Chapter 1 - Introduction Introduction This thesis concerns the speciation process in a pair of tropical butterflies; Heliconius melpomene and H. cydno. In this chapter, speciation theory and the study organisms will be introduced. Chapter 2 summarises the literature on introgressive hybridisation in animal taxa, its consequences and possible implications. Chapters 3 and 4 present gene genealogies which provide evidence for selective gene flow between the species. These chapters also help to clarify phylogenetic relationships in the genus. Chapter 5 describes experiments performed to test the hypothesis that F1 hybrids will be preferentially taken by avian predators. In Chapter 6, conclusions are drawn regarding how this thesis has contributed to current knowledge on genealogy and speciation in H. melpomene and H. cydno. Appendix 1 is a paper senior authored by Margarita Beltrán, but to which I contributed. This paper describes gene genealogies of heliconiine butterflies (from the genera Heliconius, Laparus, Eueides, Dryadula, and Dryas), based on mtDNA and nuclear genes. Speciation Species and their formation are of major importance for many fields of biology such as ecology, conservation, taxonomy and evolutionary genetics. Historically, species were seen as separate units, created individually by some higher force, but this view changed with the publication of Darwin’s “On the Origin of Species” (1859). 13 Darwin’s theory that species evolved from one another was hotly contested initially, but eventually became accepted. This led to an explosion of interest in studies of speciation, defined as the process leading to the formation of a new species. This began with the Modern Synthesis of Darwinian evolution and Mendelian genetics (Dobzhansky, 1937). Over time, the popularity of speciation waned as new issues were perceived to be more important (Barton, 2001). Recently, there has been renewed interest in the subject. Furthermore studies have become more focused on sympatric and parapatric, as opposed to allopatric speciation. The most informative method for studying the process of speciation is to look at intermediate cases where taxa still hybridise in the wild, but have developed some degree of reproductive isolation. The majority of studies of hybridising taxa have involved hybrid zones, where hybrids are present in large numbers (e.g. Endler, 1977; Barton and Hewitt; 1989; Harrison, 1993). Although these studies provide much useful information, it could be argued that they teach us little about speciation, as the taxa have not yet speciated (Jiggins et al, 1996; Jiggins and Mallet, 2000). A better approach might be to investigate species that hybridise only rarely in the wild, and show a greater degree of reproductive isolation. Reproductive isolation In order for speciation to occur, strong barriers must be present to prevent two species from mating and forming viable offspring. Reproductive isolation is any barrier that prevents successful reproduction between individuals, or reduces the fitness of a hybrid once it is formed. If a group of individuals is reproductively 14 isolated, gene flow with the rest of the population is reduced and speciation thus becomes possible. Isolating mechanisms or barriers can take many different forms, but can be divided into two major categories: pre - and post-mating barriers. Pre-mating isolation Pre-mating or prezygotic isolation is the process whereby species are prevented from forming hybrids by one or several barriers acting before fertilisation. These barriers may be geographical, ecological, behavioural or morphological (Mayr, 1963). Geographical isolation occurs when species are prevented from cross-mating due to spatial separation, for example by a water body, mountain range, continental drift, drying lakes or island formation (Dobzhansky, 1937; Mayr, 1963). Ecological divergence can also drive speciation, examples being habitat divergence and resource partitioning (e.g. Dobzhansky, 1937; Jiggins and Mallet, 2000; Schluter, 2001; Via, 2001). Behavioural differences can prevent species from mating together, for example the species may exhibit different courtship rituals, colour pattern or pheromones (e.g. Grant, 1993; Seehausen et al, 1997; Doi et al, 2001). Morphological differences between species can provide another very strong barrier to interspecific mating, e.g. physiological incompatibility of reproductive organs (Sota and Vogler, 2001). Pre-mating isolation will not be dealt with further here as it does not form a part of this thesis. 15 Post-mating isolation Post-mating or postzygotic isolation describes factors acting to reduce the fitness of a hybrid zygote once it has formed. Post-mating isolation includes any form of selection against hybrids, for instance hybrid sterility or inviability, environmental selection, increased predation, or sexual selection against hybrids. Genomic incompatibilities may cause hybrids to be inviable, or completely or partially sterile (e.g. Arntzen and Wallis, 1991; Palopoli and Wu, 1994; Saetre et al, 1997; Coyne and Orr, 1998). Ecological factors may affect hybrids in that they may not be as well adapted to their environment as the parental forms (e.g. Grant and Grant, 1993); or they may show reduced mating success (e.g. Stratton and Uetz, 1986). There may also be increased predation on hybrids due to positive frequency dependent selection. In this thesis, Chapter 5, I explore whether this is the case for two Heliconius butterfly species. Heliconius as a model system for studying speciation Heliconius Heliconius butterflies (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae: Heliconiina) are a Neotropical genus consisting of around 36 species. Ever since the work of Henry Walter Bates and Fritz Müller, Heliconius butterflies have been important in studies of mimicry and natural selection. Müller (1879) described a situation, now called Müllerian mimicry, whereby an unpalatable species may evolve to mimic another unpalatable 16 species, thereby gaining ‘safety in numbers’ against predators. Heliconius is a wellstudied group, and because of their bright coloration have been popular with collectors. All species in the genus are warningly-colored and unpalatable, many forming part of mimicry rings. They gain their toxicity from cyanogens, both from ingestion of Passiflora at the larval stage, and synthesised themselves (Engler et al, 2000). Much is already known about their biology, genetics and ecology. Their use of Passiflora host plants and pollen resources has been well documented (e.g. Benson et al, 1975; Gilbert, 1975; Smiley 1978a, 1978b; Mallet and Gilbert, 1995; Estrada and Jiggins, 2002). Other studies have looked at phylogenetics (see ‘phylogenetics of Heliconius’ section, this chapter); biogeography, (Brown, 1979), coevolution with plants (Gilbert, 1975) and colour pattern genetics (Sheppard et al, 1985; Mallet, 1989; Jiggins and McMillan, 1997; Linares, 1997; Gilbert, 2003). Heliconius are therefore ideal candidates for studies of speciation as they are well studied, widespread and hybridise naturally in the wild (Mallet et al, 2002). Here I investigate two separate, but related topics relevant to speciation between H. melpomene and H. cydno. Firstly, I use gene genealogies to look for evidence of introgression between the species, and secondly, I describe experiments designed to test whether F1 hybrids between the species undergo preferential predation. Mimicry and predation Aposematic coloration acts as a very strong predator deterrent in many taxa (Guilford, 1990). There are three main schools of thought on how warning colour may have arisen, driven by individual selection, kin selection and synergistic 17 selection (Servedio, 2000). Individual selection works on the basis that any individual has a chance of surviving a predatory attack, and will benefit if that predator learns that it is unpalatable and so avoids it in the future (Järvi et al, 1981a,b; Wiklund and Järvi, 1982; Sillén-Tullberg, 1985; Tullrot, 1994). Kin selection states that relatives of an aposematic individual may benefit from predator protection by possessing the same mutation for conspicuousness (Fisher, 1930; Waldman and Adler, 1979; Turner, 1984; Leimar et al, 1986; Tullrot, 1994). The third theory of warning colour evolution is that of synergistic, or ‘green beard’ selection (after Dawkins “green beard genes” 1976). This theory works on the basis that individuals will gain protection by having phenotypic and not necessarily genetic similarity to other aposematic prey (Guilford, 1985, 1988, 1990). Heliconius species are aposematically coloured Müllerian mimics, and gain protection against predators by mimicking other unpalatable species. This can be seen as a form of synergistic selection. Frequency dependent selection was first recognised by Poulton, (1890). There are two types of frequency dependent selection, positive and negative. Negative, or diversifying frequency dependent selection causes selection to be greatest on the commonest colour pattern. This form of selection has been implicated in maintaining colour pattern polymorphism (Haldane, 1955; Allen and Clarke, 1984; Allen, 1988). Positive, or purifying frequency dependent selection theory dictates that rare prey should be preferentially selected compared with commoner forms when found alongside each other, and may be responsible for keeping hybrid numbers low. 18 Direct observation of predation in the wild would be the best method of assessing whether a given butterfly taxon is preferentially taken over another, but this has been found to be virtually impossible to achieve. In one study using the butterfly species Vanessa atalanta, in 8000 days of observation spanning 22 years, only once was predation by a bird seen (Swanson and Monge-Najera, 2000). To overcome this problem, initial butterfly predation studies used artificial experimental set-ups, for instance using coloured pastry baits with varying levels of toxicity to test whether birds could learn to associate colour with taste, and to assess the importance of frequency on selection (e.g. Turner, 1961; Allen and Clarke, 1968; Coppinger, 1969; Brower, 1984; Greenwood et al, 1989; Weale et al, 2000). Many experiments use computer simulations or mathematical models to test which prey a predator would prefer (e.g. Turner et al, 1984; Schlenoff, 1985; Speed, 1999; Servedio, 2000; Bond and Kamil, 2002). Other studies have used real organisms, but in an unnatural setting, with dead prey items presented so that predators could choose between them (e.g. Brower et al, 1963). Some studies used beakmarks found on butterfly wings as an indirect measure of predation levels (Bowers et al, 1985; Pinheiro, 1997). Others used caged wild birds and live prey (Chai, 1986, 1988, 1996; Ritland, 1998). Very rarely have more natural situations been used to try to test theories of frequency dependent selection on butterflies, including mark-recapture studies (Brower and Brower, 1964; Benson, 1972; Mallet and Barton, 1989; Kapan, 2001), and releasing insects at bird perches (Chai, 1986; Pinheiro, 1996; Pinheiro, 1997). 19 Although there has been a large body of work concentrating on butterfly predation, most studies have concentrated on differences between species or colour morphs. The only attempt to test differential predation on hybrids was performed in a hybrid zone, using populations where the alternative form was absent (Mallet and Barton, 1989). In terms of speciation theory, we can learn relatively little from the results of such experiments, as they do not involve colour patterns relevant in a speciation context. Additionally, hybrid zones offer an unusual environment where hybrids are often present in large numbers. Often, the home range of local predators may be sufficient to have encountered colour forms from both sides of the hybrid zone, and recapture rates may not accurately reflect the numbers of butterflies released. This study did, however, show strong selection within species, suggesting that between species, the effects may be even stronger. A major gap in current knowledge about speciation in Heliconius is that of frequency dependent selection on hybrids. Theory predicts that F1 hybrids between aposematic taxa will be preferentially taken by predators, as they sport a novel colour pattern not recognised as unpalatable. If this prediction is true, purifying selection may play a great role in the process of speciation, as a strong post-mating isolation barrier. In Chapter 5, I attempt to rectify this gap in our knowledge. Phylogenetics of Heliconius The genus Heliconius has undergone a rapid radiation, which may be due to ecological adaptations, such as pollen feeding, traplining of food and oviposition sites, and longevity of adults (Gilbert, 1975; Brown, 1981). The phylogenetics of Heliconius species have been studied using wing-pattern alleles (Turner, 1981; 20 Sheppard et al 1985), morphological characters (Brown, 1981); and molecular data (18s, 28s: Lee et al, 1992: and COI, COII and wingless: Brower, 1994, 1996a,b, 1997; Brower and Egan, 1997; Brower & DeSalle, 1998). The melpomene/cydno group consisting of H. melpomene as the sister clade to the H. cydno group (H. heurippa, H. pachinus and H. cydno) has been upheld in most studies to date (e.g. Eltringham, 1916; Emsley, 1965; Brown, 1981; Brower, 1997). However, in Brower (1996a, 1996b) the H. cydno group of species is found within H. melpomene, rendering H. melpomene paraphyletic. A new study (Appendix 1) using mtDNA (COI/COII) and nuclear genes (Tpi and Mpi) reveals a pattern concordant with traditional studies at mtDNA loci, but shows a different pattern at Mpi and Tpi loci (see chapter 3 and Beltrán et al, 2002 for more detail). Heliconius melpomene and H. cydno H. melpomene and H. cydno are recently diverged sister species (Brower 1996a; Beltrán et al, 2002), broadly sympatric throughout their range in the Andes and Central America. Both are unpalatable and warningly-colored, and differ markedly in mimetic colour pattern. H. melpomene is typically red, black and yellow and mimics H. erato, whereas H. cydno is typically blue-black and white or blue-black and yellow (figure 1) and mimics species in the sapho/eleuchia group of Heliconius (Linares, 1996, 1997). 21 Figure 1 – Heliconius melpomene, H. cydno, and their mimics. Left = French Guiana races, centre and right = Panama races. Modified from Jiggins et al, 2001a. The two species differ in larval and adult ecology (Smiley, 1978a,b; Mallet et al, 1998a; Naisbit, 2001; Estrada and Jiggins, 2002), and show habitat segregation (Mallet et al, 1998a; Estrada and Jiggins, 2002). H. melpomene inhabits light second growth forest and gaps caused by tree falls or rivers; whereas H. cydno prefers dense, primary forest. However, there is considerable overlap and the two often fly together. Jiggins et al (2001a) summarise speciation between these species and show that pre- and post-mating isolation between them has resulted from an adaptive shift in ecology and mimicry leading to pre-mating isolation. In association with partial hybrid sterility, there has been subsequent evolution of assortative mating between sympatric populations. For a summary diagram of factors contributing to speciation between these species, see figure 2. There are around 29 described races, or colour morphs of H. melpomene, and 16 of H. cydno (Brown, 1979). The two species hybridise and backcross regularly in the wild (Mallet et al, 2002), although at a rate of less than 1/1000 (Mallet et al, 1998a). The number of 22 hybrids naturally occurring is kept low largely due to strong assortative mating (Jiggins et al, 2001a). 23 Figure 2 - Factors contributing to speciation in H.melpomene and H.cydno (proposed approximate order of events) 24 Introgression and phylogenetics In H. melpomene and H. cydno, speciation is strictly not yet complete under the biological species concept (Mayr, 1963), as hybrids are still found in the wild, albeit at a very low rate. This makes these species ideal candidates for population genetic studies looking for evidence of introgression. Heliconius butterflies are unpalatable to predators, and have a diverse array of colour patterns that are typically mimetic of other Heliconius species or of ithomiine butterflies. It is clear that most of these patterns have evolved independently in separate lineages, using different architecture, for instance, in the famous mimicry between H. erato and H. melpomene (Turner, 1984; Sheppard et al, 1985). However, in some related species, the same colour patterns occur again and again, with related races and species apparently re-using the same genetic machinery to produce the same mimetic pattern. These colour pattern/phylogeny anomalies appear especially frequently in the melpomene/silvaniform Heliconius, a group in which most of the known wildcaught hybrids and backcrosses between species are found (Mallet et al, 1998a, b). This suggests introgression is likely. H. melpomene and H. cydno typically belong to completely different mimicry rings; however, several races of H. cydno in E. Ecuador and E. Colombia (sometimes separated as the species H. tristero, H.timareta and H. heurippa), appear to have adopted melpomene-like colour patterns similar to those present in some races of melpomene. Another phylogenetic anomaly is the adoption of melpomene-like colour patterns by H. elevatus, a member of the silvaniform group, and only distantly related to the sister pair H. melpomene and H. cydno. Its grouping with the silvaniforms is supported by mtDNA as well as morphology (Brown, 1981; Brower and Egan, 1997; Beltrán et al, 2002). Again, there is evidence for hybridisation and backcrossing between 25 melpomene and silvaniforms. Both phylogenetic anomalies suggest either introgression being exploited as a source of ready-made adaptations (Gilbert, 2003), or independent polyphyletic evolution of apparently homologous colour patterns. Introgression between H. melpomene and H. cydno should theoretically be possible as hybrids of both sexes are viable. Although female F1 hybrids are sterile in accordance with Haldane’s Rule, male F1 hybrids are fertile and readily backcross in both directions (Naisbit et al, 2001b). In Chapters 3 and 4. I use gene genealogies to investigate whether there is evidence for introgression between these two species. 26 References Allen, J.A. (1988) Frequency dependent selection by predators. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. 319: 485-503. Allen, J.A. and Clarke, B.C. (1968) Evidence for apostatic selection by wild passerines. Nature 220: 501-502. Allen, J.A. and Clarke, B.C. (1984) Frequency dependent selection: homage to E.B. Poulton. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 23: 15-18. Arntzen, J.W. and Wallis, G.P. (1991) Restricted gene flow in a moving hybrid zone of the newts Tritarus cristatus and T.marmoratus in Western France Evolution 45: 805-826. Barton, N.H. (2001) Speciation. TREE 16(7): 325. Barton, N.H. and Hewitt, G.M. (1989) Adaptation, speciation and hybrid zones. Nature 341: 497-503. Beltrán, M., Jiggins, C.D., Bull, V., Linares, M., Mallet, J., McMillan, W.O. and Bermingham, E. (2002) Phylogenetic discordance at the species boundary: Comparative gene genealogies among rapidly radiating Heliconius butterflies. Mol.Biol.Evol. 19(12): 2176-2190. Benson, W.W. (1972) Natural selection for Müllerian mimicry in Heliconius erato in Costa Rica. Science 176: 936-939. Benson, W.W., Brown, K.S. and Gilbert, L.E. (1975) Coevolution of plants and herbivores: passion flower butterflies Evolution 29: 659-680. 27 Bond, A.B. and Kamil, A.C. (2002) Visual predators select for crypticity and polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature 415: 609-613. Bowers, M.D., Brown, I.L. and Whey, D. (1985) Bird predation as a selective agent in a butterfly population. Evolution 39: 93-103. Brower, A.V.Z. (1994) Phylogeny of Heliconius butterflies inferred from mitochondrial DNA sequences (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) Mol.Phylogenet.Evol. 3:159-174. Brower, A.V.Z. (1996a) Parallel race formation and the evolution of mimicry in Heliconius butterflies: A phylogenetic hypothesis from mitochondrial DNA sequences. Evolution 50(1): 195-221. Brower, A.V.Z. (1996b) A new mimetic species of Heliconius (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae), from southeastern Colombia, as revealed by cladistic analysis of mitochondrial DNA sequences. Zool.J.Linn.Soc. 116: 317-332. Brower, A.V.Z. (1997) The evolution of ecologically important characters in Heliconius butterflies (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae): a cladistic review. Zool.J.Linn.Soc. 19: 457-472. Brower, A.V.Z. and DeSalle, R. (1998) Patterns of mitochondrial versus nuclear DNA sequence divergence among nymphalid butterflies: the utility of wingless as a source of characters for phylogenetic inference. Insect Mol.Biol. 7: 73-82. Brower, A.V.Z. and Egan, M.G. (1997) Cladistic analysis of Heliconius butterflies and relatives (Nymphalidae: Heliconiiti): a revised phylogenetic position for 28 Eueides based on sequences from mtDNA and a nuclear gene Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B 264: 969-977. Brower, L.P. (1984) Chemical defense in butterflies. In: Vane-Wright.R.I and Ackery.P.R (eds.). The Biology of Butterflies. Academic Press, New York pp. 109-134. Brower, L.P. and Brower, J.V.Z. (1964) Birds, butterflies, and plant poisons: a study in ecological chemistry. Zoologica 49: 137-159. Brower, L.P., Brower, J.V.Z. and Collins, C.T. (1963) Experimental studies of mimicry. 7. Relative palatability and müllerian mimicry among neotropical butterflies of the subfamily Heliconiinae. Zoologica 48: 65-84. Brown, K.S. (1979) Ecologia Geográfica e Evolução nas Florestas Neotropicais. 2 vols. (Tese apresentada à Universidade Estadual de Campinas como parte das exigências de um Concurso de Livre Docência, area de Ecologia). Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, Brazil. Brown, K.S. (1981) The biology of Heliconius and related genera. Annu.Rev.Entomol. 26: 427-456. Brown, K.S., Sheppard, P.M. and Turner, J.R.G. (1974) Quaternary refugia in tropical America: evidence from race formation in Heliconius butterflies Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B 187: 369-378. Chai, P. (1986) Field observations and feeding experiments on the response of rufous-tailed jacamars (Galbula ruficauda) to free-flying butterflies in a tropical rainforest. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 29: 161-189. 29 Chai, P. (1988) Wing colouration of free-flying Neotropical butterflies as a signal learned by a specialized avian predator. Biotropica 20: 20-30. Chai, P. (1996) Butterfly visual characteristics and ontogeny of responses to butterflies by a specialized tropical bird. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 59: 37-67. Coppinger, R.P. (1969) The effect of experience and novelty on avian feeding behaviour with reference to the evolution of warning colouration in butterflies. I. Reactions of wild-caught adult blue-jays to novel insects. Behaviour 35: 45-60. Coyne, J.A. and Orr, H.A. (1998) The evolutionary genetics of speciation Phil.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.B 353: 287-305. Darwin, C. (1859) On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray. Dawkins, R. (1976) The Selfish Gene, Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, UK. Dobzhansky, T. (ed.) (1937) Genetics and the origin of species. Columbia Univ. Press. Doi, M., Matsuda, M., Tomaru, M., Matsubayashi, H. and Oguma, Y. (2001) A locus for discrimination behaviour causing sexual selection in Drosophila. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 98(12): 6714-6719. Eltringham, H. (1916) On specific and mimetic relationships in the genus Heliconius. Trans.Entomol.Soc.Lond. pp. 101-148. Emsley, M.G. (1965) Speciation in Heliconius (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae): morphology and geographic distribution. Zoologica (NY) 50: 191-254. 30 Endler, J.A. (1977) Geographic variation, speciation and clines. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, USA. Engler, H.S., Spencer, K.C. and Gilbert, L.E. (2000) Preventing cyanide release from leaves Nature 406: 144-145. Estrada, C. and Jiggins, C.D. (2002) Patterns of pollen feeding and habitat preference among Heliconius species Ecol.Entomol. 27: 448-456. Fisher, R.A. (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon, Oxford, UK. Gilbert, L.E. (1975) Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants. In: Gilbert, L.E., Raven, P.R. eds.. Coevolution of Animals and Plants. Univ. of Texas Press, Austin, TX, USA. pp. 210-240. Gilbert, L.E. (2003) Adaptive novelty through introgression in Heliconius wing patterns: evidence for a shared genetic “tool box” from synthetic hybrid zones and a theory of diversification, in Ecology and evolution taking flight: butterflies as model systems, edited by C.L.Boggs, W.B.Watt and P.R.Ehrlich. Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago. Grant, B.R. and Grant, P.R. (1993) Evolution of Darwin’s finches caused by a rare climatic event. Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B 251: 111-117. Grant, P.R. (1993) Hybridization of Darwin’s finches on Isla Daphne Major, Galapagos. Phil.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.B 340: 127-139. Greenwood, J.J.D., Cotton, P.A. and Wilson, D.M. (1989) Frequency dependent selection on aposematic prey – some experiments. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 36(12): 213-226. 31 Guilford, T. (1985) Is kin selection involved in the evolution of warning coloration? Oikos 45: 31-36. Guilford, T. (1988) The evolution of conspicuous coloration. Am. Nat. 131: S7-S21. Guilford, T. (1990) The evolution of aposematism. In: D.L.Evans and J.O.Schmidt eds. Insect defenses. Adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators. State Univ. of New York Press, Albany, NY, USA. Haldane, J.B.S. (1955) On the biochemistry of heterosis, and the stability of polymorphism. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 144: 217-220. Harrison, R.G. (1993) Hybrids and hybrid zones: historical perspective. In: Harrison.R.G. ed. Hybrid zones and the Evolutionary Process. Oxford Univ. Press, New York, USA pp. 3-12. Järvi, T., Sillén-Tullberg, B., and Wiklund, C. (1981a) The cost of being aposometic: an experimental study of predation on larvae of Papilio machaon by the great tit Parus major. Oikos 36: 267-272. Järvi, T., Sillén-Tullberg, B., and Wiklund, C. (1981b) Individual versus kin selection for aposometic coloration: a reply to Harvey and Paxton. Oikos 37: 393-395. Jiggins, C.D., McMillan, W.O., Neukirchen, N. and Mallet, J. (1996) What can hybrid zones tell us about speciation? The case of Heliconius erato and H.himera (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Biol.J.Linn.Soc. 59: 221-242. Jiggins, C.D. and Mallet, J. (2000) Bimodal hybrid zones and speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 15: 250-255. 32 Jiggins, C.D. and McMillan, W.O. (1997) The genetic basis of an adaptive radiation: warning colour in two Heliconius species. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264: 11671175. Jiggins, C.D., Naisbit, R.E., Coe, R.L. and Mallet, J. (2001a) Reproductive isolation caused by colour pattern mimicry. Nature (Lond.) 411: 302-305. Jiggins, C.D., Linares, M., Naisbit, R.E., Salazar, C., Yang, Z. and Mallet, J. (2001b) Sex-linked hybrid sterility in a butterfly. Evolution 55: 1631-1638. Kapan, D.D. (2001) Three-butterfly system provides a field test of müllerian mimicry, Nature 409: 338-340. Lee, C.S., McCool, B.A., Moore, J.L., Hillis, D.M. and Gilbert, L.E. (1992) Phylogenetic study of heliconiine butterflies based on morphology and restriction analysis of ribosomal RNA genes. Zool.J.Linn.Soc. 106: 17-31. Leimar, O., Enquist, M. and Sillén-Tullberg, B. (1986) Evolutionary stability of aposometic coloration and prey unprofitability: a theoretical analysis. Am. Nat. 128: 469-490. Linares, M. (1996) The genetics of the mimetic coloration in the butterfly Heliconius cydno weymeri. J.Hered. 87: 142-149. Linares, M. (1997) The ghost of mimicry past: laboratory reconstitution of an extinct butterfly ‘race’ Heredity 78: 628-635. Mallet, J., Neukirchen,W. and Linares, M. (2002) Hybrids between species of Heliconius and Euides butterflies: a database. http://www.ucl.ac.uk/taxome/hyb/ 33 Mallet, J. (1993) Speciation, Raciation, and Color pattern Evolution in Heliconius Butterflies: Evidence from Hybrid Zones, In R.G.Harrison (ed.): Hybrid zones and the evolutionary process. Oxford Univ. Press, NY. pp. 226-260. Mallet, J. (1989) The genetics of warning colour in Peruvian hybrid zones of Heliconius erato and H. melpomene. Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B 236: 163-185. Mallet, J. and Barton, N.H. (1989) Strong natural selection in a warning colour hybrid zone. Evolution 43: 421-431. Mallet, J. and Gilbert, L.E. (1995) Why are there so many mimicry rings? Correlations between habitat, behaviour and mimicry in Heliconius butterflies Biol.J.Linn.Soc 55: 159-180. Mallet, J., Jiggins, C.D. and McMillan, W.O. (1998a) Mimicry and warning colour at the boundary between races and species. In Howard, D.J. and Berlocher, S.H. (eds.) Endless Forms: Species and Speciation. Oxford University Press. pp.390-403. Mallet, J., McMillan, W.O., and Jiggins, C.D. (1998b). Estimating the mating behaviour of a pair of hybridizing Heliconius species in the wild. Evolution 52: 503-510. Mayr, E. (1963) Animal Species and Evolution. Belknap, Cambridge, MA. Muller, F. (1879) Ituna and Thyridia; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies Trans. Ent. Soc. Lond. xx-xxix. Naisbit, R.E., Jiggins, C.D. and Mallet, J. (2001) Disruptive sexual selection against hybrids contributes to speciation between Heliconius cydno and Heliconius melpomene. Proc.R.Soc.Lond.B 268: 1-6. 34 Naisbit, R.E. (2001) Ecological divergence and speciation in Heliconius cydno and H. melpomene. Doctoral dissertation, University College London, London, UK. Naisbit, R.E., Jiggins, C.D., Linares, M., Salazar, C. and Mallet, J. (2002) Hybrid sterility, Haldane’s rule, and speciation in Heliconius cydno and H. melpomene. Genetics 161: 1517-1526. Palopoli, M.F. and Wu, C.I. (1994) Genetics of hybrid male sterility between Drosophila sibling species – A complex web of epistasis is revealed in interspecific studies, Genetics 138(2): 329-341. Pinheiro, C.E.G. (1996) Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (Tyrannus melancholicus, Tyrannidae). Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 59: 351-365. Pinheiro, C.E.G. (1997) Unpalatability, mimicry and escaping ability in neotropical butterflies: experiments with wild predators. Doctoral dissertation, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK. Poulton, E.B. (1890) The Colours of Animals. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trübner & Co Ltd pp. 46-47. Ritland, D.B. (1998) Mimicry-related predation on two viceroy butterfly (Limenitis archippus) phenotypes. Am. Mid. Nat. 140(1): 1-20. Saetre, G.P., Moum, T., Boures, S., Kral, M., Adamjam, M. and Moreno, J. (1997) A sexually selected character displacement in flycatchers reinforces premating isolation. Nature 387: 589-592. 35 Schlenoff, D.H. (1985) The startle responses of blue jays to Catocala (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) prey models. Anim. Behav. 33: 1057-1067. Schluter, D. (2001) Ecology and the origin of species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16: 372380. Seehausen, O., van Alphen, J.J. and Witte, F. (1997) Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. Science 277: 18081811. Servedio, M.R. (2000) The effects of predator learning, forgetting, and recognition errors on the evolution of warning coloration, Evolution 54(3): 751-763. Sheppard, P.M., Turner, J.R.G., Brown, K.S., Benson, W.W. and Singer, M.C. (1985) Genetics and the evolution of mullerian mimicry in Heliconius butterflies. Phil.Trans.R.Soc.Lond.B 308: 433-613. Sillén-Tullberg, B. (1985) Higher survival of an aposometic than of a cryptic form of a distasteful bug. Oecologia 67: 411-415. Smiley, J.T. (1978a) The host plant ecology of Heliconius butterflies in Northeastern Costa Rica. Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin, Austin TX, USA. Smiley, J.T. (1978b) Plant chemistry and the evolution of host specificity: new evidence from Heliconius and Passiflora. Science 201: 745-747. Sota, T. and Vogler, A.P. (2001) Incongruence of Mitochondrial and Nuclear Gene Trees in the Carabid Beetles Ohomopterus. Syst. Biol. 50(1): 39-59. Speed, M.P. (1999) Robot predators in virtual ecologies: the importance of memory in mimicry studies. Anim. Behav. 57: 203-213. 36 Stratton, G.E. and Uetz, G.W. (1986) The inheritance of courtship behaviour and its role as a reproductive isolating mechanism in two species of Schizocosa wolf spiders (Aranae; Lycosidae). Evolution 40: 129-141. Swanson, H.F. and Monge-Najera, J. (2000) The effects of methodological limitations in the study of butterfly behaviour and demography: a daily study of Vanessa atalanta (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) for 22 years Revista de Biologia Tropical 48(2-3): 605-613. Tullrot, A. (1994) The evolution of unpalatability and warning coloration in softbodied marine invertebrates. Evolution 48: 925-928. Turner, E.R.A. (1961) Survival values of different methods of camouflage as shown in a model population. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 136: 273-284. Turner, J.R.G. (1981) Adaptation and evolution in Heliconius: A defense of NeoDarwinism. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 12: 99-121. Turner, J.R.G. (1984) Mimicry: the palatability spectrum and its consequences. In: R.I.Vane-Wright and P.R.Ackery, eds. The biology of butterflies. Academic Press, NY, USA pp. 141-162. Turner, J.R.G., Kearnet, E.P. and Exton, L.S. (1984) Mimicry and the Monte Carlo predator: the palatability spectrum and the origins of mimicry. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 23: 247-268. Via, S. (2001) Sympatric speciation in animals: the ugly duckling grows up. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16: 381-390. Waldman, B. and Adler, K. (1979) Toad tadpoles associate preferentially with siblings. Nature 282: 611-613. 37 Weale, M.E., Whitwell, D., Raison, H.E., Raymond, D.L. and Allen, J.A. (2000) The influence of density on frequency dependent food selection: a comparison of four experiments with wild birds. Oecologia 124: 391-395. Wiklund, C. and Järvi, T. (1982) Survival of distasteful insects after being attacked by naïve birds: a reappraisal of the theory of aposematic coloration evolving through individual selection. Evolution 36: 998-1002. 38