Electronic Supplementary material 1. Detailed Methods The accuracy of the data obtained was constrained by several features of the biology of the study species. These generate a number of difficulties and require assumptions that need careful explanation. Age is not always known precisely. Early in the study the precise age of many birds is unknown, as they were already adult when first encountered. While by 1991 it was possible to classify these as four years or older (eg Fig 1c), it was not possible to include them in analyses treating age as a continuous variable (Fig 1a). This problem had largely disappeared by 1997, as many old birds died in 1996. Female recruitment cannot be measured. All females undertake at least one dispersal movement before acquiring a breeding vacancy (Cockburn et al. 2003). While some birds disperse within the study area, many move out of the area and sometimes travel several kilometres to gain a vacancy. Hence, we can measure the number of sons recruited during each year of selection, but not recruitment of daughters, as we cannot reliably distinguish between female death and dispersal. Hence, in this case we use recruitment of sons as the fecundity measure in the calculations of fitness. This limitation will be occur commonly in field studies, and is exacerbated because the trait under sexual selection cannot be scored in females. Coulson et al. (Coulson et al. 2006) report a comparable example in their calculation of selection in Soay sheep. The assumption that males are an adequate measure of fecundity would be particularly problematic if there were strong facultative adjustment of the offspring sex ratio in response to the same environmental features that are influencing sexual selection gradients. That difficulty does not arise in M. cyaneus because there is no facultative adjustment of the sex ratio in this species (Cockburn & Double 2007). Male reproductive success if imperfectly known. Our population is not closed. Because males can achieve extra-group success some territories from their own (Double & Cockburn 2000), some males recruited into the study area are sired by males from outside the study area, and some of the males in the study area presumably also sire males elsewhere. We have tried to minimise this problem by excluding marginal territories that were not largely surrounded by neighbours, but this is not a complete solution, and hence our data must be viewed as a partial estimate of male success. Our assumption that we can proceed despite this difficulty is supported by the strong patterns we have uncovered, and the observation that males on or towards the edge of the study area attained some of the highest levels of reproductive success in this study. The precise date that some males acquire nuptial plumage is unknown. It is easy to determine precisely the moult date of a male that undergoes a conventional moult from eclipse plumage to nuptial plumage and vice versa. However, the moult date of some males is more difficult to record. These fall into five classes: 1. Some males use the post-breeding moult to renew their nuptial plumage, and hence never exhibit eclipse plumage. The completion of this process is difficult to determine, so we have assumed a value of 8 weeks, earlier than any male that gained at least some eclipse feathers (Fig 1a). 2. Similarly, some males (almost always males in their first year) initiate the moult but do not complete it, retaining a variable amount of eclipse plumage. We cannot always tell when the moult has stopped, so we have arbitrarily given these males a value of 48 weeks, after the last date for conventional acquisition of males in their first year (Fig 1a). 3. A much smaller number of males never moult. There is some evidence that this failure is pathological rather than part of a continuum (Mulder & Cockburn 1993), but we have assigned these males a value of 52 weeks. 4. Some males die during the process of moult, particularly during the Pre-breeding phase when mortality is generally high (Fig. 2c). We have assigned them a moult completion date estimated from males that survived but were in a similar stage of moult when the original bird died. 5. Most difficult, some birds died without initiating moult. Failure to include these birds would distort the calculation of pt(i), as by definition these birds biased towards the lowest possible level of fitness in the season in which they die, and the calculation is sensitive to the number of males included in the sample (Coulson et al. 2006). We therefore assigned these birds a moult date and class in the following way. a. for the birds that were three years or older, we assigned moult date based on the observation that older birds typically retain approximately the same moult rank within their cohort from year-to-year. We therefore calculated their percentile position within their cohort, and assigned a moult date accordingly. b. this approach was not appropriate for one and two year old males, as we had no prior information on one year old males, and moult is not correlated over the first two years of life. However, these cohorts show much less variance than the other age classes (95% of individuals generally complete moult within a three week period in any year), so we randomly assigned them a value based on the distribution for their cohort in that season. This is obviously not entirely satisfactory, but we experimented with alternative methods of assignment and the exclusion of these birds, and obtained qualitatively similar results in each case. In addition, the measurement we ultimately used to look at variation in reproductive monopolisation is completely insensitive to these assumptions. References Cockburn, A. & Double, M. C. 2007 Cooperatively breeding superb fairy-wrens show no facultative manipulation of offspring sex ratio despite plausible benefits. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, in review. Cockburn, A., Osmond, H. L., Mulder, R. A., Green, D. J. & Double, M. C. 2003 Divorce, dispersal and incest avoidance in the cooperatively breeding superb fairy-wren Malurus cyaneus. Journal of Animal Ecology 72, 189-202. Coulson, T., Benton, T. G., Lundberg, P., Dall, S. R. X., Kendall, B. E. & Gaillard, J.M. 2006 Estimating individual contributions to population growth: evolutionary fitness in ecological time. Proceedings of the Royal Society BBiological Sciences 273, 547-555. Double, M. & Cockburn, A. 2000 Pre-dawn infidelity: females control extra-pair mating in superb fairy-wrens. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 267, 465-470. Mulder, R. A. & Cockburn, A. 1993 Sperm competition and the reproductive anatomy of male superb fairy-Wrens. Auk 110, 588-593.