Antagonistic managers, careless workers and extraverted salespeople: An examination of personality in occupational choice By Roger Hama, P.N Junankarab and Robert Wellsa* Revised February 2011 a School of Economics and Finance, University of Western Sydney. Locked Bag 1797 Penrith South DC NSW 1797 , Australia b School of Economics, University of New South Wales and IZA. Bonn, Germany * Corresponding Author. Email: R.Wells@uws.edu.au, Phone: +61 405 577 717 Fax: + 61 2 9685 9105 1 Antagonistic managers, careless labourers and extraverted salespeople: An examination of personality in occupational choice Abstract This paper is an econometric investigation of the choice of individuals between a number of occupation groupings utilising an extensive array of conditioning variables measuring a variety of aspects of individual heterogeneity. Whilst the model contains the main theory of occupational choice, human capital theory, it also tests dynasty hysteresis through parental status variables. The focus is an examination of the relationship between choice and personality with the inclusion of psychometrically derived personality variables. The empirical model of occupational choice is a multinomial logit estimated using the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey data. Human capital variables are found to exhibit strong credentialism effects. Parental status has a small and limited effect on occupation outcomes indicative of only some small dynasty hysteresis. On the other hand, personality effects are found to be significant, relatively large and persistent across all occupations. Further, the strength of these personality effects are such that they can in many instances rival that of various education credentials. These personality effects include but are not limited to: managers being less agreeable and more antagonistic; labourers being less conscientiousness; and sales people being more extraverted. JEL Classification: J24; J62; C25 Keywords: Occupational choice; personality traits; credentialism; dynasty hysteresis 2 Introduction1 Heckman and Rubinstein (2001) explicitly refer to the difficulty of analysing the implications for labour market outcomes of non-cognitive skills, because of measurement problems, “[M]any different personality and motivational traits are lumped into the category of non-cognitive skills” (p. 145) and liken non-cognitive skills to the “dark matter” of astrophysics. Nevertheless they cogently argue that “success in life” is determined by non-cognitive traits as well as cognitive skills, but point out that: “too little is understood about ...the separate effects of all these diverse traits currently subsumed under the rubric of noncognitive skills. What we currently know ... suggests that further research on the topic is likely to be very fruitful.” Heckman and Rubinstein (2001, p. 149) This paper uses well recognised measures of psychological traits, from the “Five Factor Model” (FFM) of empirical psychology. These traits are contained within the category non-cognitive skills to examine the effects of some psychological characteristics on occupational outcomes. 1 This paper uses unit record data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey. The HILDA Project was initiated and is funded by the Australian Government Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs (FaHCSIA) and is managed by the Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (MIAESR). The findings and views reported in this paper, however, are those of the authors and should not be attributed to either FaHCSIA or the MIAESR We are grateful for very helpful comments provided by anonymous referees of this journal. The data used in this paper was extracted using the Add-On package PanelWhiz for Stata®. PanelWhiz (http://www.PanelWhiz.eu) written by Dr. John P. Haisken-DeNew (john@PanelWhiz.eu). See HaiskenDeNew and Hahn (2006) for details. The PanelWhiz generated DO file to retrieve the data used here is available from me the authors on request. Any data or computational errors in the paper are due to the authors. 3 In economics it is often thought that differences between individual characteristics, such as preferences, are far too numerous, and are not a sensible area, for the scientific examination of economic behaviour (Caplan 2003). This is not the case in psychology which has long sought to find and classify the differences between individuals’ consistent and enduring behavioural characteristics; these are known as personality traits. The focus of this paper is an examination of the influence of personality traits on occupational choice by way of a standard multinomial logit model. The categorical variable, occupation as measured by the Australian and New Zealand Standard Classification of Occupations (ANZSCO) is used as the indicator of occupational outcomes. The eight single digit occupations are used in this study. Whilst this may seem a high level of aggregation it provides a larger number of categories relative to other studies. Further, the use of two digit categories leads to computational problems, through narrower cell widths with subsequent reduced variation leading to identification issues. Whilst the focus of the paper is the conditioning of occupational outcomes by personality measures, other sets of important conditioning variables are included in the model. In terms of human capital, following Heckman et al. (2003) and Leigh (2008) the model uses a series of binary variables associated with educational qualifications and credentials. Parents’ characteristics by way of their occupational achievements and the socio-economic index, AUSEI06, developed at the Australian National University (McMillan et al., 2009) are included to capture dynastic hysteresis, The mechanism underlying dynasty hysteresis is subject to debate. Laband and Lentz (1983) argue that dynasty hysteresis is caused by human capital transfer which is more predominant in some occupations than others. Fan (2008) argues that dynasty hysteresis may be transmitted by religion and its associated characteristics. Akerlof (1997) puts forward a theory of social distance in which individuals have both a desire to excel and a desire to conform to their social group. Other potential mechanisms include the intergenerational transfer of preferences (Doepke & Zilibotti 2005) and of non-cognitive factors (Bowles & Gintis 2002). Irrespective of the transfer mechanism, dynasty is an important phenomenon to be examined in occupational choice as it has a crucial effect on the ability of an individual to have freedom to pursue any occupation they desire and the extent to 4 which the other determinants influence that choice (Bradley 1991; Mazumder 2005; Bjerk 2007). Previous analyses of occupational choice have included parental variables to attempt to control for dynasty hysteresis (Robertson & Symons 1990; Bradley 1991; Tsukahara 2007). Further controls are also used in terms of demographic; personal; industry; and location variables. Whilst the psychological characteristics used in this empirical investigation of occupational outcomes is only a subset of all those traits encompassed in non cognitive traits, this investigation will help shed some light on Heckman and Rubinstein’s “dark matter” of labour market outcomes. The next section deals at length with the personality measures used to examine occupation outcomes and psychological traits. Psychometrically Derived Personality Traits and Occupational Choice McCrae and Costa (2003) provide a summary of the literature on personality psychology to date and argue, along with others (Digman 1990; Goldberg 1993; Caplan 2003; Terracciano et al. 2006; Cole 2007; Borghans et al. 2008a; Costa Jr & McCrae 2008; Terracciano et al. 2010), that the general consensus model for personality traits is that of the five factor model (FFM). The FFM states that personality traits are “dimensions of individual differences in tendencies to show consistent patterns of thoughts, feeling, and actions” (McCrae & Costa 2003, p. 25). These personality trait dimensions are typically viewed as broad level dispositions or propensities, that is they are not the sole determinant of behaviour but should be viewed as a contributing factor in a ceteris paribus context. Caplan (2003) emphasizes that, whilst personality traits are an important aspect of the examination of economic activity, the incentives of economics should not be neglected. Personality traits consist of broad dispositional traits that can influence behaviour, this does not mean that individuals with the same level of personality traits are identical as the traits can manifest themselves in different specific mannerisms, or characteristics adaptations, as stated by McCrae and Costa (2003). It should be noted that these personality traits consist of subscales representing more specific behavioural 5 tendencies and further these subscales are in turn formed by facets which represent increasingly more specific behavioural tendencies. Both facets and subscales covary amongst each other and consequently form subscales and factors respectively, with factors representing personality traits at the broadest and most consistent level. Subscales might well have stronger influences on occupational outcomes. However, they are not available in the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) panel used here, Further even if they were available, there are potential problems with their use. Because of the high covariance between subscales within a factor there is a strong potential for collinearity. Further, even though the occupational categorisation used here is coarse, there are still a significant number of different occupations. Increasing the number of personality measures may well lead to a low variation in some categorical cells and identification problems. These five traits consist of Openness to experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness and Neuroticism2. Each of these traits have a corresponding negativeclosed to experience, carelessness, introversion, antagonism and emotional stabilitywhich correspond to low scores of the corresponding dimensions. It should be noted that here emotional stability is used in the place of neuroticism to preserve the original arrangement of the data. Openness to experience can be defined as a trait associated with being accepting of new ideas and alternative points of view, appreciation of new concepts, imaginative and creative and generally inquisitive and curious. Conscientiousness is the trait that is associated with diligence, self discipline, punctuality, organised and general competence3. Extraversion is the trait associated with being talkative, friendly, energetic and outgoing. This trait has been previously examined in economics by Krueger and Schkade (2008) and found to influence occupational outcomes and wages. Agreeableness can be described as the tendency to be generous, warm, altruistic, tender, and complaisant and tend to get along with others. Individuals with a lack of agreeableness can conversely be aggressive, tough and quite adversarial. 2 3 This can be remembered with the mnemonic device OCEAN which corresponds to the order stated above. This trait in a precursor to the FFM is referred to as ‘work’ (Goldberg,1993) 6 Neuroticism is the last trait in the FFM, it is often commonly referred to by its negative of emotional stability. In this paper both terms are used interchangeably depending on contextual appropriateness. However, the estimated empirical model uses the positive counterpart, emotional stability, as the conditioning variable. Neuroticism can be described as the tendency of experiencing negative emotions more frequently and intensely. Neuroticism can be described as a trait associated with anxiety, worry, paranoia and stress. Traits associated with these dimensions of personality can been seen in Table 1 from McCrae and Costa (2003). 7 Table 1: Characteristics of higher and lower scores in the personality traits of the Five Factor Model Personality trait Low Scorer Higher scorer Openness Favours conservative values Values intellectual matters Judges in conventional terms Rebellious, nonconforming Uncomfortable with complexities Unusual thought processes Moralistic Introspective Eroticizes situations Behaves ethically Unable to delay gratification Dependable, responsible Self-indulgent Productive Engages in fantasy, daydreams Has high aspiration levels Emotionally bland Talkative Avoids close relationship Gregarious Overcontrol of impulses Socially poised Submissive Behaves assertively Critical, sceptical Sympathetic, considerate Shows condescending behaviour Warm, Compassionate Tries to push limits Arouses liking Expresses hostility directly Behaves in a giving away Calm, relaxed Thin-skinned Satisfied with self Basically anxious Clear-cut personality Irritable Prides self on objectivity Guilt-Prone Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Neuroticism Source: McCrae and Costa, 2003 In psychology, research suggests that personality traits influence occupational outcomes through choices made by individuals and the requirements of employers in occupations (Barrick & Mount 1991; Larson et al. 2002; Barrick et al. 2003; Ozer & Benet-Martinez 2006; Furnham & Fudge 2008). Barrick and Mount (1991) put forward a number of hypotheses in their meta-analysis of the effect of the FFM in regard to how personality variables influence an individual’s productivity and consequently their occupational achievement. Conscientiousness is argued to carry a ubiquitous positive effect on labour market outcomes as individuals who possess this trait are often hardworking, productive, punctual, organised and accepting of responsibility. It can be argued that openness has an effect on the ability of individuals to be trained, being embracing of new ideas and 8 consequently can be positively valued. Emotional stability is argued to influence an individual’s occupational achievement via the individual being less likely to experience negative emotions and they can take on more stressful, non-routine and risky tasks. Barrick and Mount (1991) posit that agreeableness may be valued in some occupations as friendliness is desirable for interpersonal interaction; however, it has been argued that antagonistic personality may be required for certain tasks by both the supplier of and demander for labour (McCrae & Costa 2003; Borghans et al. 2008b). In addition, it has been argued that a tendency to Machiavellian behaviour, that is a desire to manipulate people for benefit, is positively valued in some labour markets (Bowles et al. 2001a; Bowles et al. 2001b; Wakefield 2008). Extraversion may be valued in occupations that involve a large amount of interaction with others, as extraverted individuals would gain greater utility from these interactions (Barrick & Mount 1991; Krueger & Schkade 2008). The results of Barrick and Mount’s (1991) meta-analysis show that conscientiousness and openness behave as predicted, extraversion is valued, in terms of job productivity, in both social jobs and training while agreeableness and neuroticism are observed to have no effect on labour market outcomes. They argue that the lack of an observed effect with regard to neuroticism may be due to a sample selection bias as you require a minimum amount of emotional stability -the negative of neuroticism- to achieve a position in the labour market. There has been a neglect of the analysis of non cognitive skills on life outcomes in economics. The limited literature sees certain non cognitive traits as being valued by employers, Heckman and Rubinstein (2001) and Heckman et al (2006). Bowles et al. (2001a) argue that non cognitive traits as well as cognitive skills are rewarded on the labour market. Nyhus and Pons (2005) estimate earnings functions incorporating the FFM personality measures. Borghans et al. (2008a) argue that personality traits, as measured by the FFM condition many economic outcomes for individuals, including occupation. Moreover, they discuss at length how best to incorporate personality into conventional economic models. It could be argued that personality traits influence individuals’ preferences towards various different occupations, that is, they are supply side forces. Alternatively, personality traits will be valued by employers and therefore 9 they may be modelled as goods demanded by employers and will appear on the supply side as a constraint to occupational choice. The empirical model that is proposed here is not structural, recognising that outcomes are determined by supply and demand forces and as such it is a reduced form which simply assesses how personality acts to sort individuals into occupations. The stability of personality traits is an important issue in the literature. McCrae and Costa (2003) present a comprehensive review of a large body of work which suggests that from the age of thirty personality, as defined above, is stable within the broad definition of the FFM and some stability can even be seen in younger individuals. This study includes a review of longitudinal research (pp. 108-115) which provide strong evidence for stability citing a study reporting 3 year retest correlations for both men and woman in two groups aged 25-56 and 57-84 ranging from 0.55 to 0.87. Alternatively, Roberts and DelVecchio (2000) in an influential meta analysis of a wide mixture of studies find stability peaks at 50 to 70 years of age. These studies are based on group retest correlations and not individual retest data and in their conclusion the authors state that it would be inappropriate to make inferences about individual consistency from their results: “it would be premature to render a final judgement concerning whether and when personality traits are fixed” (Roberts and DelVecchio, 2000, p. 19). Further research supports the claim for earlier stability (Terracciano et al. 2006; Terracciano et al. 2010), where the latter uses individual rather than grouped data. Even in young adults some retest correlations are as high as 0.3. Finally, Viinikainen et al. (2010), using a Finnish data set, find correlations between childhood personality scores and adult labour market outcomes 35 years later. Whilst the personality scores pre-date the FFM the personality measures considerably overlap the FFM and these correlations are indicative of long run stability in broad personality factors. Most studies find that stability decreases with time between retest scores, that is the longer the period between retest, the lower are retest correlations. Personality traits, like cognitive skills are determined by “nature and nurture”, Cuhna and Heckman (2008). They are not immutably fixed and can change and can be changed by 10 an individual’s occupation (Mueller & Plug 2006), (Groves 2005), (Heckman et al. 2006), (Cole 2007), (Semykina & Linz 2007), (Cunha & Heckman 2008). Yet, this potential for the endogeneity of personality traits, measured by the FFM, in modelling occupational outcomes is not necessarily large. “[c]hange may be more difficult later in the life cycle, change may be more enduring for some (such as more emotionally stable individuals) than for others, change may require persistent and consistent environmental pressure (as opposed to transient pressure from short-term interventions), and there are powerful forces for stability (such as genes and habit) which make change difficult.” (Borghans et al., 2008, p. 1021) Whilst occupation might influence traits, that influence is one small part of adulthood environmental influences which may themselves be marginal relative to the core influences of environment in childhood and early adulthood and genetic endowment. Further, occupation may not necessarily be seen as some persistent environmental force in the HILDA sample. Over the seven annual waves used in this analysis, only 54% of the sample recorded as “Managers” in Wave one were recorded as Managers in Wave seven, Whilst the percentages for “Professional, “Technician”, “service”, “Clerical” and “Operator” were somewhat higher at 75%, 62%, 63%, 63%, and 61% respectively, these are still indicative of substantial movement between occupations. “Sales” and “Labourer” experienced higher turnover with only 37% and 40% retention over the seven Waves. Given the marginal nature of occupation determining change in the FFM and the fact that there is considerable movement amongst occupations strong reverse causality is highly unlikely. The Data The data source is the Household Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) longitudinal data set. The HILDA survey is an approximately one in one thousand sample 11 of the Australian population consisting of 19,914 individuals in 7,682 households in sample Wave one (Watson 2009). The HILDA dataset contains an extremely rich set of variables which capture details on a large number of individual characteristics including the focus of this study occupational and labour market outcomes, education, parental status, and standard demographic variables. Particularly, it contains a comprehensive set of personality measures which conform with the FFM. These were derived, following the well established procedures of the FFM, using a factor analysis of underlying variables. The sample used to estimate the model consists of pooled data from Waves 1 to Wave 7 (years 2001 to 2007) of the HILDA panel. Individuals were included in the sample if they had a complete record of all the variables included in the model for any wave. Occupational status is determined by recorded primary occupation; as such full time students are excluded from the sample. Occupations in the HILDA data are coded into the ANZSCO system. The HILDA data in its general release provide the ANZSCO coding at both the one digit and two digit levels. This research uses the one digit ANZSCO coding which consists of eight mutually exclusive and exhaustive occupation outcomes. The eight one digit categories are Managers, Professionals, Technicians and tradespersons, Community and personal service workers, Clerical and administrative workers, Sales workers, machine operators and drivers, and Labourers. Each of these categories describes a set of skill specialisations that are relatively homogeneous when compared with other groups. These categories give a good representation of occupations based on the view that an occupation is a set of relatively homogenous tasks. A brief description of each of these eight occupational categories and examples of jobs that fall into these categories can be seen in Table 2. 12 Table 2: Definitions and examples of the ANZSCO coding of occupations. Occupation (abbreviations underlined) Description of tasks Examples Managers Plan, organize, coordinate and review various General manager, legislators, farm manager, finance operations managers, retail manager, & customer service manager analytical, conceptual and creative tasks Actors, airline pilots, Engineers, Physical and social require the application of a body of Scientists, Medical professionals, lawyers, IT professionals, knowledge and educators Skilled tasks requiring broad or specific Scientific Technicians, motor mechanics, construction knowledge. workers, chefs, florists and hairdressers Provision of service to either individuals Paramedics, child carers, baristas, waiters, security officer, personally or the community as a whole that military personnel, driving instructors and sportspersons. Professionals Technicians and tradespersons Community and personal service workers often requires interaction with others. Clerical and administrative workers Sales workers Machinery operators and drivers Labourers Organize, store, manipulate and retrieve Office managers, data entry clerks, receptionists, payroll information clerk, mail clerks and proofreader Sell goods and services and provide sales Sales representative, insurance brokers, retail supervisors, support checkout operator, models and telemarketers, Operate machinery, plant vehicles and other Industrial spraypainter, sewing machinist, motion picture equipment projectionist, crane operator, forklift driver, and train driver Repetitive and routine tasks that may include Cleaners, steel fixer, product assembler, packer, slaughter, the use of hand or power tools. farm worker, kitchen hand, freight handler and handypersons While these groups are relatively homogeneous in terms of tasks, some occupations can be expected to have differing desired characteristics; for example, security workers are in the same category as baristas. Analysis of the two digit level of 43 categories would capture some of the heterogeneity in the broader class. However, analysis at the two digit level leads to difficulties given: the necessary number of parameters; a low frequency of observations in particular states; and the possible violation of necessary conditions for the model’s adequacy. For these reasons the current work is limited to occupational categories at the one digit level. As well as personality trait measures, the HILDA data give very detailed listings of variables that theory elects as important in conditioning occupational outcomes. Along with the standard demographic variables such as age, gender, location, marital status, etc., the HILDA survey data set contains data on the educational attainment of an individual in terms of qualifications. In order to capture occupation specific human capital and to 13 introduce non-linearity into the influence of education on labour market outcomes, the analysis uses a series of binary variables based on educational achievement. Two sets of variables are used to capture parental status. The first is the labour market success of an individual’s parents. This is measured by a set of binary variables which reflect the occupation of an individual’s mother and father. Of particular interest is the probability of an individual being in the same occupation as their parent, this would provide support for the phenomenon of dynasty hysteresis. The second is the AUSEI06 measure of social status. This is an index ranging between zero and one hundred, to one decimal place, which incorporates a variety of education, occupation, income and other demographic effects that reflect an individual’s social success (Jones & McMillan 2001; McMillan et al. 2008). The AUSEI06 allows for a continuous version of parental status to be used in order to determine if it influences an individual’s occupational outcome. Prior to presenting tables of measures and summary statistics which describe the data, it would be useful to return to the personality data. The HILDA data set provides a rich array of variables that are rare within nationally representative data sets to examine the “dark matter” of the economics of personality factors (Heckman & Rubinstein 2001). The survey administered a questionnaire based on the FFM in Wave five. The test takes 30 questions consisting of adjectives that describe the respondent’s typical behaviour from the mini-marker test developed by Saucier (1994) and 6 questions from other valid personality tests. These personality tests are usually subjected to standard tests in order to ensure that they are valid psychometric instruments and can be used for meaningful analysis of unobserved psychological phenomena and are not purely mathematical constructs (Borghans et al. 2008a). The personality tests in HILDA are no exception and Losoncz (2007) provides an assessment of validity tests of the personality tests and finds that the derived psychometric instruments are valid. 14 Table 3: The Frequency of Occupations Occupation Frequency Percentage Managers Professionals Technicians and Trades Workers Community and Personal Service Work Clerical and Administrative Workers Sales Workers Machinery Operators and Drivers Labourers 4,211 8,063 3,749 2,641 4,883 1,911 1,560 2,216 14.40 27.58 12.82 9.03 16.70 6.54 5.34 7.58 Table 4: Summary Statistics Variable Mean S.D. Age Age 40.45 11.69 Age Squared 1773.32 977.79 Personality Agreeableness 5.3722 0.8792 Openness 4.3131 1.0197 Conscientiousness 5.1541 1.0052 Extraversion 4.4732 1.0840 Emotional Stability 5.1739 1.0885 Parental Status Father’s AUSEI06 45.7487 22.718 Mother AUSEI06 42.2437 22.908 Min. Max. 15 225 893 6889 1 1 1 1 1 7 7 7 7 7 0 3.4 100 100 Table 3 gives the frequency and relative frequency counts for occupational status categories used in the multinomial logit. The preponderance of professionals and clerical and administrative workers reflects the distribution of occupations in the population and mirrors outcomes of other Australian surveys. Table 4 contains summary statistics for those variables where these measures are meaningful. Table 5 summarises the binary variables used in the analysis. For each binary the score is the proportion of the sample scoring one. 15 Table 5: Binary Variables Variable Proportion Variable Gender (Base: male) Female Education (Base: year 12) PhD or master Graduate diploma Bachelor Advance diploma Certificate 3 or 4 Certificate 1 or 2 Year 11 or Less State (Base: New S Wales) Proportion Variable Father’s occupation (Base: Professional) Year (Base: 2001) Proportion 0.4946 2002 0.1315 Father Manager 0.2536 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 Country of Origin (Base 0.2139 Australia) English Speaking country of 0.0117 origin 0.2137 Non English country of origin 0.1371 0.1431 0.1602 0.1521 0.1478 0.2444 0.0346 0.0623 0.0480 0.1017 0.0494 0.0782 0.1787 0.1096 Marital Status (Base: single) Father Technician Father Service Father Clerical Father Sales Father Operator Father labourer Mother’s occupation (Base: 0.1152 Professional) 0.0737 Mother Manager 0.0915 Mother Technician 0.0794 0.0993 Victoria 0.2507 Couple 0.5580 Mother Service 0.0809 Queensland 0.2084 Post Couple 0.1416 Mother Clerical 0.2311 South Australia 0.0846 Mother Sales 0.1206 Western Australia 0.0968 Mother Operator 0.0347 Tasmania 0.0317 mother labourer 0.1713 Northern Territory Australian Capital Territory 0.0074 0.0239 Estimation and Results A multinomial logit model was estimated using STATA 10. The estimate was normalized on the modal occupation, Professional. Using the test proposed by Long and Freese (2006) in their SPOST suite of STATA commands (not reported), which is a test based on the Cramer-Ridder test (Cramer & Ridder 1991), it can be concluded that none of the occupational states can be pooled together, suggesting that no further aggregation is possible without biasing the results. Based on this, one should not expect violations of the independence of irrelevant alternatives (IIA) assumption . Rather than report the rather large numbers of estimated coefficients the marginal effects alone are reported. The analysis used the marginal effects estimated by the MARGEFF module (Bartus 2005). Further, these are the mean marginal effects for the sample data rather than the marginal effects at means. For brevity, the average marginal effects for the personality traits, the focus of this study, are presented in Table 6 in the text. All other 16 average marginal effects are reported and discussed in separate tables in the Appendix. In order to assess the relative impacts of discrete and continuous variables the paper compares the average effect of a change in a discrete variable from its minimum to maximum value to the same measure for a continuous variable4. 4 A comment on an earlier version of this paper suggested that comparisons of magnitudes in shift should be on the basis of a standard deviation shift rather than minimum to maximum. This is problematical given that a comparison may be personality against education credentials, where the latter are binary and therefore discrete variables. The standard deviation is not a valid statistic for nomial variables (Stevens 1946)-and would produce poor estimates of the relative magnitudes of these effects especially in non-linear models such as the multinomial logit. 17 Table 6: Average Marginal Effects Personality Traits for base model Variable (Base) Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Female Specific Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Female Specific Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Observations Legend Managers 0.0003 0.0177*** 0.01128** -0.0142** 0.0025 Professionals 0.0279*** -0.0043 -0.0008 -0.0115 0.0084 Technician 0.0035 0.0034 0.0057 -0.0027 -0.0005 Service -0.0170** -0.0004 0.0020 0.0121 0.0089 0.01485 -0.0100 0.0118 -0.0100 -0.0039 Clerical -0.0003 0.0055 -0.0312*** 0.0074 -0.0117 -0.0322*** 0.0066 -0.0031 0.0034*** 0.0024 Sales -0.0044 -0.0089** 0.0115*** 0.0038 -0.0025 0.0058 -0.0035 -0.0058 -0.0066 -0.0093 Operator -0.0047 -0.0024 0.0013 0.004508 -0.0013 0.0143* -0.0026 -0.0061 --0.0089 -0.0081 Labourer -0.0051 -0.0107*** 0.0001 0.0015 -0.0037 -0.0129 0.0089 0.0231*** 0.0004 0.0031 29,234 * p<0.1 0.0054 0.0021 -0.0045 -0.0042 0.0012 0.0058 -0.0066 -0.0099 -0.0047 0.0102 Pseudo R2 *** p<0.01 -0.0015 0.0050 -0.0055 0.0005 0.0045 0.2444 ** p<0.05 Personality traits, discussion: Table 6 indicates that personality traits generally seem to have some influence on occupational outcomes with the average marginal effects frequently being statistically significant for each trait and occupation. Openness to experience, a trait related to receptiveness to training and accepting of new and different ideas, significantly increases the probability of individuals being found in a professional role but for males with the female specific effect perfectly negating the base effect with the sum of effect failing to be significantly different from zero (p=0.48). This is to be expected as both require individuals to deal with a variety of ideas, be accepting 18 of new ideas and concepts and require training, as can be seen from the definition of professionals Table 1. The trait openness also tends to reduce the probability of a male being a service worker with the female specific effects cancelling both effects (p=0.52). The tasks of a service worker, as seen in Table 1, tends to be relatively more routine, thus openness would not be highly valued by employers. It is possible that individuals who tend not to be receptive to new ideas may choose jobs within these occupations as it allows for a narrow specialisation. The effect of openness is relatively large and important. For example, for a professional moving from the lowest possible score, one, to the highest, seven, gives an increase of 0.17, which is slightly larger than the average effect of completion of high school on the probability of being in a professional occupation; see Table A1 in the Appendix. Conscientiousness, the trait associated with hard work and effort, is found to significantly increase the probability of an individual, both male and female, being in a management position and decrease the probability of an individual being a salesperson or labourer. One aspect of the personality trait conscientiousness is the ability to plan and be organised, an aspect that is central to the tasks performed in management roles. Thus despite the trait being considered valuable in all labour market outcomes, as in Roy’s (1951) model, people tend towards an occupation where it is most valued and are drawn away from those of salesperson, and labourers. The magnitude of the effects of conscientiousness are quite sizeable: moving from the lowest level to the highest level of this trait increases the probability of being in a management occupation by 0.097 which has a greater effect than any education credential, while close to the influence of a PhD or Masters, influence on the probability of being a manager. See Table A1 in the Appendix for details of these scores. These results highlight the potential for personality factors to matter more than traditional human capital variables. Extraversion, the trait associated with being outgoing and desiring to engage with other individuals is found to significantly affect some occupational outcomes. Individuals who are observed with higher levels of extraversion have a higher probability of being in management, with the effect larger for females, and sales while a lower probability of 19 being in a clerical occupation. These results make sense in that sales roles necessitate social interactions. In addition, management entails, as can be seen in Table 1, the organising of resources to complete tasks such as human labour and thus requires social interaction. Conversely, clerical tasks are generally not focused on interacting with others and may even inhibit social interaction thus individuals who prefer less social interaction would tend to select these occupations. The effects of extraversion are also quite sizeable with for example, the maximum effects for clerical workers, that is the effect of moving from the lowest to the highest value of the personality trait, being larger in magnitude (0.19) than the effects of any education credentials. Agreeableness has a negative effect on the probability of an individual being a manager and a positive effect on the probability of females being professionals. As suggested earlier, certain occupations would tend to favour individuals who are less concerned with pleasing others. This includes occupations in which there is a greater focus on task completion and competition rather than interacting with others such as management positions. Conversely, females with higher levels of agreeableness have a higher probability of being in the professions. The fact that this effect applies only to females suggests there might be gender differences in the distribution of females in professional occupations in that females select professions that require greater interpersonal actions. Emotional stability, the negative of neuroticism, is a trait associated with being less likely to experience negative emotions, and unlike all the other personality traits of the five factor model, has no significant effect on occupational choice. The lack of a significant effect simply suggests that this effect is relative unimportant in influence the sorting of individual across occupations. Neuroticism may be associated with risk aversion and therefore the sorting of occupations here might reflect risk preferences5. DeLeire and Levy (2004) show that attitude towards risk affects occupational choice. Personality significantly influences the probability of an individual choosing or being chosen for a particular occupation, with each trait except emotional stability influencing 5 We are grateful to an anonymous referee for pointing out this issue. 20 at least one occupational outcome. Generally these effects are found to be modest in comparison to that of human capital but they can rival educational credentials in certain occupations. Due to the strength and magnitude of personality effects, it can be argued that they are relatively more important than parental status; with the parental status effects tending to be similar if not smaller in magnitude and less persistent. It is possible that the findings with regards to personality are suppressed as personality and parental status may also influence education and thus the indirect effect of personality is not captured by the current model. To test for this, a model was estimated that placed restrictions on the parameters associated with parental status to zero in order to see if the effects changed; the parameters were virtually identical across both models. In order to determine the robustness6 of the influence of personality on occupation outcomes, a number of restrictions were made to the parameters of the model to see if they altered the parameter values of the personality variables. Restricting parental status parameters to zero caused little or no change in the parameter values including those associated with personality. The restriction of education parameters to zero changed the personality estimated parameter values but did not change their significance. In addition, in order to examine if the age of an individual altered the effects of personality due to the possible instability in personality traits, two models were estimated for those under thirty and those thirty and over. Most parameters estimated for the sample restricted to less than thirty years were not significant. The under thirties accounted for less than 25% of the whole sample and it appears that the over thirties dominate the complete sample. Because personality traits are the source of novelty in this research, discussion of the results for other influences on occupational are left to the Appendix. However, in summary, it can be found that labour market heterogeneity is important as individuals are sorted between occupations based on the various other different characteristics nominated by theory and incorporated in the multinomial logit model. The results broadly indicate that education exhibits a non-linear effect in years, with arguably occupation specific 6 We would like to thank an anonymous reviewer for the suggestion of these robustness checks. Detailed results are available on request. 21 education effects and that parental status has a small and limited effect on occupational outcomes. Conclusion This paper examines the effect of human capital, parental status and personality on occupational outcomes for a representative panel of Australian households. Economic theory argues that individuals should select the occupation that grants them the highest utility and that because both individuals and tasks in the labour markets are quite heterogeneous individuals are sorted into occupations. Using a multinomial logit model, it is found that human capital exhibits a non-linear effect on occupational attainment, parental status has a limited influence and the broad personality traits of the highly validated five factor model have a significant, relatively strong, persistent and expected effect over occupational outcomes. To conclude, we found in our exploration of the “dark matter” in economics a significant role for personality in influencing occupational outcomes. We found that there were significant gender differences in certain occupations for particular personality traits. References Akerlof, G. A. Social distance and social decisions. Econometrica 1997. 65; 5; 10051027. Barrick, M. R. & Mount, M. K. The big five personality dimensions and job performance: a meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology 1991. 44; 1; 1-26. Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K. & Gupta, R. Meta-analysis of the relationship between the five-factor model of personality and Holland's occupational types. Personnel Psychology 2003. 56; 1; 45-74. 22 Bartus, T. Estimation of marginal effects using margeff. The Stata Journal 2005. 5; 3; 309-329. Bjerk, D. The differing nature of black-white wage inequality across occupational sectors. Journal of Human Resources 2007. 42; 2; 398-434. Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J. & ter Weel, B. The Economics and Psychology of Personality Traits. Journal of Human Resources 2008a. 43; 4; 972-1059. Borghans, L., ter Weel, B. & Weinberg, B. A. Interpersonal Styles and Labor Market Outcomes. Journal of Human Resources 2008b. 43; 4; 815-858. Bowles, S. & Gintis, H. The inheritance of inequality. Journal of Economic Perspectives 2002. 16; 3; 3-30. Bowles, S., Gintis, H. & Osborne, M. The determinants of earnings: a behavioral approach. Journal of Economic Literature 2001a. 39; 4; 1137. Bowles, S., Gintis, H. & Osborne, M. Incentive-enhancing preferences: personality, behavior, and earnings. American Economic Review 2001b. 91; 2; 155-158. Bradley, S. An empirical analysis of occupational expectations. Applied Economics 1991. 23; 7; 1159. Caplan, B. Stigler-Becker versus Myers-Briggs: why preference-based explanations are scientifically meaningful and empirically important. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 2003. 50; 4; 391-405. Cole, K. Good for the soul: the relationship between work, wellbeing and psychological capital, PhD in Economics. University of Canberra: 2007, Costa Jr, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. 2008. The NEO inventories In: Personality Assessment. R. P. Archer and S. R. Smith (Eds.), Personality Assessment. Routledge: New York; 2008. p. Cramer, J. S. & Ridder, G. Pooling states in the multinomial logit model. Journal of Econometrics 1991. 47; 2-3; 267-272. Cunha, F. & Heckman, J. J. Formulating, Identifying and Estimating the Technology of Cognitive and Noncognitive Skill Formation. Journal of Human Resources 2008. 43; 4; 738-782. 23 DeLeire, T. & Levy, H. Worker sorting and the risk of death on the job. Journal of Labor Economics 2004. 22; 4; 925-953. Digman, J. M. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology 1990. 41; 1; 417. Doepke, M. & Zilibotti, F. Social class and the spirit of capitalism. Journal of the European Economic Association 2005. 3; 2/3; 516-524. Fan, C. S. Religious participation and children's education: a social capital approach. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organisation 2008. 65; 303-317. Furnham, A. & Fudge, C. The five factor model of personality and sales performance. Journal of Individual Differences 2008. 29; 1; 11-16. Goldberg, L. R. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist 1993. 48; 1; 26-34. Groves, M. O. How important is your personality? Labor market returns to personality for women in the US and UK. Journal of Economic Psychology 2005. 26; 6; 827-841. Heckman, J. J., Lochner, L. & Todd, P. E. Fifty years of Mincer earnings regressions. Cambridge, Massachusetts; 2003 Heckman, J. J. & Rubinstein, Y. The importance of noncognitive skills: lessons from the GED testing program. American Economic Review 2001. 91; 2; 145-149. Heckman, J. J., Stixrud, J. & Urzua, S. The effects of cognitive and noncognitive abilities on labor market outcomes and social behavior. Journal of Labor Economics 2006. 24; 3; 411. Jones, F. L. & McMillan, J. Scoring occupational categories for social research: A review of current practice, with Australian examples. Work Employment Society 2001. 15; 3; 539-563. Knight, J.B. Job Competition, Occupational Production Functions, and Filtering Down, Oxford Economic Papers 1979. 31; 2; 187-204. Krueger, A. B. & Schkade, D. Sorting in the Labor Market. Journal of Human Resources 2008. 43; 4; 859-883. Laband, D. N. & Lentz, B. F. Like father, like son: toward an economic theory of occupational following. Southern Economic Journal 1983. 50; 2; 474. 24 Larson, L. M., Rottinghaus, P. J. & Borgen, F. H. Meta-analyses of big six interests and big five personality factors. Journal of Vocational Behavior 2002. 61; 2; 217-239. Leigh, A. Returns to education in Australia. Economic papers 2008. 27; 3; 233. Long, J. S. & Freese, J. Regression models for categorical dependent variables using Stata. Stata Press. College Station, Texas; 2006 Losoncz, I. (2007). Personality Traits in HILDA. HILDA survey research conference. University of Melbourne. Mazumder, B. Fortunate sons: new estimates of intergenerational mobility in the united states using social security earnings data. Review of Economics & Statistics 2005. 87; 2; 235-255. McCrae, R. R. & Costa, P. T. Personality in adulthood : a five-factor theory perspective. Guilford Press. New York; 2003 McMillan, J., Jones, F. L. & Beavis, A. (2008). The AUSEI06: A new socioeconomic index for Australia. The annual conference of The Australian Sociological Association. T. Majoribanks et al. University of Melbourne, Victoria, TASA. Mueller, G. & Plug, E. Estimating the Effect of Personality on Male and Female Earnings. Industrial and Labor Relations Review 2006. 60; 1; 3-22. Nyhus, E. K. & Pons, E. The effects of personality on earnings. Journal of Economic Psychology 2005. 26; 3; 363-384. Ozer, D. J. & Benet-Martinez, V. Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology 2006. 57; 1; 401-421. Roberts, B. & DelVecchio, W. The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin 2000. 126; 1; 3-25. Robertson, D. & Symons, J. The occupational choice of British children. Economic Journal 1990. 100; 402; 828-841. Roy, A. D. Some thoughts on the distribution of earnings. Oxford Economic Papers 1951. 3; 2; 135-146. Saucier, G. Mini-markers: a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers. Journal of Personality Assessment 1994. 63; 3; 506. 25 Semykina, A. & Linz, S. J. Gender differences in personality and earnings: evidence from Russia. Journal of Economic Psychology 2007. 28; 3; 387-410. Shaw, K. L. A formulation of the earnings function using the concept of occupational investment. The Journal of Human Resources 1984. 19; 3; 319340. Shaw, K. L. Occupational change, employer change, and the transferability of skills. Southern Economic Journal 1987. 53; 3; 702. Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. Personality Plasticity After Age 30. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, pp. 999-1009 vol. 32: 2006. 32; 999-1009 Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R. & Costa Jr, P. T. Intra-individual change in personality stability and age. Journal of Research in Personality 2010. 44; 1; 31-37. Tsukahara, I. The effect of family background on occupational choice. Labour 2007. 21; 4-5; 871-890. Viinikainen, J., Kokko, K., Pulkkinen, L. & Pehkonen, J. Personality and Labour Market Income: Evidence from Longitudinal Data. Labour 2010. 24; 2; 201220. Wakefield, R. L. Accounting and Machiavellianism. Behavioral Research in Accounting 2008. 20; 1; 115. Watson, N. (Eds.) HILDA user manual - release 7. Melbourne institute of applied economic and social research, University of Melbourne. 2009 26 Appendix Table A1: Average marginal effects of base Human Capital Variables Variable Age Age squared PhD or Masters Graduate diploma Bachelor Advanced diploma Certificate 3 or 4 Certificate 1 or 2 Year 11 or less Managers 0.0634** -0.0000 0.0096*** 0.0380 0.0186 0.0114 -0.0589*** -0.0171 -0.0243 Clerical Age 0.0043 Age squared -0.0000 PhD or Masters -0.1557*** Graduate diploma -0.1415*** Bachelor -0.1047*** Advanced diploma -0.0235 Certificate 3 or 4 -0.0510** Certificate 1 or 2 -0.2058*** Year 11 or less -0.0292 Legend * p<0.1 Professional 0.0018 0.0000 0.4174*** 0.3781*** 0.3335*** 0.0701** -0.0701*** -0.04922 -0.1257*** Sales -0.0053*** 0.0001*** -0.0774*** -0.0597**** -0.0439*** -0.0330** -0.0066 -0.0487*** 0.0177 ** p<0.05 27 Technician -0.0056*** 0.0000* -0.0746*** -0.0521*** -0.0434*** 0.0309* 0.1724*** -0.0065 0.0038 Operator 0.0044*** -0.0001*** -0.0579*** -0.0437*** -0.0536*** -0.0420*** -0.0050 0.1029** 0.06685*** *** p<0.01 Service -0.0011 0.0000 -0.0743*** -0.0539*** -0.0671*** 0.0374 0.0301 0.0390 -0.0192 Labourer -0.0072*** 0.0001*** -0.0709*** -0.0652*** -0.0395*** -0.0286** -0.0109 0.1855*** 0.0717*** Table A2: Average marginal effects of female specific Human Capital Variables Variable Age Age squared PhD or Masters Graduate diploma Bachelor Advanced diploma Certificate 3 or 4 Certificate 1 or 2 Year 11 or less Managers -0.0001 0.0000 0.0141 0.0090 -0.0232 -0.0292 -0.0230 -0.0510 -0.0044 Clerical Age -0.0039 Age squared 0.0000 PhD or Masters -0.0745 Graduate diploma -0.0559 Bachelor -0.0904*** Advanced diploma -0.00857*** Certificate 3 or 4 -0.0567** Certificate 1 or 2 -0.3973*** Year 11 or less -0.0270 Legend * p<0.1 Professional 0.0019 -0.0000 0.0986* 0.1109*** 0.0942*** 0.1132*** -0.0822** -0.1937*** 0.0589 Sales -0.0018 -0.0000 0.0539 -0.0376** -0.0327*** -0.0102 -0.0329*** 0.0385 -0.0094 ** p<0.05 Technician 0.0011 -0.0000 0.0515 0.0339 0.0403 -0.0088 0.0004 -0.0072 0.0156 Operator 0.0021 0.0000 -0.0576*** -0.0576*** 0.0176 -0.0281 -0.0227 -0.0552*** -0.0370*** *** p<0.01 Service -0.0016 0.0000 -0.0061 -0.0341 0.0162 0.0156 0.0245 -0.0642** -0.0351** Labourer 0.0028 -0.0000 -0.0799*** 0.0314 -0.0220 -0.0060 -0.0178 -0.0645*** -0.0049*** Human capital variables seem to exhibit significant effects on occupational choice. Age seems to increase the probability of an individual being a manager or operator while decreasing the probability of an individual being a technician, sales worker or labourer occupations significantly. These effects are observed to be quadratic with decreasing returns for all occupational states except managers. These are expected as these occupations relative to salespersons, labourers and service workers may require less physical exertion relative to other occupation such as clerical workers, professionals and managers. It can also be seen as promotion with individuals moving across these occupations with experience. It should be noted that there is no statistically significant different between the effects of age on females and males. These findings confirm previous research that potential experience influences labour market outcomes such as occupational choice. 28 Education is specified as a series of education credentials in order to capture any possible non-linearity and occupational specific capital that would not be captured using the standard specification of education measured in years. It should be noted that all the effects of education are relative to that of an individual with a completed high school education. University level education, consisting of PhD or masters, graduate diploma and the bachelor degree, have a strong positive effect on the probability of an individual being in a profession and, to a less extent, manager. In addition, the effect of a PhD or Masters for females being professionals is significantly larger than that of their male counterparts as also generally are their probability of being in other occupational states. The estimated model suggests that the possession of university level education credentials draws individuals away from all other occupations fairly equally. This result is expected as professionals, Table 1 in the text, require the completion of conceptual, creative and analytical tasks based on a body of knowledge. This body of knowledge is typically gained from university education and is very important for professionals. The effects on management are also expected but are due to individuals ‘rising through the ranks’ to management positions. Education credentials also exhibit some non-linearity, in terms of years, and some occupation specific human capital. Certificates 3 or 4 from technical colleges associated with the acquisition of a trade, technical or other applied skills are after a high school qualification. This credential has a significant positive effect on technician or service occupations but having a certificate 3 or 4 actually decreases the probability of being in a profession or management occupation relative to that of a high school graduate. This suggests that education does not just exhibit a constant linear effect and may possess occupational specific components. Other possible occupation specific effects can be the increased probability of being either an operator with a certificate 1 or 2. This highlights the importance of occupation when examining labour market outcomes and possible flaws with standard specification of human capital theory as have been highlighted in previous literature (Shaw 1984; Shaw 1987; Heckman et al. 2003; Leigh 2008). Education has its largest effect on the outcomes of professional occupations, with a university degree increasing the probability of a 29 professional occupation by approximately 0.3. Lack of education has a significant influence, with people who fail to complete the final year of high school having reduced probabilities, compared to high school graduates, of being a professionals and they are more likely to be operators or labourers. Table A3: Average marginal effects of the base Dynasty Hysteresis Variables Variable Father is manager Father is technician Father is service worker Father is clerical worker Father is sales worker Father is operator Father is labourer Mother is manager Mother is technician Mother is service worker Mother is clerical worker Mother is sales worker Mother is operator Mother is labourer Father’s AUSEI06 Mother’s AUSEI06 Father is manager Father is technician Father is service worker Father is clerical worker Father is sales worker Father is operator Father is labourer Mother is manager Mother is technician Mother is service worker Mother is clerical worker Mother is sales worker Mother is operator Mother is labourer Father’s AUSEI06 Mother’s AUSEI06 Legend Manager -0.0391 -0.0655*** -0.0544 -0.0530** -0.0728*** -0.0672*** -0.0683** 0.0024 -0.0095 -0.0161 0.00079 0.0110 -0.04300 -0.0171 -0.0012*** -0.0001 Clerical -0.0277 -0.0095 -0.0207 -0.0090 0.0138 0.0077 -0.0132 0.0169 -0.0459 -0.0037 -0.0306 -0.0087 -0.0055 0.01734 0.0005 -0.0008 * p<0.1 Professional 0.0026 0.0127 -0.0129 -0.0241 0.0256 -0.0328 -0.0328 -0.0406 0.0111 0.0236 -0.0034 -0.0335 -0.0407 -0.0227 0.0006 0.0001 Sales -0.0094 -0.0167 -0.0037 -0.0203 -0.0122 -0.0083 -0.0121 0.0214 0.0342 0.0274 0.0196 0.0416 0.0419 0.0423 -0.0004. 0.0008 ** p<0.05 30 Technician -0.0147 0.03575 0.0573* 0.0639** -0.0146 0.0128 0.0019 0.0125 0.0059 0.0086 0.0409* 0.0158 -0.0075 0.0415 0.0000 0.0005 Operator 0.0047 0.0063 0.0133 0.0008 0.0213 0.0312 0.0250 -0.0278 -0.0402*** -0.0287 -0.0373** -0.0355* -0.0346 -0.0451** 0.0001 -0.0007 *** p<0.01 Service -0.0033 0.0030 -0.0007 0.0279 0.0071 0.0239 0.0347 0.0229 -0.0086 0.0223 0.0114 0.0192 0.0073 -0.0162 0.0003 0.0003 Labourer 0.0087 0.3200 0.0218 0.00137 0.0320 0.0332 0.0477 -0.0077 0.0001 0.0139 -0.0085 -0.0099 0.0007 0.00040 0.0001 -0.0001 Table A4: Average marginal effects of the female specific Dynasty Hysteresis Variables Variable Manager Professional Technician Service 0.0083 0.0005 0.0547 0.0022 Father is manager 0.0729 -0.0347 0.0580 -0.0092 Father is technician 0.0737 -0.0185 0.0452 0.0064 Father is service worker 0.0687 0.0427 -0.0087 -0.0352 Father is clerical worker 0.0352 -0.0331 0.1058* 0.0026 Father is sales worker 0.0787 -0.0145 0.0912 -0.0270 Father is operator 0.1009 -0.0246 0.1006 -0.0534** Father is labourer 0.0607 0.0087 -0.0179 -0.0569** Mother is manager 0.0188 -0.0217 -0.0881** -0.0429 Mother is technician 0.0181 0.0168 -0.0681 -0.0399 Mother is service worker 0.00203 -0.0137 -0.0711** -0.0344 Mother is clerical worker -0.0106 -0.0200 -0.0846** -0.0366 Mother is sales worker 0.0843 -0.0552 -0.0965** -0.0307 Mother is operator 0.0118 -0.01500 0.0955** -0.0195 Mother is labourer 0.0019*** -0.0006 0.0016** -0.0007 Father’s AUSEI06 0.0005 0.0003 -0.0012 -0.0010 Mother’s AUSEI06 Clerical Sales Operator Labourer 0.0290 -0.0050 -0.0612*** -0.0284 Father is manager 0.0372 0.0010 -0.0618*** -0.0634*** Father is technician 0.0436 -0.0164 -0.0691*** -0.0648 Father is service worker 0.0254 0.0188 -0.0541*** -0.0575*** Father is clerical worker 0.0037 -0.0006 -0.0552*** -0.0584*** Father is sales worker -0.0119 0.0023 -0.0649*** -0.0540** Father is operator 0.0024 0.0080 -0.0681*** -0.0658*** Father is labourer -0.0053 -0.0327 0.0288 0.0147 Mother is manager 0.0865 -0.0478* 0.1162 -0.0212 Mother is technician 0.0021 -0.0417 0.1307 -0.0179 Mother is service worker 0.0724 -0.0526** 0.0959 -0.0168 Mother is clerical worker 0.0598 -0.0465 0.1526 -0.0142 Mother is sales worker 0.0347 -0.0747*** 0.1177 0.0203 Mother is operator -0.0008 -0.0628** 0.1687 0.0132 Mother is labourer -0.0006 0.0006 -0.0014 -0.0008 Father’s AUSEI06 0.0013 -0.0015** 0.0016* 0.0001 Mother’s AUSEI06 Legend * p<0.1 ** p<0.05 *** p<0.01 Parental status also seems to have significant effects on occupational outcomes, albeit these effects are much smaller than those of education. Generally, social status measured by the AUSEI06 seems to be insignificant; however, father’s AUSEI06 has a significant and positive effect for males only on the probability of being a manager. Father's social 31 status significantly increases the probability of females being technicians and mother social status increases the probability of being an operator and reducing the occurrence of sales. These effects do suggest that parents with high social status do generally improve the labour market outcomes of their offspring and supports dynasty hysteresis. The binary variables for parental occupation provide some support for the hypothesis of dynasty hysteresis. If the father of an individual is in any occupation other than a manager then the probability of being a manager relative to a professional decreases. This suggests fathers being in service occupations seem to have a significant positive effect on the probability of their offspring being in a clerical occupation or for females in service occupations. In addition a mother being a technician, clerical worker, sales worker or labourer reduces the probability of that individual being an operator. Mother’s social status has a number of interesting effects which can be generally summarised by if the mother is in any occupation other than a profession it reduces the probability of an individual being a technician, an operator or for a female being a sales worker. Generally these results suggest some evidence of dynasty hysteresis; however, the effects are not large in magnitude or systemic. 32 Table A5: Average effect of Base Demographics variables Variable Couple Post couple English speaking Non-English speaking Couple Post couple English speaking Non-English speaking Legend Managers 0.0510*** 0.004236 Professional -0.106 -0.0515** Technician -0.0321*** -0.0080 Service 0.0132 0.0429 -0.0029 -0.0027 0.0078 -0.0045 -0.0489*** Clerical 0.0161 -0.0074 -0.0536*** Sales -0.0150 0.0090 0.0479*** Operator -0.0056 -0.0053 0.0152 Labourer -0.0170** 0.0160 0.0010 0.0002 -0.0101 0.0115 0.0168 * p<0.1 0.0074 ** p<0.05 -0.0238*** *** p<0.01 0.0391** Table A6: Average effect of Female Specific Demographics variables Variable Female Couple Post couple English speaking Non-English speaking Female Couple Post couple English speaking Non-English speaking Legend Managers -0.2093*** -0.0347* -0.0171 Professional -0.1258 0.0295 0.0527 Technician -0.0622 0.0298 0.0105 Service 0.2361 -0.0222 -0.036 -0.0234 0.0001 0.0171 0.0096 -0.0198 Clerical 0.0955 -0.0115 0.0117 -0.0000 Sales 0.0145 0.0109 0.0273 -0.05324** Operator -0.0584** -0.0173 -0.0299** 0.0074 Labourer -0.0207 0.0155 0.0050 0.0156 0.0004 -0.0189 -0.0005 -0.0078 * p<0.1 *0.0009 ** p<0.05 0.0617 *** p<0.01 0.0126 33 Table A7: Average Effect of Location, Date and Industry for base variables Variable Victoria Queensland South Australia Western Australia Tasmania Northern territory Australia CT 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 Production industry Victoria Queensland South Australia Western Australia Tasmania Northern territory Australia CT 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 Production industry Legend Managers -0.0054 -0.0349*** -0.0096 -0.0127 -0.0127 0.0391 0.0077 0.0019 -0.0050 -0.0042 -0.0090 -0.0023 -0.0169 Professional 0.0070 -0.0175 0.0161 0.0360 -0.0201 -0.0699 0.0139 -0.0145* -0.0287*** -0.0205** -0.00253 -0.0321*** -0.0313*** Technician -0.0135 0.0240** -0.0213 0.00150 0.0004 -0.0263 -0.0068 0.0069 0.0041 0.0123** 0.0095* 0.0105* 0.0200*** Service 0.0085 0.0040 -0.0232 -0.0360** -0.0016 0.0213 -0.0225 0.0174** 0.0234*** 0.0191** 0.0298*** 0.0263*** 0.0299*** 0.0876*** Clerical -0.0022 0.0115 0.0102 0.0080 0.0146 0.0673 0.0556 0.0065 0.0025 0.0012 0.0028 0.0028 0.0063 -0.01111*** Sales 0.0124 0.0023 0.0054 0.0115 0.02311 -0.0177 -0.0373*** -0.0089 0.0038 -0.0044 -0.0059 -0.0090 -0.0067 0.1478*** Operator -0.0022 -0.0012 0.0001 0.0043 0.0024 0.0008 -0.0163 0.0013* -0.0009 -0.0019 0.0019 0.0017 0.0020 -0.1099*** Labourer -0.0046 0.0117 0.0221 -0.0126 -0.0028 -0.0157 -0079 0.0.105* 0.0010 -0.0048 -0.0022 0.0020 -0.0033 -0.0608*** * p<0.1 -0.0667*** ** p<0.05 0.0185*** *** p<0.01 0.0947*** 34 Table A8: Average Effect of Location, Date and Industry for female specific variables Variable Victoria Queensland South Australia Western Australia Tasmania Northern territory Australia CT 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 Production industry Victoria Queensland South Australia Western Australia Tasmania Northern territory Australia CT 2002 2003 2004 2005 Managers 0.0008 0.0039 -0.0092 0.0359 -0.0626** -0.0114 -0.0063 -0.0085 0.0110 0.0177 -0.0019 -0.0006 -0.0031 0.0412* Clerical 0.0094 -0.0277 -0.0059 -0.0253 -0.0452 -0.0561 0.0100 0.0094 0.0099 0.0009 0.0178 2006 0.0148 2007 0.0048 Production industry 0.0922 Legend * p<0.1 Professional -0.0137 0.0074 -0.0039 -0.0245 0.0618 0.0140 0.0025 0.0125 0.0170 0.0083 0.0142 0.0159 0.0138 -0.1147*** Sales -0.0002 -0.0023 0.0035 -0.0087 -0.0313* 0.0129 0.0164 0.0070 -0.0081 0.0134 0.0076 Technician 0.0223 -0.0285 0.0425 -0.0257 -0.0103 0.1676 -0.0142 -0.0159 -0.0327*** -0.0341 -0.0255** -0.0281** -0.0316** -0.0632*** Operator 0.0137 0.0327 -0.0229 0.0058 0.0025** -0.0524 0.0309*** 0.0038* 0.0190 -0.0075 -0.0039 -0.00203 0.0136 0.0161 0.0214 ** p<0.05 0.0057134 0.0249 -0.0042 0.0393 -0.0274 *** p<0.01 35 Service -0.0176 0.0054 0.0197 0.0622 0.0255 -0.0490* -0.0011 -0.0124 -0.0149 -0.0077 -0.0183* -0.0192* -0.0206** 0.0113 Labourer -0.0147 0.0090 -0.0237 -0.0197 0.0374* 0.0254 0.03827 0.0116 -0.001131 0.00898 0.0099 Table A9: Average effect of a one standard deviation change in personality traits Variable Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Managers 0.0061 0.0125 0.0162 -0.0126 0.0019 Clerical Openness -0.0098 Conscientiousness 0.0066 Extraversion -0.0144 Agreeableness 0.0065 Emotional Stability -0.0084 Professionals 0.0117 -0.0006 -0.0020 -0.0016 0.0071 Sales -0.0005 -0.0082 0.0060 0.0035 -0.0020 Technician 0.0015 0.0014 0.0012 -0.0032 -0.0026 Operator -0.0044 -0.0032 -0.0000 0.0054 -0.0004 Service -0.0006 -0.0008 -0.0004 0.0031 0.0027 Labourer -0.0041 -0.0077 -0.0066 -0.0012 0.0016 Table A10: Average effect of one standard deviation change in education credentials Variable Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Emotional Stability Managers 0.0061 0.0125 0.0162 -0.0126 0.0019 Clerical Openness -0.0098 Conscientiousness 0.0066 Extraversion -0.0144 Agreeableness 0.0065 Emotional Stability -0.0084 Professionals 0.0117 -0.0006 -0.0020 -0.0016 0.0071 Sales -0.0005 -0.0082 0.0060 0.0035 -0.0020 36 Technician 0.0015 0.0014 0.0012 -0.0032 -0.0026 Operator -0.0044 -0.0032 -0.0000 0.0054 -0.0004 Service -0.0006 -0.0008 -0.0004 0.0031 0.0027 Labourer -0.0041 -0.0077 -0.0066 -0.0012 0.0016