Neuron, 55, 493505. Durand, J. B., Peeters, R., Norman, J. F., Todd, J. T., & Orban, G. A. (2009). Parietal regions processing visual 3D shape extracted from disparity. Neuroimage., 46, 1114-1126. Faillenot, I., Toni, I., Decety, J., Gregoire, M. C., & Jeannerod, M. (1997). Visual pathways for object-oriented action and object recognition: functional anatomy with PET. Cereb.Cortex, 7, 77-85. Fluet, M. C., Baumann, M. A., & Scherberger, H. (2010). Context-specific grasp movement representation in macaque ventral premotor cortex. J.Neurosci, 30, 15175-15184. Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). Cortical mechanism for the visual guidance of hand grasping movements in the monkey: A reversible inactivation study. Brain, 124, 571-586. Frey, S. H., Vinton, D., Norlund, R., & Grafton, S. T. (2005). Cortical topography of human anterior intraparietal cortex active during visually guided grasping. Brain Res.Cogn Brain Res., 23, 397-405. Gallese, V., Murata, A., Kaseda, M., Niki, N., & Sakata, H. (1994). Deficit of hand preshaping after muscimol injection in monkey parietal cortex. Neuroreport, 5, 15251529. Georgieva, S., Peeters, R., Kolster, H., Todd, J. T., & Orban, G. A. (2009). The processing of three-dimensional shape from disparity in the human brain. J.Neurosci, 29, 727-742. Gerbella, M., Belmalih, A., Borra, E., Rozzi, S., & Luppino, G. (2011). Cortical connections of the anterior (F5a) subdivision of the macaque ventral premotor area F5. Brain Struct.Funct., 216, 43-65. Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci., 15, 20-25. Grill-Spector, K., Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vision Res., 41, 1409-1422. Hegde, J., & Van Essen, D. C. (2005). Role of primate visual area V4 in the processing of 3-D shape characteristics defined by disparity. J.Neurophysiol., 94, 2856-2866. Hinkle, D. A., & Connor, C. E. (2002). Three-dimensional orientation tuning in macaque area V4. Nat.Neurosci, 5, 665-670. Howard, I. P., & Rogers, B. J. (2002). Seeing in depth. Toronto: I. PORTEUS. Jacobs, S., Danielmeier, C., & Frey, S. H. (2010). Human anterior intraparietal and ventral premotor cortices support representations of grasping with the hand or a novel tool. J.Cogn Neurosci., 22, 2594-2608. Janssen, P., Vogels, R., Liu, Y., & Orban, G. A. (2001). Macaque inferior temporal neurons are selective for three-dimensional boundaries and surfaces. J.Neurosci., 21, 94199429. Janssen, P., Vogels, R., & Orban, G. A. (1998). Assessment of stereopsis in rhesus monkeys using visual evoked potentials. Doc.Ophthalmol., 95, 247-255. Janssen, P., Vogels, R., & Orban, G. A. (1999). Macaque inferior temporal neurons are selective for disparity-defined three-dimensional shapes. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A, 96, 8217-8222. Janssen, P., Vogels, R., & Orban, G. A. (2000a). Selectivity for 3D shape that reveals distinct areas within macaque inferior temporal cortex. Science, 288, 2054-2056. Janssen, P., Vogels, R., & Orban, G. A. (2000b). Three-dimensional shape coding in inferior temporal cortex. Neuron, 27, 385-397. Jeannerod, M. (1981). Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In Attention and Performance IX. Long, J. & Baddeley (Eds.), A.Erlbaum, Hillsdale. 153-168. Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., & Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci., 18, 314-320. Joly, O., Vanduffel, W., & Orban, G. A. (2009). The monkey ventral premotor cortex processes 3D shape from disparity. Neuroimage., 47, 262-272. Katsuyama, N., Yamashita, A., Sawada, K., Naganuma, T., Sakata, H., & Taira, M. (2010). Functional and histological properties of caudal intraparietal area of macaque monkey. Neuroscience. Kourtzi, Z., & Connor, C. E. (2011). Neural representations for object perception: structure, category, and adaptive coding. Annu.Rev Neurosci, 34, 45-67. Malach, R., Reppas, J. B., Benson, R. R., Kwong, K. K., Jiang, H., Kennedy, W. A. et al. (1995). Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A, 92, 8135-8139. Marotta, J. J., Behrmann, M., & Goodale, M. A. (1997). The removal of binocular cues disrupts the calibration of grasping in patients with visual form agnosia. Exp.Brain Res., 116, 113-121. Matelli, M., Camarda, R., Glickstein, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (1986). Afferent and efferent projections of the inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. J.Comp Neurol., 251, 281298. Murata, A., Gallese, V., Luppino, G., Kaseda, M., & Sakata, H. (2000). Selectivity for the shape, size, and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of monkey parietal area AIP. J.Neurophysiol., 83, 2580-2601. Nelissen, K., & Vanduffel, W. (2011). Grasping-related functional magnetic resonance imaging brain responses in the macaque monkey. J.Neurosci, 31, 8220-8229. Nguyenkim, J. D., & DeAngelis, G. C. (2003). Disparity-based coding of three-dimensional surface orientation by macaque middle temporal neurons. J.Neurosci, 23, 7117-7128. Orban, G. A. (2011). The extraction of 3D shape in the visual system of human and nonhuman primates. Annu.Rev.Neurosci., 34, 361-388. Preston, T. J., Li, S., Kourtzi, Z., & Welchman, A. E. (2008). Multivoxel pattern selectivity for perceptually relevant binocular disparities in the human brain. J.Neurosci, 28, 11315-11327. Raos, V., Umilta, M. A., Murata, A., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Functional properties of grasping-related neurons in the ventral premotor area F5 of the macaque monkey. J.Neurophysiol., 95, 709-729. Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., & Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp.Brain Res., 71, 491-507. Rizzolatti, G., & Luppino, G. (2001). The cortical motor system. Neuron, 31, 889-901. Romero, M. C., Van Dromme, I., & Janssen, P. (2012). Responses to two-dimensional shapes in the macaque anterior intraparietal area. Eur.J.Neurosci., 36, 2324-2334. Sakata, H., & Kusunoki, M. (1992). Organization of space perception: neural representation of three-dimensional space in the posterior parietal cortex. Curr.Opin.Neurobiol., 2, 170-174. Sakata, H., Taira, M., Kusunoki, M., Murata, A., & Tanaka, Y. (1997). The TINS Lecture. The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action. Trends Neurosci., 20, 350-357. Sakata, H., Taira, M., Murata, A., & Mine, S. (1995). Neural mechanisms of visual guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Cereb.Cortex, 5, 429-438. Shikata, E., Tanaka, Y., Nakamura, H., Taira, M., & Sakata, H. (1996). Selectivity of the parietal visual neurones in 3D orientation of surface of stereoscopic stimuli. Neuroreport, 7, 2389-2394. Srivastava, S., Orban, G. A., De Maziere, P. A., & Janssen, P. (2009). A distinct representation of three-dimensional shape in macaque anterior intraparietal area: fast, metric, and coarse. J.Neurosci., 29, 10613-10626. Taira, M., Mine, S., Georgopoulos, A. P., Murata, A., & Sakata, H. (1990). Parietal cortex neurons of the monkey related to the visual guidance of hand movement. Exp.Brain Res., 83, 29-36. Taira, M., Tsutsui, K. I., Jiang, M., Yara, K., & Sakata, H. (2000). Parietal neurons represent surface orientation from the gradient of binocular disparity. J.Neurophysiol., 83, 31403146. Theys, T., Pani, P., van Loon, J., Goffin, J., & Janssen, P. (2012). Selectivity for threedimensional shape and grasping-related activity in the macaque ventral premotor cortex. J.Neurosci., 32, 12038-12050. Theys, T., Srivastava, S., van Loon, J., Goffin, J., & Janssen, P. (2012). Selectivity for threedimensional contours and surfaces in the anterior intraparietal area. J.Neurophysiol., 107, 995-1008. Verhoef, B. E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2010). Contribution of inferior temporal and posterior parietal activity to three-dimensional shape perception. Curr.Biol., 20, 909913. Verhoef, B. E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2011). Synchronization between the end-stages of the dorsal and the ventral visual stream. J.Neurophysiol.. Verhoef, B. E., Vogels, R., & Janssen, P. (2012). Inferotemporal cortex subserves threedimensional structure categorization. Neuron, 73, 171-182. Watt, S. J., & Bradshaw, M. F. (2003). The visual control of reaching and grasping: binocular disparity and motion parallax. J.Exp.Psychol.Hum.Percept.Perform., 29, 404-415. Figure legends Figure 1. Recording positions and stimuli. A. Recording areas. Coronal images illustrating the most representative recording sites in area F5a and area AIP for monkey M1. The inset shows the estimated recording positions in F5a and AIP (red areas on the lateral view of the monkey brain). B. Monocular images presented to the left and right eyes and schematic illustrations of the perceived 3D structure are shown for one convex 3D shape stimulus, the linear approximation, one of the discrete approximations and the first-order stimulus approximation. Note that the only depth cue arises from binocular disparity and that the shading in the figures is added for illustration only. Exchanging the monocular images between the eyes yielded the opposite depth profiles. Figure 2. First-order disparity selectivity in area F5a. A. Example neuron. PSTH of a first-order F5a neuron showing the responses to the preferred (top row) and nonpreferred (bottom row) original 3D shape (first column), its linear approximation (second column, which was also a first-order approximation), and the three discrete approximations (rightmost columns). The neuron showed robust selectivity for the inclined three-dimensional shape and for the first-order stimulus. The three discrete approximations yielded similarly strong selectivity. The vertical calibration bar indicates 100 spikes/s. B. Population response. Mean (± SEM) net responses to the preferred (open bars) and nonpreferred (shaded bars) original 3D shape (first column), the linear approximation (second column) and three discrete approximations (right), for all first-order neurons (N = 39) are shown. Asterisks indicate significant selectivity in the population (p < 0.01). Figure 3. Second-order disparity selectivity in area F5a. A. Example neuron. PSTH of a second-order F5a neuron for the preferred (top row) and nonpreferred (bottom row) original 3D shape (first column), its linear approximation (second column), followed by the three discrete approximations, and the first-order approximation (rightmost column). The vertical calibration bar indicates 100 spikes/s. B. Population response. Mean net responses to the preferred (open bars) and nonpreferred (shaded bars) original three-dimensional shape (left), the linear approximation (wedge stimulus), the three discrete approximations, and the first-order stimuli (right), for all second-order F5a neurons (N = 32). Asterisks indicate significant selectivity in the population (p < 0.01). Figure 4. Disparity order test in F5a: scatterplots A. The net response (left panel) and the net response difference (right panel) to the best firstorder stimulus is plotted as a function of the response and the response difference to the original preferred 3D shape for first-order (blue) and second-order (red) neurons. B. The net response (left) and the net response difference (right) to the best discrete approximation is plotted against the net response and the net response difference to the original smoothly curved 3D shape for all second-order neurons (N = 32). C. The net response (left) and the net response difference (right) to the linear approximation is plotted against the net response and the net response difference to the original smoothlycurved 3D shape for all second-order neurons (N = 32). Figure 5. Sensitivity test in F5a: example neurons. The mean net response (spikes/sec) is plotted as a function of the 12 different degrees of disparity variation (-1.3 +1.3 deg, positive values were used for the preferred disparity variation) for three example F5a cells illustrating the different types of neurons: monotonic (green), tuned (blue) and broadband (red). Figure 6. Sensitivity test: population analysis. A. The mean normalized response in M1 (left) and M2 (right) plotted as a function of the 12 different degrees of disparity variation (-1.3 +1.3 deg) for F5a neurons tested in the sensitivity test (N = 61 for M1 and N = 70 for M2). B. The mean normalized population response for M1 (left) and M2 (right) plotted as a function of the 12 different degrees of disparity variation (-1.3 +1.3 deg) for AIP neurons tested in the sensitivity test (N = 38 for M1 and N = 33 for M2). Positive values indicate the preferred disparity variation. Figure 7. Visually-guided grasping: population response. The average response of a subset of 3D-shape selective neurons in area AIP (N =16) is plotted as a function of time (during object fixation and reach-to-grasp) after stimulus onset (left), time of the go-signal (middle) and time of object lift (right panel). For comparison, the average response of a subset of 3D-shape selective neurons in area F5a (N = 98) is plotted in the same task. Tables Table 1. Sensitivity test: relative proportions (%) of different types of neurons (monotonic, broadband and tuned neurons) in area F5a and area AIP. F5a AIP M1 M2 M1 M2 Monotonic 79 66 74 85 Broadband 8 30 16 6 Tuned 13 4 10 9 M1: Monkey 1, M2: Monkey 2