NEUROENDOCRINE AND VASCULAR PROCESSES DURING SEXUAL AROUSAL AND ORGASM Tillmann H.C. Krüger1, Manfred Schedlowski1, and Michael S. Exton1,2 1 Department of Medical Psychology, University of Essen, 45122 Essen, Germany 2 Eli Lilly Pty Ltd, 112 Wharf Rd Sydney Australia 2 ABSTRACT A series of studies in our laboratory have reliably demonstrated that plasma prolactin (PRL) concentrations are substantially increased for over an hour following orgasm in both men and women, but unchanged following sexual arousal without orgasm. As chronic elevations of PRL (hyperprolactinemia) reduce sexual activity and functions, we propose that the PRL response to orgasm may play an important role in regulating post-orgasmic sexual drive, as a peripheral regulatory factor for reproductive function, and/or a feedback mechanism that signals CNS centres controlling sexual arousal and behaviour. Current studies are being undertaken to test this hypothesis. 3 NEUROENDOCRINE RESPONSE TO ACUTE SEXUAL AROUSAL Despite investigation now spanning well over 30 years, little consensus has been reached regarding the endocrine control of sexual arousal in healthy humans. Historically, the approach to this question has been to measure the endocrine response to various modes of sexual stimulation. This undertaking allows an examination of hormones that may be involved in the up- or down-regulation of a sexual response, depending on the timing and magnitude of such changes. The numerous studies that have spanned over three decades of research have shown a high level of agreement in regard to cardiovascular responses to sexual activity (Carminchael et al., 1994; Nemec et al., 1976; Whipple et al., 1992; Littler et al., 1974), but in contrast, a remarkable lack of consistency in regard to sympathetic, pituitary and gonadal hormones been notable for lack of consistency in conclusions made between studies (Levi, 1969; Wiedeking, 1977; Carani et al., 1990; Purvis et al., 1976; Brown et al., 1975; Rowland et al., 1987; La ferla et al., 1978; Stoleru et al., 1993; Lee et al., 1974; Lincoln, 1974; Pirke et al., 1974; Fox et al., 1972; Heiman et al., 1991; Blaicher et al., 1999; Carmichael et al., 1994; 10- 20). Nevertheless, we must be cognisant that technical advances, combined with a number of very different methodological approaches, contribute significantly to the variance between data from different experiments. INCONSISTENT METHODOLOGY Studies examining the neuroendocrine response to sexual arousal and orgasm have employed many different methodologies, thus making general interpretations difficult. One potential confound amongst studies is the contrasting methods employed for the induction of sexual arousal. Researchers have employed the viewing of stimulating films, imagery of fantasies, masturbation, and coitus. These methods clearly demonstrate different characteristics of sexual stimulation, with differences in duration and intensity of exposure, as well as the 4 amount of physical contact. Additionally, differences between such studies are further compounded by some studies requesting participants to achieve orgasm, whilst others did not. Together, such factors have contributed to the inability to directly compare data generated from different laboratories. Additionally, a primary concern limiting the evaluation of results from these studies is the method of blood collection. Blood has often been sampled at single discrete time points, sometimes with the experimenter entering the experimental room. Such a methodology has a number of potential deficits. Firstly, short-term alterations of certain neuroendocrine variables may be missed by using this technique. Secondly, entering the experimental room may cause the participant undue concern that may potentially contribute to any observed endocrine alterations. Thirdly, punctual blood sampling may induce physical discomfort that may also impact hormonal status. Therefore, we designed a method for the examination of the neuroendocrine response to sexual arousal and orgasm in healthy males and females (Krüger et al., 1998; Exton et al., 1999; 2000; 2001). We established this paradigm so as to eliminate difficulties due to punctual blood sampling, as well as the influence of the presence of the experimenter. By keeping these factors constant we are able to compare factors that are incomparable in the literature – namely the effect of different modes of stimulation, as well as the influence of orgasm on endocrine alterations following sexual arousal. IMPROVED METHODOLOGY For the examination of the endocrine response to sexual arousal and orgasm we developed a laboratory model of continuous blood sampling that we formerly established in experimental field studies. Most experiments were conducted in ten healthy male or female volunteers. 5 Each experiment was conducted in participants naïve to the experimental conditions. All subjects were exclusively heterosexual, and reported a relaxed attitude towards masturbation and pornography. All volunteers underwent an intensive non-structured clinical interview to exclude participants with confounding physical or mental health problems. Subjects with drug or alcohol abuse, medication intake, or sexual dysfunctions were excluded from participation. Volunteers were requested to refrain from any kind of sexual activity and to avoid alcoholic beverages or other drugs 24 h prior to the laboratory investigation. The study paradigm is displayed in Figure 1. A balanced cross-over design was implemented, involving two sessions on consecutive days, with each session commencing at 15:00 h. Subjects laid on a comfortable bed in front of a video screen, with the head propped by pillows to allow viewing of the video. In the control session volunteers viewed a neutral documentary video, whilst the experimental session was composed of three video sequences, each lasting 20 min. The first and last sections of this tape were composed of the identical documentary film. The middle 20 min of the experimental video was a pornographic film that showed different heterosexual couples having sexual intercourse. To ensure accuracy of measurements and privacy of the participants, our method of continuous blood withdrawal was achieved by the use of a small portable pump. An intravenous cannula was inserted into a brachial vein, and then connected to heparinised silicon tubing that passed through the test room wall into the adjoining room. The use of the minipump allowed adjustment of blood flow, which was typically 1-2 ml/min. Blood was collected into EDTA tubes, which allows variation of the time period of each collection, simply via replacing the tubes. For the current set of experiments we changed tubes every 10 min to allow a time kinetic of endocrine variables. Samples were centrifuged at 4°C and plasma stored at -70°C until assayed. 6 By using the method of blood collection we established an accurate paradigm of measuring hormone concentrations over a period of sexual arousal. This allowed a comparison of various methods for inducing sexual arousal on hormone concentrations, and furthermore, the impact of orgasm on these responses. Thus, we completed three sets of experiments in both males and females, altering the stimulation paradigm: 1. Following 10 min viewing of the pornographic video (anticipatory phase), subjects in the experimental session were asked to masturbate until orgasm. 2. Following 10 min viewing of the pornographic video, subjects in the experimental session were asked to have coitus until orgasm. Heterosexual couples participated in this experiment. In this paradigm, subjects who were being examined for endocrine changes laid comfortably on the bed, whilst all the movement was conducted by the partner. This reduced any changes in hormone concentrations that may have been due to movement, allowing direct examination of the specific neuroendocrine response to sexual arousal and orgasm. 3. Subjects in the experimental session watched the pornographic film for 20 min, but did not masturbate to orgasm. ENDOCRINE RESPONSE TO ORGASM: PROLACTIN MAY BE A REGULATOR OF POST-ORGAMIC SEXUAL DRIVE In our laboratory, sexual stimulation consistently produced high levels of subjective sexual arousal, as assessed by visual-analogue scale. Furthermore, this result was corroborated by objective measurements of sexual arousal in females, using vaginal photoplethysmography. These data give face validity to our endocrine measurements, as they reflect hormonal changes that would occur in “real world” sexual situations. 7 Measurement of cardiovascular responses and sympathetic hormones also demonstrate a consistent response to sexual arousal and orgasm, increasing heart rate, blood pressure, adrenaline and noradrenaline, which return rapidly to control levels after orgasm. Further consistent responses were observed in cortisol, FSH, LH, testosterone, -endorphin, progesterone and estradiol: orgasm produced no reliable alteration of these hormones. In contrast, PRL was shown to be consistently and specifically altered by orgasm in both males and females. Figure 2 demonstrates the PRL response to various forms of sexual arousal in both genders. Across all experiments, no changes in PRL were observed during the first 20 minutes of documentary film. Furthermore, no changes of any significance were observed following the first 10 minutes of pornographic film viewing. However, the generation of orgasm via either self-masturbation or coitus produced pronounced increases in PRL concentrations in peripheral circulation of both males and females. Additionally, these alterations remained significantly elevated 60 minutes following orgasm. Furthermore, the PRL response is clearly specific to orgasm, as sexual arousal alone (both film and masturbation without orgasm) induced no changes of PRL in either males or females. This was confirmed in a recent study where we employed a 2-minute blood sampling interval (Krüger et al., 2003). Interestingly, in this study, we observed for the first time in our laboratory an orgasm-induced increase in oxytocin, although this was small and nonsignificant. As this hormone has been considered a responsive hormone to sexual arousal and orgasm, these data support the significance of the pronounced and long-lasting impact of orgasm on PRL. Indeed, together the data suggest that PRL may play a role in the acute regulation of further sexual arousal and/or reproductive functions following orgasm. DOES PROLACTIN REGULATE SEXUAL FUNCTION AND BEHAVIOUR? 8 Support from Physiology The physiological role of the pituitary peptide hormone PRL contributes to the feasibility that it may be involved in the acute regulation of sexual drive. Indeed, although PRL was considered initially to be a mechanism of lactation (Stricker & Greuter, 1928), it has broad physiological importance, being ascribed over 300 biological functions (Bole-Feysot et al., 1998). Unlike most most pituitary hormones whose release is triggered by specific releasing hormones from the hypothalamus, PRL is under tonic inhibitory control by the hypothalamus. Of particular importance to PRL control of sexual function is that the primary inhibitory input is dopamine (Maruyama et al., 1999), a neurotransmitter that is intimately involved in the regulation of sexual drive and behaviour. That PRL may be involved in regulation of sexual drive is underscored by the expression of the PRL receptor within CNS structures that are known to regulate sexual behaviour. Specifically, PRL receptors are located in the hippocampus, cortex, amygdala and various hypothalamic nuclei (Roky et al., 1996; Pi et al., 1999a; 1999b). In addition, PRL receptors have been characterised within both male and female reproductive organs. In addition to the mammary gland, PRL receptors are located in females in the ovary and uterus, and in males in the testis, epididymis, and prostate (Yoshimura et al., 1992; Prigent-Tressier et al., 1996; Howell Skala et al., 2000; Reddy et al., 1985; Perez-Villamil et al., 1980). Clinical support Key to the theory that acute PRL may regulate acute sexual drive is an extrapolation from extensive animal and clinical evidence of the impact of chronic elevations of PRL (hyperprolactinemia) on sexual behaviour and function. Animal data consistently demonstrates that hyperprolactinemia impairs sexual function on both motivational and 9 physiological levels in the male (increased latency to mounting and ejaculation, decreased frequency of mounts, intromissions, and ejaculations) and female rat (lordosis behaviour) (Doherty et al., 1990; 1985; 1986; 1985; Kooy et al., 1988). This animal evidence is well supported clinically. Hyperprolactinemia commonly occurs during pregnancy and lactation. Additionally, hyperprolactinemia may be produced by pathological mechanisms, such as prolactinoma, para- and suprasellar tumours affecting hypothalamic PRL regulatory factors, “empty sella”-syndrome, severe primary hypothyreosis and chronic renal failure. The development of a prolactinoma, an adenoma of pituitary lactotrophs, is one of the most common reasons for hyperprolactinemia. Hyperprolactinemia is associated with pronounced reductions of both sexual motivation and function. Elevated levels of PRL inhibit GnRH pulsatility (Sauder et al., 1984) and thus low gonadotropin production, amenorrhoea, gynaecomastia and galactorrhoea in some women. In men hyperprolactinemia is commonly associated with low testosterone levels and oligospermia (Buvat et al., 1985; Walsh and Pullan, 1997; Sobrinho, 1993). Although some experimental evidence suggests that hyperprolactinemia suppresses physiological reproductive functions whilst maintaining sexual drive (Carani et al., 1996), other studies clearly indicate that chronic PRL elevation also negatively impact upon sexual libido (Koppelman et al., 1987). This has been particularly observed in clinical psychopharmacology. For example, new generation of antidepressants such as Selective-Serotonin-ReuptakeInhibitors (SSRIs) produce hyperprolactinemia (Rosen et al., 1999). The increased PRL secretion induced by this medication is associated with dramatically reduced sexual appetence and delayed ejaculation in men (Rosen et al., 1995; Waldiger et al., 1998). In women 10 symptoms range from decreased sexual drive to orgasmic disturbances such as anorgasmia and delayed orgasm (Shen et al., 1995; Montejo-Gonzalez et al., 1997). Further, typical neuroleptics, and some atypicals, used to treat schizophrenia, produce a strong elevation of plasma PRL. Hyperprolactinemia is associated with loss of libido, erectile dysfunction and anorgasmia, with this effect not observed with atypical neuroleptics that do not elevate PRL (eg olanzapine). Underscoring the importance of PRL in regulating sexual function, dopaminergic agonists have become a common approach to the treatment of hyperprolactinemia. Recently, the first commonly utilised dopaminergic agonist bromocriptine has been replaced by cabergoline, which produces striking normalisation of hyperprolactinemia (Verhelst et al., 1999). Indeed, carbergoline has recently been shown in a number of large studies to normalise PRL levels and thereby restore libido and gonadal function in hyperprolactinemic patients (De Rosa et al., 1998). Together, these data suggest a strong association between chronic elevations of PRL and marked suppression of both sexual drive and gonadal functions. This raises the possibility that acute elevations of PRL following orgasm may play a role in the regulation of sexual arousal and function. Indeed, some evidence suggests that this hormone may play an integral role in acute regulation of sexual behaviour. COMPARING APPLES WITH APPLES: IMPLICATIONS OF HYPERPROLACTINEMIA FOR PRL RELEASE FOLLOWING ORGASM While the effects of chronic elevations of PRL on sexual drive and reproductive function are well described, the relevance of acute changes in PRL for sexual activity is equivocal. This 11 raises the question of whether it is feasible to extrapolate the well-known effects of chronic prolactin elevation to acute changes. Indeed, animal models have demonstrated that acute increases in peripheral PRL, particularly at levels that are in the normal physiological range (e.g. 50ng/ml), stimulate the sexual behaviour of male rats (Drago and Dissandrello, 2000). In contrast, other reports have demonstrated that acute increases of peripheral PRL increase, decrease, or have no effect on rat sexual behaviour (Cruz-Cassalas et al., 1999; Nasello et al., 1997). Thus, the biological effects of acute increases in PRL on sexual behaviour are unclear. Nevertheless, some evidence suggests that orgasm-induced PRL secretion may contribute to the acute regulation of sexual arousal and reproductive function. Indeed, acute elevations of this peptide may have both peripheral and central consequences for sexual function and arousal. Peripheral actions In addition to the well characterised impact of PRL in regulating reprodtive functions such as spermatocyte-spermatid conversion in germ cells, enhanced energy metabolism in spermatozoa, transport of ejaculated and epididymal spermatozoa, formation and destruction of the corpus luteum, uterine endometrial development and blastocyst implantation (BoleFeysot et al., 1998; Outhit et al., 1993), some evidence suggests that acute PRL may impact sexual functions. Specifically, in males, the acute release of PRL following orgasm may impact sexual behaviour via direct action on the penile tissue. Although not extensively examined, some data clearly demonstrate that acute increases in PRL inhibit erectile function via inhibition of smooth muscle relaxation of the corpus cavernosum (Aoki et al., 1995; Ra et al., 1996). This suggests that an acute increase in PRL levels may participate in penile 12 detumescence. However, our data showing that acute increases in PRL remain over 60 minutes following orgasm suggest a complex interaction of this hormone with neural regulation of erection. It is plausible that initial PRL increases may contribute to penile detumescence, which is then reversed by other central and local factors. This would concur with the known physiological regulation of erection, which represents a balance between different neural and endocrine inputs at multiple levels (Andersson and Wagner, 1995). Indeed, the effects of PRL are likely to interact both with other neuroendocrine factors as well as behavioural variables, as drugs inhibiting basal PRL have been shown not to alter erectile functions in impotent males (March, 1979; Cooper, 1977). However, it must be noted that the expression of PRL receptors in the corpus cavernosum has yet to be demonstrated. Thus, these data show that in contrast to the negative impact of hyperprolactinemia on fertility, the acute increase in PRL following orgasm in males and females may contribute to an environment that ensures successful conception. However, the acute increase of PRL may also provide a feedback signal to nuclei in the central nervous system controlling both peripheral reproductive functions as well as centres involved in the regulation of sexual drive. Central actions The three major dopaminergic networks in the CNS may form the primary targets for peripheral PRL feedback. These networks are firstly, the neuroendocrine hypothalamic and incerto-hypothalamic neurons (diencephalic; DC), secondly, the mesolimbocortical dopaminergic neurons (MLC), and finally, the nigrostriatal dopamine system (NS). Together, these systems are recognised as playing a major role in modulating sexual motivation, behaviour, and function, with both sensory stimulation and copulation producing dopaminergic activity in all three systems (Hull et al., 1999; Mas, 1995). The central role of 13 dopamine in regulating sexual activity is underscored by the pronounced impact of dopaminergic drugs on both animal and human sexual function (Bancroft, 1999; Meston and Frohlich, 2000). Thus, these systems present as primary targets for PRL feedback to the CNS, and some evidence indeed suggests that the activity of each system is modified by PRL. The most recognised pathway of PRL feedback to the CNS is to hypothalamic neuroendocrine neurons. Three populations of hypothalamic dopaminergic neurons regulate PRL release (De Maria et al., 1998; 1999): Tuberoinfundibular dopaminergic neurons (TIDA) originating in the arcuate nucleus (ARN) and terminating in the median eminence (ME) (Fuxe, 1964); tuberohypophyseal dopaminergic neurons (THDA) extending from the rostral ARN and terminating in the intermediate (IL) and neural (NL) lobes of the pituitary gland (Bjorklund et al., 1973); and periventricular-hypophyseal dopaminergic neurons arising in the hypothalamic periventricular nucleus (PeVN) and terminating exclusively in the IL (Goudreau, 1992). These neurons express PRL receptors (Freeman et al., 2000), thus providing the prerequisites for a feedback loop by peripheral PRL. Although PRL is not able to pass the blood-brain-barrier it can be secreted by the choroids plexus into the cerebrospinal fluid or passes the area postrema and subsequently reach the brain tissue (Sobrinho, 1993). The capacity of PRL to reach the CNS is shown by a series of studies demonstrating that subcutaneously administered ovine PRL activates all three populations of hypothalamic dopaminergic neurons. Importantly, this effect occurred within one hour, thus contradicting the notion that peripheral PRL requires up to five days to reach optimal brain concentrations by active transport mechanisms (Walsh et al., 1987). Thus, PRL forms a negative feedback loop to control its own release, similar to pathways observed for many other pituitary hormones. 14 In addition to feedback on neurons controlling its own secretion, PRL also may feedback to dopaminergic systems that have been implicated in controlling sexual arousal. Specifically, animal studies have revealed three (main) integrative dopaminergic systems primarily responsible for the control and modulation of sexual behaviour. First, the incertohypothalamic dopaminergic system which projects to the medial preoptic area (MPOA) is identified as one of the most important areas for the control of motivational and consummatory aspects of sexual behaviour. Specifically, the generation of genital reflexes required for erection and ejaculation, the focussing of male attention on sexually relevant stimuli, and the increase of species-specific motor patterns during copulation are controlled by the MPOA. Importantly, the PRL receptor is strongly expressed in the MPOA (Pi et al., 1998), with increased PRL decreasing the dopaminergic activity of the MPOA (Lookingland and Moore, 1984). Although no data exists showing the PRL-induced inhibition of MPOA activity reduces sexual drive, PRL has been demonstrated to inhibit maternal behaviours also driven by dopaminergic MPOA activity (Brodges et al., 2001). Thus, peripheral PRL is clearly capable of modifying the dopaminergic activity of the MPOA, and indeed appears to act as a negative feedback mechanism. The mesolimbocortical dopaminergic system (MLC), which originates in the ventral tegmental area and projects to the mesial components of the limbic system (e.g. nucleus accumbens, amygdala, mesial frontal cortex), is the second potential target of PRL feedback. Similar to its role in reward processes, the dopaminergic output of the MLC is primarily responsible for appetitive/motivational regulation of sexual activity. This is evidenced by stimulation of MLC dopamine in response to sexually related sensory stimuli (Bradley and Meisel, 2001; Fiorino et al., 1997). Similar to the MPOA, dopaminergic activity of the nucleus accumbens and limbic forebrain is antagonised by acute peripheral or central PRL 15 administration (Gonzales-Mora et al., 1990; Chen and Ramirez, 1988). In contrast, other reports have demonstrated increased MLC dopaminergic activity following direct PRL infusion in the nucleus accumbens (Hernandez et al., 1994). These contrasting data are likely to be attributable to differentially PRL concentrations, resulting from the route of administration. Nevertheless, these data clearly demonstrate a mechanism whereby peripherally secreted PRL may feedback to modify MLC dopaminergic control of sexual motivation. In addition, the nigrostriatal dopaminergic system (NS), which originates in the substantia nigra and projects primarily to the putamen and caudate nucleus, is a candidate for a feedback mechanism of PRL. The NS is proposed to integrate both sensory and motor aspects of sexual behaviour, with NS dopamine enabling a state of ‘preparedness’. Dopaminergic activity of the NS thus contributes to generation of consummatory motor functions, such as the pursuit of a sexual partner before copulation (Robbins and Everitt, 1992). PRL is clearly capable of modifying dopaminergic activity within the NS. Acute central and peripheral administration of PRL modifies the activity of dopaminergic neurons in the striatum, with both excitatory and inhibitory effects noted (Chen and Ramirez, 1989; Cebeira et al., 1991). Furthermore, dopamine production by superfused slices of striatum in vitro is stimulated by PRL (Chen and Ramirez, 1988). Indeed, stimulation of striatal dopamine output by acute PRL administration has been demonstrated to be associated with facilitation of sexual behaviour in the male rat (Cruz-Casallas et al., 1989). Thus, dopaminergic activity of NS is clearly influenced by peripheral PRL, and associated with altered sexual behaviour. A MODEL OF PRL REGULATION OF SEXUAL FUNCTION FOLLOWING ORGASM 16 We have argued that the weight of data suggests that PRL may be an endocrine regulator of human sexual behaviour, which is integrated with neural control of sexual function. The pathways whereby PRL may fill this role is schematically displayed in Figure 3. PRL may impact upon peripheral reproductive organs to either facilitate physiological mechanisms essential for successful conception and/or inhibit further reproductive activity. Alternatively, PRL may form a feedback mechanism to the CNS, modifying the activity of DC, MLC and NS dopaminergic neurons. As no direct experimental evidence exists demonstrating a feedback role of PRL in modulating sexual arousal and function, extrapolations have clearly been made in proposing the current model. First, although chronic hyperprolactinemia is related to the suppression of both reproductive function and sexual arousal, this is commonly characterised by PRL levels >200 ng/ml, which are experienced over a number of months. In contrast, PRL levels are increased following orgasm to levels between 15 and 25 ng/ml for at least an hour, although the exact duration of this effect in unknown. Thus, the marked inhibitory effect of hyperprolactinemia cannot be directly inferred to occur following acute PRL increases. Nevertheless, data from animal experiments suggest that acute PRL administration, in levels that we observe following orgasm, may produce meaningful modification of sexual behaviour as well as alterations of central and peripheral systems responsible for sexual drive and reproductive function. Nevertheless, the only way to confirm this position experimentally is to manipulate PRL levels acutely in healthy humans, and examine the impact of this on sexual arousal and behaviour. We have recently completed the first experiment of this kind in our laboratory, with data indicating that pharmacological manipulation of comparatively small changes in PRL increases is sufficient to produce significant changes in sexual drive. Clearly 17 however, further investigation is required to elucidate the effect of acute increases in PRL for human sexual arousal. An important assumption in the current model is PRL access to the CNS. Certainly, although the size of PRL does not allow it to cross the blood-brain-barrier (Walsh et al., 1978), it can directly pass into the brain via the highly permeable circumventricular organs such as the area postrema, subfornical organ and medien eminence (Ganong, 2000), all of which express PRL receptors (Mangurain et al., 1999). Additionally, PRL may be indirectly transported into the brain via cerebrospinal fluid Sobrinho, 1993). Thus, acute PRL is able to access brain tissue via mechanisms that enable rapid transport. This suggests that rather than time-consuming transport across the blood brain barrier by active transport, peripheral PRL can rapidly access the CNS, supporting the role of this peptide in feedback to sites regulating sexual drive and function. It is important however that we continue to examine the accessibility of PRL to the CNS; ongoing studies in our laboratory are specifically examining CNS involvement in PRL and functional changes following orgasm. This model also assumes that PRL plays a role in regulating sexual drive and function in both males and females, although the genders display distinct differences in sexuality, such as the characteristics of the refractory period (Masters and Johnson, 1966). Nevertheless, although the PRL response to orgasm is similar in both genders, it must be recognized that the effect of this response is dependent upon a number of other factors, such as receptor expression and sensitivity (Bole-Feysot et al., 1998). Accordingly, the PRL response may differ between the genders according to impact upon peripheral and central regulation of sexual arousal and function, thus potentially differentially regulating physical and psychological components of 18 the refractory period. Clearly, the specific consequences of acute PRL increases for both genders clearly warrant further investigation. A final assumption of this model is that the secretion of PRL is biologically relevant. In contrast to this position, as PRL secretion is dopaminergically controlled, it is possible that it rather represents a downstream effect of the well-known dopaminergic involvement in the regulation of sexual behaviour. Although possible, this position is counteracted by the specificity of the dopaminergic control of sexual behaviour. The discrete secretion of PRL following orgasm suggests a directed response that is not initiated during both sensory and motor phases of sexual arousal. Thus, it is unlikely that the PRL response represents generalised dopaminergic activity during sexual encounters. Rather, the data suggest that the PRL response represents a directed, biologically relevant response that is initiated by inhibition of neuroendocrine dopaminergic neurons. In summary, we have posited that the pituitary hormone PRL may represent an important endocrine regulator of sexual drive and behaviour. As we proceed with examining the role of PRL in post-orgasmic sexual drive, it is becoming clear that, while this hormone plays a role, it is likely one among a constellation of neuroendocrine factors. An understanding of the multiple physiological levels of human sexual behaviour represents a worthy challenge, with the promise of an understanding of normal, pathological and psychopathological sexual behaviour (Haake et al., 2003). 19 References Andersson K-E, Wagner G. Physiology of penile erection. Physiol Rev 1995;75:191-236. Aoki H, Fujioka T, Matsuzaka J, Kubo T, Nakamura K, Yasuda N. Suppression by prolactin of the electrically induced erectile response through its direct effect on the corpus cavernosum penis in the dog. J Urol 1995;154:595-600. Bancroft J. 1999. Central inhibition of sexual response in the male: a theoretical perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1999;23:763-784. Bjorklund A, Moore RY, Nobin A, Stenevi U. The organization of tubero-hypophyseal and reticulo-infundibular catecholamine neuron systems in the rat brain. Brain Res 1973;51:171191. Blaicher W, Gruber D, Bieglmayer C, Blaicher AM, Knogler W, Huber JC. The role of oxytocin in relation to female sexual arousal. Gynecol Obstet Invest 1999;47:125-126. Bole-Feysot C, Goffin V, Edery M, Binart N, Kelly PA. Prolactine (PRL) and its receptor: actions, signal transduction pathways and phenotypes observed in PRL receptor knockout mice Endocrine Rev 1998;19:225-268. Bradley KC, Meisel RL. Sexual behavior induction of c-Fos in the nucleus accumbens and amphetamine-stimulated locomotor activity are sensitized by previous sexual experience in female Syrian hamsters. J Neurosci 2001;21:2123-2130. Bridges R, Rigero B, Byrnes E, Yang L, Walker A. Central infusions of the recombinant human prolactin receptor antagonist, S179D-PRL, delay the onset of maternal behavior in steroid-primed, nulliparous female rats. Endocrinology 2001;142: 730-739. Brown WA, Heninger G. Cortisol, growth hormone, free fatty acids, and experimentally evoked affective arousal. Am J Psychol 1975;132:1172-1176. Buvat J, Lemaire A, Buvat-Herbaut M, Fourlinnie JC, Racadot A, Fossati P. Hyperprolactinemia and sexual function in men. Horm Res 1985;22:196-203. Carani C, Bancroft J, Del Rio G, Granata ARM, Facchinetti F, Marrama P. The endocrine effects of visual erotic stimuli in normal men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1990;15:207-216. Carani C, Granata ARM, Faustini Fustini M, Marrama P. Prolactin and testosterone: their role in male sexual function Int J Androl 1996;19:48-54. Carmichael MS, Warburton VL, Dixen J, Davidson JM. Relationships among cardiovascular, muscular and oxytocin responses during human sexual arousal. Arch Sex Behav 1994;23:5979. Carmichael MS, Humbert R, Dixen J, Palmisano G, Greenleaf W, Davidson JM. Plasma oxytocin increases in the human sexual response. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1987;64:27-31. 20 Carmichael MS, Warburton VL, Dixen J, Davidson JM. Relationships among cardiovascular, muscular, and oxytocin responses during human sexual activity. Arch Sex Behav 1994;23:5979. Cooper AJ. Bromocriptine in impotence. Lancet 1977;2:567. Cruz-Casallas PE, Nasello AG, Hucke EE, Felicio LF. Dual modulation of male sexual behavior in rats by central prolactin: relationship with in vivo striatal dopaminergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999;24:681-693. DeMaria JE, Lerant AA, Freeman ME. Prolactin activates all three populations of hypothalamic neuroendocrine dopaminergic neurons in ovariectomized rats. Brain Res 1999;837:236-241. DeMaria JE, Livingstone JD, Freeman ME. Characterization of the dopaminergic input of the pituitary gland throughout the estrous cycle of the rat. Neuroendocrinology 1998;67:377-383. DeMaria JE, Zelena D, Vecsernyés M, Nagy GM, Freeman ME. The effect of neurointermediate lobe denervation on hypothalamic neuroendocrine dopaminergic neurons, Brain Res 1998;806:89-94. De Rosa M, Colao A, Di Sarno A, Ferone D, Landi ML, Zarilli S, Paesano L, Merola B, Lombardi G. Cabergoline treatment rapidly improves gonadal function in hyperprolactinemic males: a comparison with bromocriptine Eur J Endocrinol 1998;138:286-293. Doherty PC, Wu DE, Matt KS. Hyperprolactinemia preferentially inhibits erectile function in adrenalectomized male rats. Life Sci 1990;47:141-148. Doherty PC, Lane SJ, Pfeil KA, Morgan WW, Bartke A, Smith MS. Extra-hypothalamic dopamine is not involved in the effects of hyperprolactinemia on male copulatory behavior. Physiol Behav 1989;45:1101-1105. Doherty PC, Baum MJ, Todd RB. Effects of chronic hyperprolactinemia on sexual arousal and erectile function in male rats. Neuroendocrinology 1986;42:368-375. Doherty PC, Bartke A, Smith MS, Davis SL. Increased serum prolactin levels mediate the suppressive effects of ectopic pituitary grafts on copulatory behavior in male rats. Horm Behav 1985;19:111-121. Drago F, Lissandrello CO. The ‘low dose’ concept and the paradoxical effects of prolactin on grooming and sexual behavior. Eur J Pharmacol 2000;405:131-137. Exton MS, Bindert A, Krüger T, Scheller F, Hartmann U, Schedlowski M. Cardiovascular and endocrine alterations after masturbation-induced orgasm in women. Psychosom Med 1999;61:280-289. Exton NG, Truong TC, Exton MS, Wingenfeld SA, Leygraf N, Saller B, Hartmann U, Schedlowski M. Neuroendocrine response to film-induced sexual arousal in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000;25:189-199. 21 Exton MS, Krüger THC, Koch M, Paulson E, Knapp W, Hartmann U, Schedlowski M. Coitus-induced orgasm stimulated prolactin secretion in healthy subjects. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2001;26:287-294. Exton MS, Krüger THC, Bursch N, Knapp W, Schedlowski M, Hartmann U. Neuroendocrine response to masturbation-induced orgasm following a 3-week sexual abstinence. World J Urol 2001;19:377-382. Fox CA, Ismail AAA, Love DN, Kirkham KE, Loraine JA. Study on the relationship between plasma testosterone levels and human sexual activity. J Endocrinol 1972;52:51-58. Freeman ME, Kanyicska B, Lerant A, Nagy G. 2000. Prolactin: structure, function, and regulation of secretion. Physiol Rev 2000;80:1523-1631. Fuxe K. Cellular localization of monoamines in the median eminence and in the infundibular stem of some mammals, Acta Physiol Scand 1964;58:383-384. Goudreau JL, Lindley SE, Lookingland KJ, Moore KE. Evidence that hypothalamic periventricular dopamine neurons innervate the intermediate lobe of the rat pituitary. Neuroendocrinology 1992;56:100-105. Haake P, Krüger THC, Exton MS, Giepen C, Hartmann U, Osterheider M, Flesch M, Janssen O, Leygraf N, & Schedlowski M. Acute neuroendocrine response to sexual stimulation in sexual offenders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 2003;48: 265-271. Heiman JR, Rowland DL, Hatch JP, Gladue BA. Psychophysiological and endocrine responses to sexual arousal in women. Arch Sex Behav 1991;20:171-186. Howell Skalla L, Bunick D, Bleck G, Nelson RA, Bahr JM. Cloning and sequence analysis of the extracellular region of the polar bear (Ursus maritimus) luteinizing hormone receptor (LHr), follicle stimulating hormone receptor (FSHr), and prolactin receptor (PRLr) genes and their expression in the testis of the black bear (Ursus americanus). Mol Reprod Dev 2000;55:136-45. Hull EM, Lorraine DS, Du J, Matuszewich L, Lumley LA, Putnam SK, Moses J. Hormoneneurotransmitter interactions in the control of sexual behavior. Behav Brain Res 1999;105:105-116. Kooy A, Weber RF, Ooms MP, Vreeburg JT. Deterioration of male sexual behavior in rats by the new prolactin-secreting tumor 7315b. Horm Behav 1988;22:351-361. Koppelman MCS, Parry BL, Hamilton JA, Alagna SW, Loriaux DL. Effect of Bromocriptine on affect and libido in hyperprolactinemia. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144:1037-1041. Krüger T, Exton MS, Pawlak C, von zur Mühlen A, Hartmann U, Schedlowski M. Neuroendocrine and cardiovascular response to sexual arousal and orgasm in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1998;23:401-411. 22 Krueger THC, Haake P, Chereath D, Knapp W, Janssen OE, Exton MS, Hartmann U, & Schedlowski M. Specificity of the neuroendocrine response to orgasm during sexual arousal in men. Journal of Endocrinology 2003;177:57-64. La Ferla J, Anderson D, Schalch D. Psychoendocrine response to sexual arousal in human males. Psychosom Med 1978;40:166-172. Lee R, Jaffe R, Midgley A. Lack of alteration of serum gonadotropins in men and women following sexual intercourse. Am J Obst Gynecol 1974;120:985-987. Levi L. Sympatho-adrenomedullary activity, diuresis, and emotional reactions during visual sexual stimulation in human females and males. Psychosom Med 1969;31:251-268. Lincoln, G. Luteinising hormone and testosterone in man. Nature 1974;252:232-233. Littler WA, Honour AJ, Sleight P. Direct arterial pressure, heart rate and electrocardiogram during human coitus. J Reprod Fert 1974;40:321-331. Lookingland KJ, Moore KE. Effects of estradiol and prolactin on incertohypothalamic dopaminergic neurons in the male rat. Brain Res 1984;323:83-91. Macleod RM, Lehmeyer JE. Studies on the mechanism of the dopamine-mediated inhibition of prolactin secretion. Endocrinology 1974;94:1077-1085. March CM. Bromocriptine in the treatment of hypogonadism and male impotence. Drugs 1979;17:49-58. Mas M. Neurobiological correlates of masculine sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995;19:261-277. Meston CM, Frohlich PF. The neurobiology of sexual function. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:1012-1030. Montejo-Gonzalez AL, Llorca G, Izquierdo JA, Ledesma A, Bousono M, Calcedo A, Carrasco JL, Ciudad J, Daniel E, De la Gandara J, Derecho J, Franco M, Gomez MJ, Macias JA, Martin T, Perez V, Sanchez JM, Sanchez S, Vicens E. SSRI-induced sexual dysfunction: fluoxetine, paroxetine, sertraline, and fluvoxamine in a prospective, multicenter, and descriptive clinical study of 344 patients. J Sex Marital Ther, 1997;23:176-194. Nasello AG, Vanzeler ML, Madurieira EH, Felicio LF. Effects of acute and long term domperidone treatment on prolactin and gonadal hormone levels and sexual behavior of male and female rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1997;58:1089-1094. Nemec ED, Mansfield L, Kennedy JW. Heart rate and blood pressure during sexual activity in normal males. Am Heart J 1976;92:274-277. Pérez-Villamil B, Bordiu E, Puente-Cueva M. Involvement of physiological prolactin levels in growth and prolactin receptor content of prostate-glands and testes in developing male rats. J Endocrinol 1980;132:449-459. 23 Pi XJ, Grattan DR. Differential expression of the two forms of prolactin receptor mRNA within microdissected hypothalamic nuclei of the rat. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 1998;59:1-12. Pi XJ, Grattan DR. Increased expression of both short and long term forms of prolactin receptor mRNA in hypothalamic nuclei of lactating rats. J Mol Endocrinol 1999;23:13-22. Pi XJ, Grattan DR. Increased prolactin receptor immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus of lactating rats. J Neuroendocrinol 1999;11:693-705. Pirke K, Kockott G, Dittmar F. Psychosexual stimulation and plasma testosterone in man. Arch Sex Behav 1974;3:577-584. Prigent-Tessier A, Pageaux JF, Fayard JM, Lagarde M, Laugier C, Cohen H. Prolactin upregulates prostaglandin E2 production through increased expression of pancreatic-type phospholipase A2 (type 1) and prostaglandin G/H synthase 2 in uterine cells. Mol Cell Endocrinol 1996;122:101-108. Purvis K, Landgren B, Cekan Z, Diczfalusy E. Endocrine effects of masturbation in men. J Endocrinol 1976;70:439-444. Ra S, Aoki H, Fujioka T, Sato F, Kubo T, Yasuda N. 1996. In vitro contraction of the canine corpus cavernosum penis by direct perfusion with prolactin or growth hormone. J Urol 1996;156:522-525. Reddy YD, Reddy KV, Govindappa S. Effect of prolactin and bromocriptine administration on epididymal function: a biochemical study in rats. Ind J Physiol Pharmacol 1985;29:234238. Roky R, Paut-Pagano L, Goffin V, Kitahama K, Valatx JL, Kelly PA, Jouvet M. Distribution of prolactin receptors in the forebrain: immunohistochemical study. Neuroendocrinology 1996;63:422-429. Rosen RC, Lane RM, Menza M. Effects of SSRIs on sexual function: a critical review J Clin Psychopharmacol 1999;19:67-85. Rowland DL, Heiman JR, Gladue BA, Hatch JP, Doering CH, Weiler SJ. Endocrine, psychological, and genital response to sexual arousal in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1987;12:149-158. Sauder SE, Frager M, Case GD, Kelch RP, Marshall JC. Abnormal patterns of pulsatile luteinizing hormone secretion in women with hyperprolactinemia an amenorrhea: responses to bromocriptine. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1984;59: 941-948. Shen WW, Hsu JH. Female sexual side effects associated with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: a descriptive clinical study of 33 patients. Int J Psychiatr Med 1995;25:239-248. Sobrinho LG. The psychogenic effects of prolactin. Acta Endocrinol 1993;129:S38-40. 24 Stricker P, Grueter R. Action du lobe antérieur de l’hypophyse sur la montée laiteuse. C R Soc Biol 1928;99:1978-1980 Stoléru SG, Ennaji A, Counot A, Spira A. LH pulsatile secretion and testosterone blood levels are influenced by sexual arousal in human males. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1993;18:205-218. Verhelst J, Abs R, Maiter D, van den Bruel A, Vandeweghe M, Velkeniers B, Mockel J, Lamberigts G, Petrossians P, Coremans P, Mahler C, Stevenaert A, Verlooy J, Raftpoulos C, Beckers A. Cabergoline in the treatment of hyperporlactinaemia: a study in 455 patients. J Endocrinol Metab 1999;84:2518-2522. Waldinger MD, Olivier B. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor-induced sexual dysfunction: clinical and research considerations. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1998;13:S27-33. Walsh JP, Pullan PT. Hyperprolactinaemia in males: A heterogeneous disorder. Aust NZ J Med 1997;27:385-390. Walsh RJ, Slaby FJ, Posner BI. A receptor-mediated mechanism for the transport of prolactin from blood to cerebrospinal fluid. Endocrinology 1987;120:1864-1850. Whipple B, Ogden G, Komisaruk BR. Physiological correlates of imagery-induced orgasm in women. Arch Sex Behav 1992;21:121-133. Wiedeking C, Lake CR, Ziegler M, Kowarski A, Money J. Plasma noradrenaline and dopamine-beta-hydroxylase during sexual activity. Psychosom Med 1977;39:143-148. Yoshimura Y, Nakamura Y, Oda T, Ando M, Ubukata Y, Kayama N, Karube M, Yamada H. Effects of prolactin on ovarian plasmin generation in the process of ovulation. Biol Reprod 1992;46:322-327. Fiorino DF, Coury A, Phillips AG. Dynamic changes in nucleus accumbens dopamine efflux during the Coolidge effect in male rats. J Neurosci 1997;17:4849-4855. Gonzales-Mora JL, Guadalupe T, Mas M. In vivo voltammetry study of the modulatory action of prolactin on mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Brain Res Bull 1990;25:729-733. Chen JC, Ramirez VD. In vivo dopaminergic activity from the nucleus accumbens, substantia niagra and ventral tegmental area in the freely moving rat : basal neurochemical output and prolactin effect. Neuroendocrinology 1988;48:329-335. Hernandez ML, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ, Navarro M, de Miguel R, Cebeira M, Vatic S, Ramos JA. Modifications of mesolimbic and nigrostriataldopaminergic activity after intracerebroventricular administration of prolactin. J Neural Trans Gen Sec 1994;96:63-79. Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Functions of dopamine in the dorsal and ventral striatum. Sem Neurosci 1992;4:119-128. 25 Chen JC, Ramirez VD. Effects of prolactin on tyrosine hydroxylase activity of central dopaminergic neurons of male rats. Eur J Pharmacol 1989;166:473-479. Cebeira M, Hernandez ML, Rodriguez de Fonsesca F, de Miguel R, Fernandez-Ruiz JJ, Ramos JA. Lack of effect of prolactin on the dopaminergic receptor sensitivity of striatal and limbic areas after experimentally-induced alterations in peripheral levels. Life Sci 1991;48:531-541. Chen JC, Ramirez VD. Comparison of the effect of prolactin on dopamine release from the dorsal and ventral striatum and from the mediobasal hypothalamus superfused in vitro. Eur J Pharmacol 1988;149:1-8. Cruz-Casallas PE, Nasello AG, Hucke EETS, Felicio LF. Dual modulation of male sexual behavior in rats by central prolactin: relationship with in vivo striatal dopaminergic activity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 1999;24:681-693. Walsh RJ, Posner BI, Kopriwa BM, Brawer JR. 1978. Prolactin binding sites in the rat brain. Science 1978;201:1041-1043. Ganong WF. Circumventricular organs: definition and role in the regulation of endocrine and anatomic function. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 2000;27:422-247. Mangurian LP, Jurjus AR, Walsh RJ. Prolactin receptor localization to the area postrema. Brain Res 1999;836:218-220. Masters WH., Johnson, VE. Human sexual response. Little & Brown: Boston. 1966. 26 Figure Legends Figure 1. Experimental design for examination of sexual arousal and orgasm-induced prolactin secretion. Each subject participates in a control and an experimental condition. Both control and experimental conditions involve viewing of an emotionally neutral documentary film. However, subjects view a pornographic film in the middle 20 min of the experimental condition. Additionally, in some experiments, following 10 minutes viewing of the pornographic film participants are required to achieve orgasm via masturbation or coitus. Figure 2. Effect of coitus induced orgasm (a), masturbation induced orgasm (b), and sexual arousal without orgasm (c) on peripheral prolactin concentrations in males and females. Experimental sessions are depicted by filled circles, control condition by hollow circles. (Redrawn from 28-31) Figure 3. Theoretical model of the impact of PRL secretion following orgasm. PRL may influence peripheral reproductive organs, and/or may feedback to dopaminergic systems in the CNS (DC, MLC, NS) recognised to play an important role in regulation of sexual behavior.