Managing Flash Floods: A Cultural Perspective of Risk Perception Ashley R. Coles University of Arizona Abstract This research investigates how culture affects risk perception and behavior in the context of flash floods. Approximately 1,000 citizens of Tucson, Arizona will fill out surveys assessing cultural characteristics as well as historical and hypothetical behavior during flash flood scenarios. Neighborhoods will be selected from those with regular street flooding problems and the highest amount of diversity regarding race and socioeconomic status. The cultural factors explored here include trust, social incorporation, self- and community-efficacy, social autonomy, and time orientation. Multiple linear regression analysis will reveal which of these factors predict whether people say they would cross a flooded roadway or wash. If cultural variation appears to predict stated behavior, this study confirms that risk managers must consider cultural variation in order to effectively execute hazard mitigation. Introduction Risk perception research has proven useful in natural hazards mitigation by investigating how people might respond to hazard information, and how risk managers might alter warnings or dissemination methods to stimulate a wider practice of what they consider the “appropriate” response. These studies often assume that actions result from either universal human nature or from psychological traits unique to each individual. In most cases, risk managers attribute failure to respond appropriately to irrationality or lack of information (Douglas, 1992; Jasanoff, 1998; Slovic, 1999; Frewer, 2004). Thus, risk communication research and practice have focused on tailoring the message until people understand and cease engagement in risk-taking behaviors (Douglas, 1992; Kasperson & Kasperson, 2005). However, risk perception studies often fail to include the assessment of how factors such as cultural context and social networks may affect how people perceive threats and respond to warnings, not to mention how cultural norms may influence the distribution of vulnerability. The studies that do consider the effects of culture tend to focus on a singular aspect, and many do not contain information that risk managers may utilize in practical application. This study aims to fill this research gap by working directly with risk managers to create a comprehensive understanding of how several cultural factors affect propensity to engage in risk-taking behavior, using the example of crossing a flooded roadway or wash. Investigators will examine five key cultural factors known to affect risk perception and how these factors predict participants’ historical or hypothetical behavior in flash flood scenarios. These factors include (a) trust in the hazard science as well as the government; (b) social incorporation, or the extent of social networks; (c) self- and communityefficacy, which describes how confident people feel in their ability to handle both extreme events and ordinary life; (d) social autonomy, the degree of freedom to fill any social role; and (e) time orientation, whether people focus on the past, present, or future. Survey questions will elicit responses that reveal each participant’s cultural characteristics, the predicting power of which will be determined through multiple regression analysis. The survey will also present past and hypothetical situations as both a series of questions and a few short vignettes for which participants must decide whether or not they would cross the flooded street or wash. Investigators hypothesize that propensity to claim tendencies to engage in risk-taking behaviors will vary among cultures, and that the multiple regression analysis will reveal which cultural factors have the strongest predictive powers. The information gathered in this project may help flood control managers understand risk-taking behavior as it varies with culture, rather than attributing such behaviors to irrationality or lack of information. Literature Review Douglas (1992) suggests that the lens through which to view risk perception and response is culture. Culture refers to a way of life learned from and shared by a social unit, including – but not limited to – attitudes, beliefs, values, and habits. Values and social norms shape an individual’s worldview, and therefore affect how the individual will understand threats and determine appropriate methods of threat avoidance or amelioration (Douglas, 1992). Tobin and Montz (1997) also recognize the need to consider non-geophysical factors to understand all aspects of natural hazards. They define natural hazards as the “potential interaction between humans and extreme natural events (p. 5),” and natural disasters as the actual interactions that have impacts on society economically or physically. They claim that humans are not only necessarily part of this system, but that everyday social factors such as goals, concerns, values, and institutions play a large role in how people perceive, mitigate, and respond to hazard events. This is not to say that other factors, such as previous experience with a particular hazard, do not influence behavior before, during, and after an event, but that in fact the enormous impact of culture on people’s values and beliefs may affect just how these other factors play a role in an individual’s actions. Cultural psychologists explain that culture has such a strong effect on an individual’s actions because culture and psyche mutually and constantly constitute one another. Norms, values, and practices shape how individuals process information and make decisions, and the resulting behaviors either maintain or reshape those norms, values, and practices (Fiske et al., 1998; Kitayama & Markus, 1995; Markus et al., 1997; Shweder, 1995). While many regard culture as a national or multi-national variable, it also includes subgroups based on “gender, ethnicity, religion, cohort or generation, historical period, profession, social class, and country of origin” (Kitayama & Markus, 1995, p. 368). To obtain a comprehensive understanding of how culture and behavior construct one another, researchers must consider multiple levels as well as multiple factors of culture. Theoretical and applied hazards research tend to focus on trust, social incorporation, self- and community-efficacy, social autonomy, and time orientation as the key cultural factors that affect risk perception and response (e.g., Douglas, 1992; McGee & Russell, 2003; Slovic, 1999). Trust holds such strong influence over an individual’s risk perception because it serves as an adaptation to an increasingly complex environment. To reduce the necessity for learning every detail about every possible risk, individuals place trust in those whom they believe to both have the knowledge and willingness to share accurate information about particular risks (Slovic, 1999; Siegrist and Cvetkovich, 2000; Savadori et al., 2004; Lang & Hallman, 2005). In the case of flash floods, the public must place trust in both the science behind risk assessment as well as the government that sponsors such research and holds responsibility for flood mitigation. The fragility of trust makes the issue much more salient, particularly in matters of risk management. As Slovic (1999) notes, “distrust, once initiated, tends to reinforce and perpetuate distrust” (p. 698). Once an institution has earned a reputation for distrust, individuals tend to avoid contact or automatically reject even trust-building information. The resilience of opinions formed based on distrust thus often creates a barrier that prevents experts from communicating new or amended hazard information. The search for appropriate media and messages for risk communication becomes futile when the public lacks trust in the messenger, because “if trust is lacking, no form or process of communication will be satisfactory” (Slovic, 1999, p. 697). Social incorporation as defined by Douglas (1992) refers to the extent of an individual’s social network as well as the degree of connection between its members. Extensive social networks facilitate appropriate precautions and actions for hazard events by increasing the likelihood that information will be heard and believed by individuals. Many studies have shown that people seek confirmation of the severity of a hazard and the need to prepare or respond appropriately from a variety of sources before they will take action (Mileti, 1995; Brilly & Polic, 2005). These sources may be different agencies, government or emergency management officials, or the media, but often people consult with friends, family, or neighbors before making a decision. Scherer and Cho (2003) have developed the social contagion theory, which states that individuals model their attitudes and behaviors after those of the members of their social networks. This helps to explain studies that have shown that people evacuate more readily if they know that neighbors, friends, or family members are evacuating (Mileti 1995; Dow & Cutter, 2000). The social amplification of risk framework (SARF) provides a theoretical model for how information about hazards and risk perceptions may be amplified or attenuated as people consult with their social networks (Kasperson, 1992; Masuda & Garvin, 2006). Rumors or information will be propagated as it fits preconceived mental models, beliefs, or values (Kasperson, 1992). This means that if the hazard message conforms to these predetermined notions of reality, the receiver will attempt to spread information and amplify the apparent risk. Otherwise, the threat will be downplayed by either circulating contrary information or failure to propagate any information at all. Kasperson (1992) describes culture as a “super-variable” that affects all stages within SARF, including amplification and attenuation processes. Since culture determines an individual’s worldview, it is likely to affect whether individuals amplify or attenuate hazard information (Kasperson, 1992; Masuda & Garvin, 2006). Sáenz (2003) would agree, and states that just educating the public is insufficient, because “individuals and groups possess a series of subjective or affective components that impede or allow for the incorporation of new knowledge” (p. 124). Efficacy describes whether individuals or groups determine that a risk may actually be reduced as well as whether they are capable of taking the proper precautions or reactions during a hazard event (Tobin & Montz, 1997; Paton & Johnston, 2001). Both self- and community-efficacy are primarily related to an individual’s apparent locus of control, the latter referring to an aggregate of individuals. An internal locus of control puts the individual in charge of his or her own destiny, and the resulting higher sense of efficacy increases the likelihood that an individual will react to warnings and take measures to secure their life and property. However, should the individual assume they have complete control over their destiny, regardless of their awareness of the hazard in question, they may feel invincible and take no precautionary or reactionary measures. On the community level, a high sense of efficacy will encourage and foster collective action and cooperation. Individuals with an external locus of control tend to feel helpless in hazard situations and depend on others for protection, rather than attempt to protect themselves. Certain religious beliefs contribute to fatalistic tendencies because people may assume that they cannot alter their pre-determined destiny by taking precautionary or reactionary measures (Douglas, 1992; Inelman et al., 2004). Social marginalization also produces this sense of fatalism by reducing resources and options available to people during exposure to a hazard. Social autonomy measures variation in both real and perceived access to information and materials as a function of the degree to which individuals have the freedom to move between various social roles (Douglas, 1992). Many factors determine social status including age, gender, race, ethnicity, education, socioeconomic status, or any other standards by which societies produce social hierarchies. Even in the United States, widely considered an egalitarian society, certain groups must cope with downward social constitution. Markus, Steele, and Steel (2000) define downward social constitution as the experience of being in a setting where, based on a given group identity, one is exposed to a potentially limiting and devaluing concert of representations, historical narratives, possible judgments, treatments, interactions, expectations, and affective reactions. (p. 235) Thus, not only will certain groups find access to resources limited, they will come to expect such limitations. This may drastically affect decision-making not only by restricting which options an individual has available, but also which options an individual even considers. As mentioned before, time orientation refers to whether individuals focus on the past, present, or future. Cultures that focus on the past tend to have difficulty conceptualizing unforeseen or unpredictable events (Douglas, 1992). This produces beliefs that because an event has failed to occur in the past, it probably will not occur in the future. When a major event does occur, however, many see it as anomalous and believe that it will not happen again within their lifetime (Tobin & Montz, 1997). This also fails to account for natural and anthropogenic changes in the environment, which will affect the magnitude and frequency of future hazards. On the other hand, little emphasis on the past causes people to disregard the frequency of hazard events despite experience with them, and little emphasis on the future leads to the tendency to take action (or fail to take action) in order to improve current conditions at the expense of those to come. In the context of natural hazards, present-focused individuals may opt out of insurance and invest the money they would have spent in other ventures or luxuries. This occurs despite knowledge of past events and probability of future events to come. This does not necessarily reflect lack of reasoning, as Douglas (1992) is quick to point out, but rather indicates a preference. Wisner et al. (2004) would agree that these individuals have merely decided that their current standard of living is more important than their future one, which may or may not include a hazard event. Those with a particular concern for the future will likely exercise more caution for fear of the unknown, and will therefore engage in risk-taking behavior less often. Theoretical Framework This research falls within the psychometric paradigm commonly used in hazards and risk perception research (Slovic, 2000). However, previous studies conducted under this paradigm mostly focus on psychological functioning at an individual level. This project integrates cultural psychology theory by attempting to discern how culture affects psychological processes, which then affect behavior. This process will be examined by assessing the five aforementioned cultural characteristics, and then determining how these characteristics differ among the groups, as well as how they influence risk perception and behavior. The following factors are expected to increase the likelihood that an individual will report that they have historically or would hypothetically cross a flooded roadway or wash: Hypothesis 1: Low trust in the science behind risk assessments, the government, or both will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals will not accept information from a distrusted source. Hypothesis 2: Low social incorporation will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals have less opportunity to receive new information. Hypothesis 3: High self-efficacy will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals will believe themselves capable of surviving the risk unscathed. Hypothesis 4: Either high or low social autonomy may increase reports of risktaking behavior. Those with low social autonomy may feel they have no choice but to take the risk. However, those with low social autonomy have more to lose by taking the risk, so those with high social autonomy will probably report more risk-taking behavior than those with low social autonomy. Hypothesis 5: Past or present time orientation will increase reports of risk taking behavior because those individuals have less concern for the future and the consequences of their actions. Method Tucson, Arizona provides an optimal site for this case study for both physical and political reasons. Hundreds of streets and intersections flood when it rains due to a combination of poor drainage systems and numerous wash crossings without bridges. Flood control managers have created several sign and road barrier projects in an attempt to deter motorists from driving into flooded roadways. An Arizona state law now obligates individuals to pay fines in order to offset costs incurred by the city if they drive around a barricade and require rescue (Arizona Revised Statutes, 2006). Despite these and other programs designed to inform the public about the dangers of driving through flooded roadways and otherwise discourage such behavior, losses to life and property continue to occur. This, coupled with Tucson’s rich variety of cultural and socioeconomic demographics provides a perfect context within which to test the theory that cultural factors affect risk perception and behavior during a flash flood. In addition, interactions with flood risk managers in Tucson have revealed the tendency to assume irrationality or lack of education as the cause for this particular risk-taking behavior. To ensure that the information gathered through this research provides a substantial degree of utility for the risk managers, they will be included throughout most stages of the project. The Climate Assessment for the Southwest research institution will host and provide support for initial, intermediary, and final meetings between the flood risk managers and the survey research team. Background information, research plans, and results will be discussed so that the final results may be used in future implementations of risk management policy. Ideally this research would target members of the population who have crossed flooded roadways or washes and compare them to those who have not. However, the ephemeral nature of the former group inhibits efforts to locate them. Many have crossed without any trouble, and many have perished in the process. This project therefore requires widespread dispersal of surveys in order to capture members of many different cultural groups who have, have not, probably will, and probably will not cross flooded roadways and washes. Locations of flood barriers and problem areas have been mapped out in relation to neighborhood association boundaries. Surveys will be mailed to approximately 1,000 Tucson residents in neighborhoods with at least one flood barrier or sign, giving preference to neighborhoods that the U.S. Census 2000 tract data show as the most culturally and socioeconomically diverse. In order to potentially increase response rate, local neighborhood associations will be contacted in advance and asked for support. Each household will receive a copy of the survey in both English and Spanish, along with a brief description of the research and a reply-paid envelope. To further maximize response rates, a postcard reminder will arrive a week later, and a second copy of the questionnaire will arrive after another two weeks (McGuirk & O’Neill, 2005). To compensate respondents for their participation and to encourage response, all respondents that return a survey will be entered in a random drawing for a prize. The social stigma attached to crossing a flooded roadway or wash may pressure participants to answer strategically about past and possible future experiences in order to appear more rational. The instructions provided with the survey will attempt to counteract this behavior by carefully explaining that the investigators seek information about the logical thought processes that lead to the decision to engage in this behavior, and do not assume that such a decision results from irrationality (Iarossi, 2006). This should help to reduce some of the disparity between reported and actual behavior. The use of a self-completed survey rather than an interview may also encourage honest responses that participants might be reluctant to provide in face-to-face contact (Buckingham and Saunders, 2004). Questions will be directed so as to elicit information regarding the five cultural factors outlined above through everyday practices and beliefs, as well as through explicit situations related to flash floods. This information will be compared to participants’ stated typical or hypothetical responses to situations in which they encounter a flooded street crossing. Types of possible answers to survey questions will include discrete categories, Likert scale ratings, and rankings. Since multiple choice answers tend to limit responses to those preconceived by the investigator, the survey will also include space for optional short answer responses that will allow the participant to elaborate or explain their selection (McGuirk and O’Neill, 2005). This survey design will allow investigators to perform quantitative analyses on the data without sacrificing the opportunity for participants to provide deep, rich, and varied information that may open discussions for later studies. Data will be stored in a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet, which will allow for the performance of statistical calculations as well as simultaneous storage of the open-ended responses with their corresponding multiple-choice answers. Quantitative analysis of the results will include a multiple linear regression of the five cultural factors to see how well each one predicts historical and hypothetical risk-taking behavior. This project requires this type of analysis because the cultural groups and their effects overlap and confound analysis of each mediating variable alone. Qualitative analysis will include the openended explicated responses, which will be coded by the investigators and then used to theorize about the results, particularly where the hypotheses fail. Results The multiple linear regression analysis will reveal which of the five factors have the strongest predicting power of participants’ stated historical or hypothetical behavior. Groups of participants based on race or ethnicity, gender, socioeconomic status, level of education, and age should vary among their responses to whether they have, have not, would, or would not cross a flooded roadway or wash. Hypothesis 1: Low trust in the science behind risk assessments, the government, or both will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals will not accept information from a distrusted source. Investigators expect level of education to positively correlate with trust in science. Groups that may display low trust in the government because of historical experiences include Mexican Americans, American Indians, and individualistic European Americans (for the latter, see Bellah et al., 1985). Hypothesis 2: Low social incorporation will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals have less opportunity to receive new information. Independent cultures, such as European Americans, tend to have smaller social networks and less connection to those networks than those in interdependent cultures, such as Asian Americans, Mexican Americans, and American Indians (Fiske et al., 1998; Kitayama & Markus, 1995; Markus et al., 1997). Hypothesis 3: High self-efficacy will increase reports of risk-taking behavior because individuals will believe themselves capable of surviving the risk unscathed. Investigators predict that European Americans, those with high socioeconomic status, those with high education, young adults, and males will have higher self-efficacy than other groups. Hypothesis 4: Either high or low social autonomy may increase reports of risktaking behavior. Those with low social autonomy may feel they have no choice but to take the risk. However, those with low social autonomy have more to lose by taking the risk, so those with high social autonomy will probably report more risk-taking behavior than those with low social autonomy. Groups with high social autonomy include European Americans, those with high socioeconomic status, those with high education, young adults, and males. Hypothesis 5: Past or present time orientation will increase reports of risk taking behavior because those individuals have less concern for the future and the consequences of their actions. Investigators expect weaker correlations between particular groups and time orientation, because factors such as low socioeconomic status may force individuals to focus on meeting current needs instead of allowing for future preparation, while other groups may have the option to prepare for the future but choose to focus on the present or past. Discussion The analysis will show which factors have the strongest effect on participants’ self-reported risk-taking behavior, but will also show how these factors might work in conjunction with or against each other. Hypotheses 1, 2, and 3 appear slightly more robust because they lack the internally confounding factors found in Hypotheses 4 and 5, but combinations with other factors may affect the magnitude and direction of influence. Should the reported risk-taking behaviors tend to vary predictably by cultural characteristics, flood risk managers will have to consider the ways culture may increase risk-taking behavior rather than attribute this tendency to irrationality or lack of information. The difficulty of assessing actual behavior requires the use of reported behavior as a proxy, which does not always match, but nevertheless should still provide useful findings for risk managers, hazards research, and cultural psychology research. References Arizona Revised Statutes (2006). Liability for emergency responses in flood areas. Title 28, Article 10, Section 910. Retrieved December 9, 2006 from http://www.azleg.state.az.us/ArizonaRevisedStatutes.asp Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A., & Tipton, S. M. (1985). Habits of the heart: Individualism and commitment in American life (pp. 142-153). New York: Harper Row. Brilly, M. & Polic, M. (2005). Public perception of flood risks, flood forecasting and mitigation. Natural Hazards and Earth System Sciences, 5, 345-355. Buckingham, A. & Saunders, P. (2004). The Survey Methods Workbook: From Design to Analysis. Cambridge: Polity Press. Douglas, M. (1992). Risk and Blame: Essays in Cultural Theory. New York: Routledge. Dow, K. & Cutter, S.L. (2000). Public orders and personal opinions: Household strategies for hurricane risk assessment. Environmental Hazards, 2, 143-155. Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 915-930). New York: McGraw-Hill. Frewer, L. (2004). The public and effective risk communication. Toxicology Letters, 149, 391-397. Iarossi, G. (2006). The Power of Survey Design. Washington, D.C.: The World Bank. Inelmen, K., Say, A.I., & Kabasakal, H. (2004). Participation lethargy in disaster preparedness organization within the framework of a Turkish CBO. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 24(10), 130-158. Jasanoff, S. (1998). The political science of risk perception. Reliability Engineering and System Safety, 59, 91-99. Kasperson, R.E. (1992). The social amplification of risk: Progress in developing an integrative framework. In S. Krimsky & D. Golding (Eds.), Social Theories of Risk. Westport: Praeger Publishers. Kitayama, S. & Markus, H.R. (1995). Culture and the self: Implications for internationalizing psychology. In N.R. Goldberger & J.B. Veroff (Eds.), The culture and psychology reader (pp. 366-385). New York: New York University Press. Lang, J.T. & Hallman, W.K. (2005). Who does the public trust? The case of genetically modified food in the United States. Risk Analysis, 25(5), 1241-1252. Markus, H.R., Mullally, P.R., & Kitayama, S. (1997). Selfways: Diversity in modes of cultural participation. In U. Neisser & D. Jopling (Eds.), The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self-understanding (pp. 13-26). New York: Cambridge University Press. Markus, H.R., Steele, C.M., & Steele, D.M. (2000). Colorblindness as a barrier to inclusion: Assimilation and nonimmigrant minorities. Daedalus, 129(4), 233-257. Masuda, J. R. & Garvin, T. (2006). Place, Culture, and the Social Amplification of Risk. Risk Analysis, 26(2), 437-454. McGuirk, P.M., & O'Neill, P. (2005). Using questionnaires in qualitative human geography. In I. Hay (Ed.), Qualitative Research Methods in Human Geography (2nd ed., pp. 147-162). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Mileti, D. S. (1995). Factors related to flood warning response. US-Italy Research Workshop on the Hydrometeorology, Impacts, and Management of Extreme Floods. Perugia, Italy. McGee, T. K. & Russell, S. (2003). "It's just a natural way of life..." an investigation of wildfire preparedness in rural Australia. Environmental Hazards, 5, 1-12. Paton, D. & Johnston, D. (2001). Disasters and communities: vulnerability, resilience and preparedness. Disaster Prevention and Management, 10(4), 270-277. Tobin, G.A., & Montz, B.E. (1997). Natural Hazards: Explanation and Integration. New York: The Guilford Press. Sáenz Segreda, L. (2003). Psychological interventions in disaster situations. In J. Zschau & A. Küppers (Eds.), Early Warning Systems for Natural Disaster Reduction (pp. 119-124). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Savadori, L., Savio, S., Nicotra, E., Rumiati, R., Finucane, M., & Slovic, P. (2004). Expert and public perception of risk from biotechnology. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 1289-1299. Scherer, C. W. & Cho, H. (2003). A social network contagion theory of risk perception. Risk Analysis, 23(2), 261-267. Shweder, R. (1995). Cultural psychology: What is it? In N.R. Goldberger and J.B. Veroff (Eds.), The Culture and Psychology Reader (pp. 41-86). New York: New York University Press. Siegrist, M. & Cvetkovich, G. (2000). Perception of hazards: The role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Analysis, 20(5), 713-719. Slovic, P. (1999). Trust, emotion, sex, politics, and science: Surveying the riskassessment battlefield. Risk Analysis, 19(4), 689-701. Slovic, P. (2000). The Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan Publications, Ltd. Wisner, B., Blaikie, P., Cannon, T., & Davis, I. (2004). At Risk: Natural Hazards, People’s Vulnerability and Disasters. New York: Routledge.