Researching classroom interaction and talk David Wray and Kristiina Kumpulainen Introduction The social and contextual nature of human learning has received great emphasis in research on learning and instruction (Anderson, et al., 1997; Greeno, 1997). Attention has been paid to the practices, processes and conditions leading to the social construction of knowledge in different learning situations (Fisher, 1993; Lemke, 1990; Palincsar, 1986; Tuyay, Jennings, & Dixon, 1995). The focus of analysis has been extended from external factors influencing learning processes and achievements to the student’s participation in and evolving interpretations of the learning activity (Grossen, 1994; Perret-Clermont, Perret, & Bell, 1991). In the midst of these changes in emphasis, new methodological questions concerning the analysis of classroom interaction and learning have arisen. Questions to which researchers have been trying to find answers are, for example: What qualitative differences can be found within and between interactive activities across a variety of learning contexts? What typically happens during classroom interaction? How can practitioners and researchers develop and strengthen their understandings about optimal learning within inherently social settings (e.g. classrooms)? This chapter will review and critique some approaches to studying and analyzing classroom interaction, and especially the discourse which characterizes this. It is based on an account of a programme of research into classroom interaction and social learning, and uses the 1 methodological decisions made during that programme as a set of pegs on which to hang discussion of methodological issues in this field. By the end of this chapter, you should have: Gained an understanding of the complexity of investigating socially shared learning practices; Considered some methodological issues emerging from researching classroom interaction and talk; Become aware of the need to move from an analysis of classroom talk to a more holistic analysis of classroom interaction; Understood the use of analytical maps that involve functional, cognitive processing and social processing analyses of classroom interaction and talk. The framework of analysis The analysis framework described here emerged as a result of a number of studies of primary-aged learners’ interactions whilst working in peer groups on various educational tasks in Finland, Greece and the United Kingdom (Fourlas & Wray, 1990; Kumpulainen & Wray, 2002). The main goal of these studies was to investigate the nature of students’ social activity, particularly verbal interaction, in different small group work learning situations. The initial development of the method concentrated on the functions of students’ verbal interaction as a basis for investigation of students’ roles as communicators and learners in teacher-centred and peer-group centred classrooms (Fourlas & Wray, 1990). This functional analysis method was later piloted, modified and applied by Kumpulainen (1994, 1996) in a study that investigated students’ social interaction during the process of collaborative writing with a word processor. Due to its fine-grained categorisations, the functional analysis method 2 was felt to give a structured overview of the nature and quality of students’ verbal interaction in this learning context. Despite the potential of the analysis method, in more recent studies of peer group learning this functional analysis of verbal interaction was found to be inadequate as a means of unravelling the complexities of socially shared learning processes. Firstly, there seemed to be a need to develop a descriptive system of analysis, which took a more holistic and multidimensional perspective on interaction. Consequently, the analysis of verbal interaction alone seemed not to be sufficient for this. Secondly, it seemed important that more attention be paid to the moment-by-moment nature of interaction in order to highlight the situated processes of meaning-making and knowledge construction within peer groups. Thirdly, it seemed important to take the individual and the group as units of analysis in order to investigate the types and forms of participation within peer groups. In the analysis method subsequently developed, the dynamics of peer group interaction were approached from three analytic dimensions. 1. The first dimension of the analysis, termed functional analysis, focussed on the character and purpose of student utterances in peer group interaction. It characterised the communicative strategies used by participants in social interaction. 2. The second dimension, cognitive processing, examined the ways in which students approached and processed learning tasks in their social interaction. It aimed at highlighting students’ working strategies and situated positions towards learning, knowledge and themselves as problem solvers. 3 3. The third dimension of the analysis, social processing, focused on the nature of the social relationships that were developed during students’ social activity. This included examining the types and forms of student participation in social interaction. Before discussing the theoretical and methodological background of these methods and highlighting the analytical framework with some empirical examples, we will firstly review some of the other analysis methods used to study peer interaction that have contributed to the present analytical approach. Investigating collaborative interaction in peer groups Peer group interaction has been studied quite extensively in different contexts in and out of school. The research objectives and methodological solutions have been diverse, being linked with the research goals and theoretical perspectives adopted by the researchers. One large group of studies focusing on peer interaction from the educational perspective is located in the systematic tradition, often referred to as process-product studies of peer interaction (e.g. Joiner, Messer, Light, & Littleton, 1995; Light, et al, 1994; King, 1989; Teasley, 1995; Tudge, 1992; Webb, Troper, & Fall, 1995). In these studies, peer interaction is analysed with coding schemes, which categorise interaction into pre-defined categories. Variables such as student achievement and performance are statistically linked to the frequency of categories as identified in the data. Usually, the development of the actual interaction process or meaning making in interaction is not the prime interest, but the focus is rather on some specific features of the interaction and their relationship to student learning or achievement. Consequently, the process of interaction over a period of time is not highlighted by such studies. The situated nature of interaction, as represented by the 4 contextual features impinging upon it, also often receives only cursory inspection. One advantage of process-product studies is that they enable the analysis of large amounts of data and use publicly verifiable criteria to make their categorisations. Probably the best known of such category systems is the Flanders Interaction Analysis (Flanders, 1970). This system has been used extensively in classroom observation studies (Wragg, 1999; Newman, 2004). It has two main uses. Firstly, it was intended to provide evidence of the differences in teaching patterns that distinguish one teaching style from another and, secondly, it has been used to try to explain differences in learning outcomes associated with different styles of teaching. The Flanders Interaction Analysis Categories (FIAC) consist of 10 categories of communication, seven used when the teacher is talking and two when a pupil is talking (and one when there is silence or confusion). An observer using the system makes timed observations, usually every three seconds, and categorises the behaviour which he/she observes at each point into one of ten categories. These categories are shown below in Table 1. 5 Table 1: Flanders' Interaction Analysis Categories (FIAC) Indirect Influence Teacher Talk Direct Influence Student Talk Accepts feeling: accepts and clarifies the feelings of the students in a non-threatening manner. Feelings may be positive or negative. Predicting and recalling feelings are included. Praises or encourages: praises or encourages student action or behaviour. Jokes that release tension, not at the expense of another individual, nodding head or saying 'uh huh?' or 'go on' are included. Accepts or uses ideas of student: clarifying, building, or developing ideas or suggestions by a student. As teacher brings more of his own ideas into play, shift to category five. Asks questions: asking a question about content or procedure with the intent that a student may answer. Lectures: giving facts or opinions about content or procedures; expressing his own ideas; asking rhetorical questions. Gives directions: directions, commands, or orders with which a student is expected to comply. Criticises or justifies authority: statements, intended to change student behaviour from non-acceptable to acceptable pattern, bawling someone out; stating why the teacher is doing what he is doing, extreme self-reference. Student talk - responses: talk by students in response to teacher. Teacher initiates the contact or solicits student statement. Student talk - initiation: talk by students which they initiate. If 'calling on' student is only to indicate who may talk next, observer must decide whether student wanted to talk. If he did, use this category. Silence or confusion: pauses, short periods of silence and periods of confusion in which communication cannot be understood by the observer. A very useful website explaining and exemplifying the Flanders system can be found at Nova South Eastern University Centre for Teaching and Learning (http://www.nova.edu/hpdtesting/ctl/fia.html). Other research traditions have produced quite different approaches to the analysis of peer group talk and learning. Barnes and Todd (1977, 1995), for example, developed an analytic system for studying peer group talk which was “grounded” in the data, as opposed to being derived from a pre-existing network of categories. Consequently, their system did try to take 6 account of the context in which peer talk was occurring. In their analysis Barnes and Todd were interested in the actual processes of interaction and the ways students developed and constructed knowledge without direct teacher presence. In analysing the dialogue amongst the groups of students, they considered types of talk and their impact upon the construction of meaning during group interactions. This demanded an analysis of both the social and cognitive functions of conversation. They developed a system describing speech acts, which was based on two levels. Level one consisted of: discourse moves (such as initiating, eliciting, extending and responding); logical processes (such as proposing a cause, advancing evidence, negating, suggesting a method, evaluating). Level two comprised: social skills (such as competition and conflict, supportive behaviour); cognitive strategies (such as setting up hypotheses, constructing new questions); reflexivity (such as monitoring one's own speech, evaluating one's own and others' performance). They identified ‘exploratory’ speech characteristics such as hesitation and changes of direction, tentativeness in voice intonation, assertions and questions made as hypotheses rather than direct assertions, invitations to modify or surmise, and self-monitoring and reflexivity. They went on to propose conditions for collaborative work amongst groups in classrooms, based on this empirical evidence. Further analysis (Barnes and Todd, 1995) provided descriptive examples of the “... four categories of collaborative moves: initiating, eliciting, extending and qualifying”. 7 Despite some limitations in the analytical system and the tools used for data collection (tape recorders only, thus losing any information from non-verbal elements of communication), Barnes and Todd’s work made an important contribution to the analysis of peer talk since it integrated ideas from discourse and conversational analysis with research on learning and instruction. Several studies have used the Barnes and Todd frameworks to enquire into classroom interaction (e.g. Edwards, 2005). Many other methods of analysis of peer group interaction, either with distinct categories or more interpretative “modes”, have been developed in the past twenty years and to review all of them here would be impossible. One important analytic approach which needs to be discussed, however, since it has contributed greatly to our understanding of children’s talk during small group learning is that developed by Fisher (1993), Mercer (1994, 1996, 2000), and Mercer & Littleton (2007). What is interesting in this approach is that it tries to investigate how children use talk to think together, thus, it uses a group as a unit of analysis, not individual children. By taking a sociocultural approach to children’s talk, it tries to show that particular ways of talking permit certain social modes of thinking. The analytic framework was derived from analyses of children’s talk during collaborative peer group learning and it includes three distinct modes of talk which characterise different ways of thinking together. These are: disputational mode, characterised by disagreement and individualised decision making; cumulative mode, consisting of positive but uncritical decision making; exploratory mode, which is seen as the most effective mode of speaking in fostering critical thinking and cognitive development (Mercer, 1996). This is characterised by constructive and critical engagements, including argumentation and hypothesis testing. 8 Theoretically, this analytical framework makes an important contribution to our increasing understanding of the different modes of talk and social thinking in peer group situations. One of the limitations of the method, though, can be found in the fact that the unit of analysis is the group - the method does not take into account individual students’ participation in the “social modes of thinking”. Consequently, the method does not highlight how the different types of social thinking are actually constructed within peer groups. Moreover, by concentrating mainly on students’ talk, the analysis may not always give a complete picture of the nature of knowledge construction in peer groups. Instead, a more dynamic approach to peer interaction is necessary, which focuses on the whole interactive context and its development, including non-verbal communication and the use of different tools, before we can unravel the processes and conditions for learning in peer group activity. Towards a new analytic method In the remainder of this chapter we will outline and discuss a descriptive system of analysis for investigating the situated dynamics of peer group interaction. Of particular importance are the mechanisms through which the social and cognitive features of peer group activity operate. The theoretical grounding of the analysis framework was informed by sociocultural and sociocognitive perspectives on interaction and learning (Cole, 1996; Resnick, Levine & Teasley, 1991; Wertsch, 1985, 1991), whereas the methodological solutions were greatly influenced by the work of Barnes and Todd (1977, 1995), Mercer (1994, 1996) as well as by interactional ethnographers (Green & Wallat, 1981; Green & Mayer, 1991; Tuyay, et al, 1995). In this method, learning is seen to take place as a result of individuals’ active participation in the social practices of their environments. Learning is viewed as an interactional process that 9 requires an understanding of language, and other semiotic tools, as both personal and social resources (Cole, 1996; Halliday & Hasan, 1989; Wells & Chang-Wells, 1992). Peer interaction is treated as a dynamic process in which language and other semiotic tools are used as instruments of communication and learning. Interaction is seen as a complex social phenomenon composed of non-verbal and social properties in addition to its verbal characteristics. Peer discourse itself is not treated as representing a person’s inner cognitive world, nor even as descriptive of an outer reality, but rather as a tool-in-action shaped by participants’ culturally-based definitions of the situation (Edwards, 1993; Edwards & Potter, 1992). The application of the method involves a microanalysis of evolving peer interactions by focusing on three analytic dimensions, namely the functions of verbal interaction, cognitive processing and social processing. Whereas the functional analysis concentrates on students’ verbal language, the analyses of students’ cognitive and social processing focus on interactive dynamics as they occur across the participants. Consequently, a group is taken as a unit of analysis. The three dimensions are treated separately for analytic purposes, although it is recognised that they are closely linked together in a complex way. In actuality the dimensions cannot be separated since each element gives meaning to all others and simultaneously obtains meaning from them. Dimension 1: Functional analysis of verbal interaction The functional analysis of students’ verbal interaction focuses on the purposes for which verbal language is used in a given context. It investigates and highlights the communicative strategies applied by individual students whilst taking part in interaction (Halliday & Hasan, 1989). Analysis of this nature often concentrates on the illocutionary force of an utterance, 10 that is, on its functional meaning (Austin, 1962; Edwards & Westgate, 1994). The functions for which students use their oral language are closely linked to the topic of discussion as well as to the individuals’ expectations and evolving interpretations of the situation shaped by the sociocultural context of the activity. The functions of language used in the course of interaction serve both intra- and interpersonal purposes. On the one hand, the purposes and intentions carried by means of verbal language serve an ideational, i.e. cognitive function. On the other hand, they serve an interpersonal function relating to the personal and social relationships between the interactors (Halliday & Hasan, 1989). The identification of language functions in peer interaction takes place on the basis of implication, that is, what a speaker can imply, suggest or mean may be different to what the speaker literally says. Consequently, the functions are not identified on the basis of specific linguistic forms. Rather, they are identified in context in terms of their retrospective and prospective effects on the actual discourse both in terms of content and form. An understanding of the functions for which students use their verbal language in interaction is greatly assisted by data gathered from direct observation, video recording and student interviews. The functions of peer interaction are the minimum units analysed in the system. They are identified on an utterance basis and defined in terms of source, purpose and situated conversational meaning. An utterance is viewed as a meaningful unit of speech, that is, a message unit. The boundary between each utterance is linguistically marked by contextual cues. Given that an utterance may serve multiple functions, more than one function can be recorded for each utterance. Examples of language functions identified in peer group interaction across learning situations are the Informative, Expositional, Reasoning, Evaluative, Interrogative, Responsive, 11 Organisational, Judgmental (agrees/disagrees), Argumentational, Compositional, Revision, Dictation, Reading aloud, Repetition, Experiential, and Affective functions. Some of these functions describe the nature of interaction more from the activity point of view (e.g. dictation, reading aloud), whereas others take a more interpretative / cognitive (e.g. informative, reasoning, evaluative) or social perspective (e.g. affective, responsive, judgmental) on the analysis of verbal interaction. However, none of the functions should be seen as reflecting only one of these dimensions. Conversely, each function in the framework is regarded as reflecting the social-cognitive-discursive actions of the participants as they verbally interact in their social activity. The language functions used in the course of joint problem solving often differ across situations and contexts, thus these functions presented in the analytic framework should not be understood as fixed, pre-defined categories. Instead, the functions must be situationally defined for each interaction situation on a post hoc basis. An example of the use of this system to analyse peer interaction is given below (for more information see Kumpulainen & Wray, 2002, p. 61). As can be seen in this example, more than one function can occur within the same language utterance. Example: Functional analysis of verbal interaction --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Context: Two girls, Elise and Madeleine, are making an imaginative story for a school magazine. They are writing collaboratively with the computer. Oral interaction Language functions Elise: Let's write that one day the boys decided to go to the grave. (OR, CRE, IM) Madeleine: Yeah. And that they took a torch and some food with them. (J, CRE, IM) 12 --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Both of the children were involved in creating writing and hence using their talk in the Compositional function. In addition, since this was not a true story they seemed to be imagining the situation and hence using the Imaginative function. Elise was also using her oral language for the Organisational function when suggesting "Let's write....". Madeleine, on the other hand, when agreeing to Elise's suggestion was using the Judgemental function. In summary, Elise used her speech for the Organisational (OR), Compositional (CRE) and Imaginative functions (IM). Madeleine, on the other hand, used her speech for the Judgemental (J), Compositional (CRE) and Imaginative functions (IM). In addition to the qualitative analysis permitted by this system, it can also be used to produce quantitative data. An example of this is the study originally carried out by Fourlas and Wray (1990) and reported in Kumpulainen & Wray (2002, p. 43-56). The aims of this study were to investigate the influence of two styles of classroom organisation, teacher-centred (a teacher directing the discussion of a whole class of students) and peer-group-centred (students organised into several peer groups to discuss materials and problems), on the ways in which students used their oral language in classroom interaction. The study was carried out in three Greek primary schools, using children aged from 7½ to 9½ years old. The research sample was eight lessons, four organised on a teacher-centred basis and four organised on a peer group basis. The data were collected and processed by means of observation, tape recordings, and written transcripts. After careful examination of the data, the functional analysis system was used to investigate the nature of children’s oral language in these two classroom organisational systems. Table 2 below show the frequency of occurrence of each language function in the talk of students in both types of lesson. 13 Table 2: Frequency (%) of occurrence of language functions in the talk of students in teacher-centred and peer group-centred lessons Language Function Teacher-centred lessons Peer group-centred (%) lessons (%) Intentional 25.1 1.5 Responsive 28.6 2.3 Reproductional 7.6 2.8 Interrogative 7.6 3.6 Experiential 3.0 1.5 Informative 10.5 19.3 Judgemental 9.4 18.2 Hypothetical 1.1 3.1 Argumentational 2.4 7.0 Affective 1.3 4.5 Organisational 2.5 16.2 Compositional 0.6 6.1 Heuristic 0.3 4.2 Imaginative 0.2 4.0 Expositional 0.0 1.1 External Thinking 0.0 4.5 Functions such as the Intentional, the Responsive, the Reproductional, the Interrogative, and the Experiential were observed to occur in higher frequencies in the teacher-centred lessons. All the other functions were found in higher frequencies in the lessons that were peer groupcentred. Of further interest was the fact that children in the peer group lessons used oral 14 language over the entire range of functions in respectable proportions. In contrast, in the teacher-centred lessons, children did not use the External Thinking or Expositional functions at all, and they used the Imaginative, Heuristic and Compositional functions quite rarely. These findings suggest that the language experiences of students in either of these two classroom organisational systems were quite different, in quality and in quantity. Dimension 2: Analysis of cognitive processing The analysis of cognitive processing examines the ways in which students approach and process learning tasks in their social activity. It aims at highlighting students’ working strategies and situated positions towards knowledge, learning and themselves as problem solvers. In the method, cognitive processes are seen as dynamic and contextual in nature, being socially constructed in students’ evolving interactions in the sociocultural context of activity. In the analytical framework we distinguished three broad modes to characterise the nature of students’ cognitive processing in peer group activity: Procedural processing refers to the routine execution of tasks without thorough planning or thinking. Ideas are not developed, rather they are cumulated or disputed without constructive judgements or criticism. The students’ activity is often product-oriented and concentrates on procedural handling of information. Interpretative or exploratory processing, on the other hand, refers to a situation during which thinking is made visible through language or other tools and the whole activity is focused on strategies, planning, and hypothesis testing. The students’ activity reflects their deep engagement and interest in the problem-solving task. 15 Off-task activity refers to a situation during which the students’ activity does not focus on the task, e.g. playing around, discussing break time activities, “absent minded” activity. It is important to recognise that these three broad analytical modes are used as heuristic devices rather than distinct categories into which students’ cognitive processing can be easily coded. Rather, the modes are reflected in different ways in different contexts and situations and, hence, require situational definitions. Example: Cognitive processing analysis --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Context: Two students, Alex and Maria, were asked to construct three-dimensional objects pictorially represented, by the use of two-dimensional shapes. Oral interaction Cognitive processing Alex Bottom ... how come speculating Maria no ... but that’s the bottom ... that’s that sort of a triangle explaining and the lid is that sort of a triangle ... they are connected ... it shows there how they are connected Alex no ... look ... this is Maria yeah ... its connected Alex wait Maria that could be created by side triangles in a way speculating Alex triangle comes here ... triangle comes here ... triangle explaining comes here and here comes a rectangle Maria yeah ... exactly and here to the roof as well 16 Alex could it go here speculating Maria this one ... this one yeah ... wait a minute ... yeah this is the testing rectangle --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Dimension 3: Analysis of social processing The analysis of social processing aims at characterising the social relationships and types of participation in peer groups. The different modes in which social processing is often constructed in peer group interaction are collaborative, tutoring, argumentative, individualistic, dominative, conflict, and confusion modes. The latter characterises interaction during which there is an obvious misunderstanding or lack of shared understanding between the children. The conflict mode reflects disagreement, usually at a social level. The dominative mode reflects the distribution of power and status in the peer group. The individualistic and dominative modes are contrasts to collaborative interaction. The individualistic mode implies that students are not developing their ideas together but rather working individually in the group. The dominative reflects an imbalance in students’ social status and power. The argumentative and tutoring modes of interaction characterise the nature of collaboration between the participants. In this sense they can be regarded as submodes of collaborative activity. The argumentative mode implies constructive interaction in which students negotiate their differing understandings in a rational way by giving judgements and justifications. This often leads to a shared understanding of the situation. The tutoring mode shows students helping and explaining for the purpose of assisting another to understand the matter at hand. In addition, collaboration includes interaction in which participants attempt to achieve a mutual understanding of the situation, ideas are 17 jointly negotiated, and discourse is coherent. In collaborative interaction participants often create bi-directional zones of proximal development assisting one another (Forman, 1989). It must be noted that, apart from the functional analysis of peer group interaction, the unit of analysis for the different modes of cognitive and social processing is not defined by distinct rules, such as an utterance basis. Instead the units of analysis for the modes of cognitive and social processing are based on their development in peer interaction on a moment-bymoment basis. In other respects the three dimensions on which the analytical framework concentrates all emerge from the data as the result of the researchers’ and, when possible, also the interactors’ interpretations of the situation. Example: Social processing analysis --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------Context: Two students, Jon and Tim, were asked to construct three-dimensional objects pictorially represented, by the use of two-dimensional shapes. Jon Oral interaction Social processing where is the large triangle? slight domination from Jon Tim oh, yeah… Joni take those away Tim hahah…what are you looking for? Tim tries to initiate collaboration Joni a kind of a triangle to the center…these tasks are a bit too difficult.. Tim how about this one then? Joni it might be…perharps two of these there… 18 Tim basically no Joni show me …hmm…let’s turn to the next exercise..let’s solve slight domination that one since it is easier…it is what one sees…hey, could from Jon it be these.. Tim do you mean these small ones? Tim tries to initiate collaboration Joni these Tim what about these big ones, I think they look big..like this. Joni I think…(indistinct) Tim I don’t know... signs of conflict starting to appear --------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------- The full categories of the analysis method are summarised in Table 3 below. 19 Table 3: Analytical framework of peer group interaction DIMENSION COGNITIVE PROCESSING SOCIAL PROCESSING DISCOURSE FUNCTIONS ANALYTICAL CATEGORIZATION DESCRIPTION exploratory/interpretative EXPO procedural/routine PROC off-task Collaborative OFF COLL Tutoring Argumentative TUTO ARGU Individualistic INDI Domination DOMI Conflict FLCI Confusion FUSI Informative Reasoning Evaluative Interrogative Responsive Organisational Judgmental Agrees Disagrees Argumentational Compositional Revision Dictation reading aloud Repetition Experiential Affectional I RE EV Q A OR critical and exploratory activity which includes planning, hypothesis testing, evaluation, and experimenting procedural on-task activity which focuses on handling, organising, and executing the task without reflective analysis activity not related to the task joint activity characterized by equal participation and meaning making student helping and assisting another student students are faced with cognitive/social conflicts which are resolved and justified in a rational way student(s) working on individual tasks with no sharing or joint meaning making student dominating the work, unequal participation social or academic conflicts which are often left unresolved lack of shared understanding, student(s) do not understand the task or each other, often includes silent episodes providing information reasoning in language evaluating work or action asking questions answering questions organising or/and controlling behavior Ja Jd AR CR RV DI RE RP E AF expressing agreement or disagreement justifying information, opinions or actions creating text revising text dictating reading text repeating spoken language expressing personal experiences expressing feelings 20 Analytical maps In our analysis, the dynamics of peer group interaction are illustrated with the help of analytical maps, which have been created for each peer group under investigation. The product of the analysis is a series of situation-specific analytical maps that describe the sequential evolution of peer group interactions as they are constructed by students interacting with and acting upon each other’s messages. In addition to highlighting the dynamics of peer interaction, these maps show the element of time in the students’ activity as well as some contextual information necessary for the interpretation of the social activity in question. Although a structural map is always a simplification, it gives a coherent and temporal picture of a complex situation making comparisons across educational contexts, peer groups and students possible. Moreover, the structural maps help one return easily to the original data to check the validity of interpretation. In addition, when presenting extracts from the data, one is able to investigate the co-text, that is, the data context to which the extract belongs. 39 Example: Interactive dynamics between Joni and Gary SESSION: 1.1.2 Mathematics STUDENTS: Gary and Jon WORKING TIME: 11:05 – 11:30 TIME 11:16 PARTICIPATION 147 JON TRANSCRIBED PEER LANGUAGE COGNITIVE SOCIAL CONTEXTUAL INTERACTION FUNCTIONS PROCESSING PROCESSING NOTES where is the large asking for looking for a slight domination students are triangle? information Q(I) face from Jon’s side trying to find correct faces to 148 GARY oh, yeah… affectional Gary is holding construct the utterance (AF) the construction geometrical students have object 149 JON take those away organising (OR) already made 150 GARY hahah…what are you affectional Gary initiates looking for? utterance (AF) collaboration and asking for information Q(I) 151 152 JON GARY a kind of a triangle to answering A(I) the center…these tasks and evaluating are a bit too difficult.. the task (EV) how about this one reasoning in a then? question form explaining speculating Q(RS) 153 JON it might be…perharps reasoning (RS) two of these there… 11:17 154 GARY basically no reasoning (RS) 155 JON show me …hmm…let’s organising (OR), organising slight domination turn to the next evaluating (EV), from Jon’s side exercise..let’s solve that and reasoning one since it is easier…it (RS) is what one sees…hey, could it be these.. 40 working, speculating 156 GARY do you mean these reasoning in a Gary initiates small ones? question form collaboration Q(RS) 157 JON these answering A(RS) 158 GARY what about these big reasoning (RS) speculating ones, I think they look big..like this. 159 JON I think…(indistinct) - 160 GARY I don’t know... informing (I) signs of “a free rider effect“ starting to appear 161 JON that’s a bit too thick that reasoning (RS) comparing cards one there..that’s there…rather small.... 162 GARY that’s not it..heheheh… reasoning (RS) 163 JON its all the same really reasoning (RS) 164 GARY let’s try both organising (OR) 165 JON let’s take…this is there organising (OR), organising below, isn’t it?..bigger reasoning (RS), one..hold it and organising working (OR) 166 GARY no, I don’t want to disagrees (Jd) social conflict Conclusion In this chapter we have explored some of the methodological issues involved in a study of classroom interaction. Our focus here has been particularly upon interactions between learners as they engage with the material of learning. A number of key methodological points have been aired here, and the analysis system we have devised has been presented as one way (among many possible such ways) of 41 responding to some of the difficulties inherent in trying to understand what are inevitably very complex processes. It should be stressed, however, that, although the past 30 years has seen substantial growth in approaches to and study of classroom interactions in a range of contexts, we are still inevitably in the infancy of our understanding of what goes on inside the ‘black box’ of classrooms, or learning situations. Key Terms Classroom interaction: all the ways in which participants in classrooms interact with one another, including oral interaction, non-verbal interaction and shared activity. Category analyses: systems for analysing classroom interaction which approach the phenomena to be analysed with pre-determined categories into which interactive behaviour is assumed to fit. Grounded analyses: attempts to interpret classroom interaction which do not use predetermined categories but derive their analyses from the data itself. Frequently Asked Questions Q1: What are the weaknesses of category analysis systems for exploring classroom interaction? A1: Systems which approach real life situations such as classrooms with predetermined categories run into the problem of what do they do when what is observed does not quite fit these categories. The danger is that important aspects of 42 classroom interaction are either ignored because they do not fit the categories, or misinterpreted by being squeezed to make them fit these categories. Q2: What are the weaknesses of grounded approaches to analysing classroom interaction? A2: Analysis which is ‘grounded’ in the data is, almost by definition, atheoretical in its approach. Lacking a theoretical framework within which to interpret observed phenomena means that researchers are at risk of producing little more than series of unconnected observations. Interpretation, in any research activity, relies upon a theoretical framework. Q3: Why can classroom interaction not be studied simply by analysing the talk that takes place within classrooms? A3: The problem is that talk does not in itself convey the entire range of meanings which interaction participants bring to and take from their interactions. These meanings are influenced by such things as context, and non-verbal interactions. Classroom interaction research needs to have some way of taking account of such non-verbal influences if it is to produce a full picture of the meanings being created. References Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., & Simon, H. A. (1997). Situative versus cognitive perspectives: Form versus substance. Educational Researcher, 26 (1), 18-21. Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Clarendon Press. 43 Barnes, D. & Todd, F. (1977). Communication and learning in small groups. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul Ltd. Barnes, D. & Todd, F. (1995). Communication and learning revisited, making meaning through talk. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook Publishers Heinemann. Centre for Teaching and Learning (2007) Flanders Interaction Analysis Fort Lauderdale, Florida: Nova Southeastern University http://www.nova.edu/hpdtesting/ctl/fia.html Accessed June 19th, 2008 Cole, M. (1996). Culture in mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Edwards, D. & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Edwards, D. & Westgate, D. (1994). Investigating classroom talk (2nd ed.). Basingstoke: Falmer Press. Edwards, D. (1993). Concepts, memory, and the organization of pedagogic discourse: A case study. Educational Research, 19, 205-225. Edwards, J. (2005) “Exploratory talk in peer groups: exploring the zone of proximal development”. In Fourth Congress of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education, Sant Feliu de Guíxols, Spain. Spain, ESRME, 44 http://cerme4.crm.es/Papers%20definitius/8/JulieAnnEdwards.pdf Accessed June 19th, 2008 Fisher, E. (1993). Distinctive features of pupil-pupil classroom talk and their relationship to learning: How discursive exploration might be encouraged. Language and Education, 7, 239-257. Flanders, N. (1970) Analyzing Teacher Behavior. Addison-Wesley: Reading, Mass. Forman, E. (1989). The role of peer interaction in the social construction of mathematical knowledge. International Journal of Educational Research, 13, 55-70. Fourlas, G. & Wray, D. (1990). Children's oral language: A comparison of two classroom organisational systems. In D. Wray (Ed.), Emerging partnerships, Current research in language and literacy (pp.76-86). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters Ltd. Green, J. & Mayer, L. (1991). The embeddedness of reading in classrom life: Reading as a situated process. In C. Baker & A. Luke (Eds.), Toward a critical sociology of reading pedagogy (pp. 141-160). Amsterdam: Benjamins Publishing Company. Green, J. & Wallat, C. (1981). Mapping instructional conversations - a sociolinguistic ethnography. In J. Green & C. Wallat (Eds.), Ethnography and language in educational settings (pp.161-228). Norwood: Ablex Publishing Corporation. 45 Greeno, J. G. (1997). On claims that answer the wrong questions. Educational Researcher, 26, 5-17. Grossen, M. (1994). Theoretical and methodological consequences of a change in the unit of analysis for the study of peer interactions in a problem solving situation. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 11, 159-173. Halliday, M.A.K. & Hasan, R. (1989). Language, context, and text. London: Oxford University press. Joiner, R, Messer, D., Light, P. & Littleton, K. (1995, August). Investigating the relationship between communicative style and productive interaction. A paper presented at the 6th European Conference for Research on Learning and Instruction, Nijmegen, the Netherlands. King, A. (1989). Verbal interaction and problem-solving within computer assisted cooperative learning groups. Educational Computing Research, 5 (1), 1-15. Kumpulainen, K. & Wray, D. (2002) Classroom Interaction and Social Learning London: RoutledgeFalmer Kumpulainen, K. (1994). The nature of children's oral language interactions during collaborative writing experience at the computer. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Exeter: University of Exeter. 46 Kumpulainen, K. (1996). The nature of peer interaction in the social context created by the use of word processors. Learning and Instruction, 6, 243-261. Lemke, J. L. (1990). Talking science: Language, learning and values. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Light, P., Littleton, K., Messer, D., & Joiner, R. (1994). Social and communicative processes in computer-based problem solving. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(1), 93-109. Mercer, N. & Littleton, K. (2007) Dialogue and the Development of Children's Thinking: a sociocultural approach. London: Routledge. Mercer, N. (1994). The quality of talk in children's joint activity at the computer, Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 10, 24-32. Mercer, N. (1996). The quality of talk in children's collaborative activity in the classroom. Learning and Instruction, 6, 359-377. Mercer, N. (2000) Words and Minds: How We Use Language to Think Together. London: Routledge. Newman, M. (2004) Problem Based Learning: An exploration of the method and evaluation of its effectiveness in a continuing nursing education programme. London: School of Lifelong Learning & Education, Middlesex University. 47 Palincsar, A. S. (1986). The role of dialogue in providing scaffolded instruction. Educational Psychologist, 21, 73-98. Perret-Clermont, A-N., Perret, J-F., & Bell, N. (1991). The social construction of meaning and cognitive activity in elementary school children. In L.B. Resnick, J.M. Levine & S.D.Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 41-62). Washington: American Psychological Association. Resnick, L.B., Levine, J.M. & Teasley, S.D. (Eds.). (1991). Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington: American Psychological Association. Teasley, S. (1995). The role of talk in children's peer collaborations. Developmental Psychology, 31, 207-220. Tudge, J. (1992). Processes and consequences of peer collaboration. A Vygotskian analysis. Child Development, 63, 1364-1379. Tuyay, S., Jennings, L., & Dixon, C. (1995). Classroom discourse and opportunities to learn: An ethnographic study of knowledge construction in a bilingual third grade classroom. In Durán, R. (Ed.), Discourse processes (literacy among latinos: Focus on school contexts), 19, 75-110. Webb, N. M., Troper, J., & Fall, J.R. (1995). Constructive activity and learning in collaborative small groups. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 406-423. 48 Wells, G. & Chang-Wells, G. L. (1992). Constructing knowledge together: Classrooms as centers of inquiry and literacy. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Wertsch, J. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wertsch, J. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wragg, E.C. (1999) An introduction to classroom observation. London: Routledge. 49