Microsoft Word - Spectrum: Concordia University Research Repository

advertisement
1
2
3
The interaction between the spatial distribution of resource patches and
4
population density: consequences for intraspecific growth and
5
morphology
6
7
Running headline: Spatial resource distribution and population density
8
9
B. Jacobson1*, J.W.A. Grant2 and P.R. Peres-Neto1†
1
Département des Sciences Biologiques, Université du Québec à Montréal, C.P. 8888,
Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec H3C3P8 Canada
2
Department of Biology, Concordia University, 1455 de Maisonneuve Blvd. West,
Montréal, Québec, H3G1M8, Canada
10
*
Corresponding author: jacobson.bailey@courrier.uqam.ca
11
12
13
14
†
Canada Research Chair in Spatial Modelling and Biodiversity
15
16
17
1
18
Summary
19
1. How individuals within a population distribute themselves across resource patches
20
of varying quality has been an important focus of ecological theory. The ideal free
21
distribution predicts equal fitness amongst individuals in a 1:1 ratio with
22
resources, whereas resource defence theory predicts different degrees of
23
monopolization (fitness variance) as a function of temporal and spatial resource
24
clumping and population density.
25
2. One overlooked landscape characteristic is the spatial distribution of resource
26
patches, altering the equitability of resource accessibility and thereby the effective
27
number of competitors. While much work has investigated the influence of
28
morphology on competitive ability for different resource types, less is known
29
regarding the phenotypic characteristics conferring relative ability for a single
30
resource type, particularly when exploitative competition predominates.
31
3. Here we used young-of-the-year rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) to test
32
whether and how the spatial distribution of resource patches and population
33
density interact to influence the level and variance of individual growth, as well as
34
if functional morphology relates to competitive ability. Feeding trials were
35
conducted within stream channels under three spatial distributions of nine
36
resource patches (distributed, semi-clumped and clumped) at two density levels (9
37
and 27 individuals).
38
4. Average trial growth was greater in high-density treatments with no effect of
39
resource distribution. Within-trial growth variance had opposite patterns across
40
resource distributions. Here, variance decreased at low-population, but increased
2
41
at high-population densities as patches became increasingly clumped as the result
42
of changes in the levels of interference vs. exploitative competition. Within-trial
43
growth was related to both pre- and post-trial morphology where competitive
44
individuals were those with traits associated with swimming capacity and
45
efficiency: larger heads/bodies/caudal fins and less angled pectoral fins.
46
5. The different degrees of within-population growth variance at the same density
47
level found here, as a function of spatial resource distribution, provide an
48
explanation for the inconsistencies in within-site growth variance and population
49
regulation often noted with regard to density-dependence in natural landscapes.
50
51
Key-words: intraspecific competition, landscape structure, morphometrics, negative
52
density dependence, phenotype-dependent growth, stream fish
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
61
62
63
3
64
65
Introduction
How individuals within a population distribute themselves across resource patches
66
of varying quality has been a central focus of ecological theory. The ideal free
67
distribution (IFD) proposes that competitively equivalent individuals with perfect
68
knowledge of and the cost-free ability to move throughout the landscape distribute
69
themselves in a 1:1 ratio with resources such that fitness across patches is equal (Fretwell
70
& Lucas 1970; Lomnicki 1988; Kennedy & Gray 1993). In reality, individual fitness
71
within populations often varies as a function of unequal competitive abilities (Abrahams
72
1986; Kennedy & Gray 1993); however, the presence of unequal competitors within a
73
landscape does not necessarily result in variation in resource gain (Grand & Grant 1994).
74
Tregenza, Hack & Thompson (1996), for instance, found that good competitors only had
75
a fitness advantage at low densities because at high densities individuals switched from
76
interference to exploitative (alternatively, contest to scramble) competition.
77
The influence of environmental properties (sensu Grant 1993) on the ability for an
78
individual to monopolize resources has been well investigated within the realm of
79
behavioural ecology. At the scale of an individual resource patch, resource defence
80
theory predicts a dome-shaped relationship between the economic defensibility of a
81
resource patch and either the degree of temporal and spatial resource clumping or the
82
number of potential competitors within the population (Grant 1993; Robb & Grant 1997;
83
Grant, Gaboury & Levitt 2000; Ward, Webster & Hart 2006). Taken together, within-
84
population fitness (growth) variation will not only depend on the amount of aggression
85
(i.e. interference competition) and resource monopolization (Grant & Guha 1993; Weir &
4
86
Grant 2004; Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005), but also on the equality of resource
87
accessibility (Boujard, Labbé & Aupérin 2002).
88
One overlooked landscape characteristic (but see Silver et al. 2000) that has the
89
potential to influence resource accessibility, and thus, relative fitness amongst individuals
90
is the spatial distribution of resource patches. When patches are spread across the
91
landscape, individuals may be able to settle into patches with few interactions between
92
competitors, thus leading to more even resource access, partitioning and fitness (Noël,
93
Grant & Carrigan 2005). Conversely, as patches become increasingly clumped in space,
94
the effective number of competitors within the population will increase due to a decrease
95
in foraging area (Fausch 1984; Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005), in turn decreasing the
96
equality of resource accessibility and potentially increasing fitness variation. In stream
97
fishes, where much of the work on density-dependent growth and regulation (e.g.
98
mortality, emigration) has been conducted, within-site growth variance has largely been
99
attributed to variation in foraging site (patch) quality (Site Quality Hypothesis; SQH)
100
(Newman 1993; Ward et al. 2007; Lobón-Cerviá 2010). Thus, the potential influence of
101
the spatial distribution of resources on growth variation may provide an alternative
102
explanation for the differential patterns of individual growth (Imre, Grant & Cunjak
103
2010) and distribution found across natural populations.
104
Just which individuals in the population are able to monopolize resources and
105
have higher relative fitness is a function of their relative competitive ability. In stream
106
fishes, competitive ability is often linked to body size, with larger individuals able to gain
107
access to the best foraging patches (Fausch 1984; Ward, Webster & Hart 2006), though
108
the causal order of this relationship has been questioned (Ward, Webster & Hart 2006).
5
109
While the resource polymorphism literature has investigated the influence of morphology
110
on competitive ability for different resources (Smith & Skúlason 1996; Bolnick 2004;
111
Araújo et al. 2008; Edelaar, Siepielski & Clobert 2008), less is known about the
112
phenotypic characteristics dictating relative competitive ability for a single resource type
113
when size variation is inconsequential. This is a particularly relevant question within a
114
single young age class where size may initially be somewhat invariant, but growth ability
115
has large impacts for subsequent survival (Ward, Nislow & Folt 2009; Lobón-Cerviá
116
2010). In such cases, competitive ability may be based on an individual’s ability to use its
117
environment more efficiently. Within riffle habitat sections of streams, for example,
118
where greater water velocity is related to increased resource delivery (Gotceitas & Godin
119
1992), an individual’s ability to gain resources may be linked to its swimming capacity
120
and ability to hold station against the current (Ward, Webster & Hart 2006; Leavy &
121
Bonner 2009).
122
In this study, we set out to test whether and how the spatial distribution of
123
resource patches with equal quality (contrary to the SQH) and population density interact
124
to influence the mean and variance of within-population growth. In addition, we tested
125
whether an individual’s morphology relates to its ability to obtain resources (compete).
126
To do so, we conducted feeding trials within flow-through stream channels using young-
127
of-the-year rainbow trout (O. mykiss) under three different spatial distributions of nine
128
resource patches: evenly distributed, semi-clumped and clumped (Fig. 1). Such trials
129
were conducted at two density levels: low, one individual per patch and high, three
130
individuals per patch. We expected (i) mean growth to be greater within high-density
131
trials due to a decrease in energy expended on aggression (i.e. less interference
6
132
competition), as per resource defence theory (Kim & Grant 2007); (ii) within-trial growth
133
variance to increase with increased resource patch clumping due to changes in effective
134
competitor number (Fausch 1984; Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005) and resource
135
accessibility; and (iii) individual growth to be most similar in low-density distributed
136
resource trials due to a 1:1 competitor to resource patch ratio, as per the IFD (Noël, Grant
137
& Carrigan 2005). Lastly, we expected (iv) competitive ability, irrespective of density
138
and resource distribution, to be based on functional morphology and its relation to
139
swimming ability (Ward, Webster & Hart 2006).
140
141
Materials and Methods
142
Species and experimental set-up
143
We acquired 500 young-of-the-year rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) (Fig.
144
1a) 5cm in length from Pisciculture des Arpents Verts, Québec, Canada for use in our
145
experiments. When not in use fish were housed within two circular 133L constant-flow
146
tanks (flow at 50% depth=0.25m/s; water temperature=18-19.5°C) on a 12h light:12h
147
dark cycle (Brown & Brown 1993) (lights on at 7am) and fed a maintenance ration of dry
148
food pellets (Skretting extruded salmonid feed). Housing conditions were monitored, and
149
daily feedings provided by animal care staff at Concordia University, Québec, Canada.
150
Experimental trials were conducted within four flow-through experimental stream
151
channels (Fig. 1b) under the same light regime. Each channel was lined to a depth of
152
2.5cm with small natural-coloured aquarium gravel wherein nine terracotta “saucers”
153
were embedded, flush with the bed of gravel, to act as resource patches. Flow in each
154
channel was held constant across all trials (average flow at 50% depth=0.15-0.18m/s
7
155
across channels). Water temperature within the system (c. 18°C; Li & Brocksen 1977)
156
was controlled by the amount of de-chlorinated city water entering each channel and was
157
continuously recorded every 15 min by a HOBO temperature/light data logger (Onset
158
Computer Corporation, Bourne, MA, USA) placed in each channel. Loggers were
159
checked in the morning before feeding and again at the end of the light cycle (average
160
across channels ± SD: week 1 19.1± 0.6°C; week 2 18.7 ± 0.2°C; week 3 17.7 ± 0.8°C;
161
week 4 18.2 ± 0.2°C; and week 5 17.7 ± 0.5°C) to ensure that temperatures remained
162
within the preferred range for rainbow trout (Wood, Grant & Belanger 2012). To record
163
feeding dynamics and inter-individual interactions throughout the light cycle, digital
164
colour CCD bullet cameras were connected to a surveillance system (GeoVision, Inc.,
165
Irvine, CA, USA) and mounted directly above each channel; with the exception of
166
recording periods, channels were covered with large-weave netting to prevent fish from
167
jumping.
168
169
170
Experimental procedure
To determine the influence of spatial resource distribution and competitor density
171
on growth, feeding trials were conducted under a 3x2 factorial design testing three
172
different resource distributions [distributed (D), semi-clumped (S) and clumped (C), Fig.
173
1b] at each of two densities (low n=9 and high n=27) with three replicates per treatment.
174
Trials were conducted four at a time from 14 October to 22 November 2011, where each
175
week the individuals and treatments tested were both randomly selected and assigned to
176
each stream channel.
8
177
Each trial lasted a total of 8 days. On day 1, individuals were selected from
178
housing tanks, anesthetized using a 1 clove oil:10 ethanol mixture (active agent eugenol)
179
(Anderson, McKinley & Colavecchia 1997; Keene et al. 1998) and each given a unique
180
identifier by way of two subcutaneous VIE tags (visible implant elastomer; Northwest
181
Marine Technologies, Shaw Island, WA, USA) in one or two of four fluorescent colours
182
in one or two of several body locations. Individuals were then photographed on one
183
lateral side for pre-trial morphology, weighed for initial mass and measured for initial
184
body and caudal widths (Fig. 1a). After recovering from anesthesia, individuals were
185
introduced into a stream channel and remained unfed for the day (Olsson, Svanbäck &
186
Eklöv 2007). On days 2-7, resources were added once daily to stream channels, evenly
187
divided across the nine patches, given at a level of 10% (Brown & Brown 1993) of the
188
total initial mass of individuals within the trial per day in order to encourage growth.
189
Feeding was recorded on days 2 and 7 (i.e. initial and final feedings) from the time food
190
entered the system until the end of the light cycle. Note that while no mortality occurred
191
as the result of handling or experimental treatment, there were some incidences of
192
individuals jumping out of the channels during initial and final recording and thus final
193
densities did not always match initially set levels (average density per treatment, low
194
density: D=8.67, S=8, C=7.67, high density: D=25.67, S=24.67, C=26; see also outliers
195
below). When this occurred during day 1 or 2, individuals were identified and the food
196
level adjusted to remain at 10% of the remaining trial initial mass. Lastly, on day 8
197
individuals were not fed (Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005) to ensure the same level of
198
gastric evacuation (Fausch 1984; Currens et al. 1989) and near the end of the light cycle
199
were anaesthetized, identified, weighed for final mass and photographed on the same
9
200
lateral side for post-trial morphology. Individuals were afterwards returned to a separate
201
holding tank to ensure that no individual was used more than once.
202
203
204
Data analyses
Three growth metrics were calculated for each individual and used as response
205
variables within analyses: (i) individual growth Gind was calculated as the difference
206
between log10 transformed final and initial mass (Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005); (ii)
207
frowth variance Gvar was calculated as the absolute value of the difference between Gind
208
and the average Gind of the trial in which the individual participated. This metric, when
209
used in an analysis of variance (ANOVA), results in a test for variance differences rather
210
than means across treatments (Zar 2010); and lastly, (iii) relative growth Grel was
211
calculated as the difference between Gind and the average Gind of the trial in which the
212
individual participated and classified individuals based on their growth relative to their
213
trial average (i.e. either less or greater growth than the average).
214
To determine the influence of treatment type on trial growth, Gind (differences in
215
average growth across treatments) and Gvar (differences in growth variance across
216
treatments) were used as response variables in separate mixed-model ANOVAs with the
217
fixed effects of density, spatial resource distribution and the density x spatial distribution
218
interaction and the random effects of channel, week in which the trial was conducted and
219
initial mass or initial mass variance (calculated as per Gvar). Given the differences in
220
variances across treatments (a measure of interest of this study, i.e. Gvar), weights
221
proportional to group variances were used (Pinheiro & Bates 2000).
10
222
In order to determine the influence of treatment type on the spatial dynamics of
223
resource patch use as well as the amount of inter-individual aggression (i.e. interference
224
competition) all recordings of the final feeding (day 7) were analyzed, as it was assumed
225
that recorded behaviour had at this point become established (Noël, Grant & Carrigan
226
2005). Spatial feeding dynamics were characterized and recorded as the number of
227
seconds within a minute that at least one individual was at least one-half of a body length
228
within each patch, recorded every 5 min for the first 15 min of feeding during which time
229
feeding activity was at its highest. As all replicates for any given treatment type had
230
similar dynamics, information garnered from the final replicate only is presented.
231
Aggressive interactions were characterized as chases, defined as a unidirectional
232
swimming burst of one fish towards another lasting for at least one body length (Noël,
233
Grant & Carrigan 2005; Kim & Grant 2007; Wood, Grant & Belanger 2012), and scored
234
for the same 3 min as spatial feeding dynamics for each video. Due to issues of low video
235
quality and the tightly packed fast movements that occurred during feeding at high
236
densities, each video was scored twice and the total number of observed chases averaged
237
for each replicate. This average was then divided by the number of fish within the trial to
238
control for differences in density (Robb & Grant 1998; Kim & Grant 2007) and the
239
number of chases per fish used as the response variable within a mixed-effect ANOVA
240
with density, spatial distribution and the density x spatial distribution interaction entered
241
as fixed effects and the channel and week in which the trial was conducted entered as
242
random effects.
243
244
To determine if there was an influence of an individual’s morphology on their
ability to gain resources (grow) within trials, geometric morphometrics were used.
11
245
Twenty-two landmarks were placed on both pre- and post-trial photographs (Fig. 1a)
246
using the program tpsDIG2 (Rohlf 2005a) and converted, separately, into partial warps
247
(shape variables) using the programs Coordgen6 and PCAgen6 (Sheets 2004a, 2004b).
248
Pre- and post-trial warps were then entered as separate fixed effects within two mixed-
249
model ANOVAs using Gind and Grel as the response variables with channel and week
250
entered as random effects. Note that three individuals, randomly distributed across
251
treatments, were identified as outliers with respect to post-trial morphology and were
252
removed from all analyses. To visualize which morphological features differed between
253
individuals with greater or less relative growth, deformation grids were produced for pre-
254
and post-trial morphologies of Grel classifications using the program tpsRegr (Rohlf
255
2005b). Lastly, as competitive ability within fish is often linked with length/size (Ward,
256
Webster & Hart 2006) and this effect is removed through standardization in geometric
257
morphometrics (Zelditch et al. 2004), regressions were performed for each treatment to
258
test for the presence of a relationship between Gind (growth) and each of log10 initial mass
259
(Massi), initial standard length (Lengthi), initial body width (BodyWi) and initial caudal
260
width (CaudalWi) (size). The significance of these correlations was determined with
261
mixed-effect regression using Gind as the response and each variable, separately per
262
treatment, as the fixed effect with random factors as above. All mixed-model ANOVAs
263
were run using the nlme package (Pinheiro et al. 2012) in R version 2.13.0 (R
264
Development Core Team, 2011 Vienna, Austria) and regressions were performed using
265
Statistica 6.0 (StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA).
266
267
Results
12
268
The degree of individual growth (Gind) within trials was found to differ as a
269
function of density, with average growth greater within high-density compared to low-
270
density treatments irrespective of the spatial distribution of resource patches (Table 1 and
271
Fig. 2a). Interestingly, the degree to which growth varied amongst individuals within
272
trials (Gvar) differed significantly across all factors, spatial distribution, density and their
273
interaction (Table 1); the variance decreased in low-density trials but increased in high-
274
density trials as resources became more spatially clumped (Fig. 2b). Such greater growth
275
variance in high-density spatially clumped treatments occurred due to the weight loss
276
(negative growth) of 14 individuals throughout the trials; only two individuals lost weight
277
in the distributed and one each in the semi-clumped and clumped treatments at low
278
densities.
279
Observation of videos recorded during the final feeding revealed different spatial
280
dynamics (Fig. 3a) and amounts of aggressive interactions between individuals as a
281
function of density and resource distribution (Table 1 and Fig. 3b). Overall, more
282
aggressive interactions (interference competition) occurred between individuals within
283
low-density compared to high-density trials, with the number of chases per fish
284
decreasing as spatial resource clumping increased. As reflected by the spatial dynamics of
285
patch use (location and shading within Fig. 3a), within low-density distributed trials,
286
individuals tended to chase and exclude others from the resource arena wherein foraging
287
attempts were made mainly at previously sampled patches. Conversely, within clumped
288
trials, patches were relatively more evenly utilized. Likewise, the spatial dynamics of
289
patch use for high-density trials also reflected more even resource partitioning with
290
individuals divided amongst and continually circulating over all patches. However,
13
291
within clumped trials, several individuals never appeared to attempt foraging. Across all
292
treatments, when not foraging, individuals remained within the downstream portion of the
293
stream channel.
294
The degree to which individuals grew within a trial, irrespective of treatment, was
295
found to be a function of their morphology; both pre- and post-trial morphology
296
correlated with individual growth (Gind) and differentiated between individuals of
297
opposite relative growth (Grel) (Table 1). Such effects could also be seen in deformation
298
grids of pre- and post-trial morphology as a function of Grel (Fig. 4), where the primary
299
differences between groups in pre-trial morphology were differences in pectoral fin
300
angle, head/body and caudal fin size and head shape and body height with regards to
301
post-trial morphology. Growth was, with few exceptions, unrelated to initial fish
302
length/size, with regressions overall largely non-significant (Table 2).
303
304
Discussion
305
The main goal of our study was to test whether the spatial distribution of
306
resources and population density interact in determining the growth and growth variance
307
(i.e. fitness) amongst individuals within a landscape. We found that this interaction can
308
generate different levels of fitness variation within a population through differences in the
309
amount of interference competition, thus causing individuals with specific morphologies
310
(phenotype) to be conferred more or less relative fitness dependent on treatment. Such
311
results have important consequences for the demographics of populations and degree of
312
regulation expected in natural systems of varying structure, potentially explaining several
14
313
inconsistencies found in the literature regarding density-dependent growth patterns (see
314
Implications).
315
316
317
Inter-treatment growth patterns: variance and competition
Results from our study clearly show that the spatial distribution of resource
318
patches themselves, irrespective of quality, can influence the degree and distribution of
319
relative fitness (i.e. growth) amongst individuals within a population through changing
320
the effective number of competitors for patches within the system. We expected the
321
distribution to follow an IFD, with growth variance lowest in low-density distributed
322
resource trials, but increasing with increased resource clumping (Noël, Grant & Carrigan
323
2005). Instead, opposite patterns of growth variance were found across resource
324
distributions between density treatments; variance was greatest when resources were
325
clumped at high densities but when resources were distributed at low densities. The high
326
variance in growth found in low-density distributed trials, as evidenced from feeding
327
recordings, was a function of increased interference competition; individuals were
328
observed attempting to defend patches and others attempting to forage at only those
329
already sampled. As patches became increasingly clumped at low densities, the effective
330
number of competitors increased. Consequently, interactions switched from interference
331
to exploitative competition, which accounted for the decrease in growth variance found
332
here, likely because defense was too costly with increasing competitor pressure (Grant
333
1993; Grant & Guha 1993; Tregenza, Hack & Thompson 1996; Syarifuddin & Kramer
334
1996; Kaspersson, Höjesjö & Pedersen 2010).
15
335
Lower levels of interference competition were found within high-density trials,
336
(Brown, Brown & Srivastava 1992; Jobling & Baardvik 1994), as individuals cycled in
337
and out of resource patches (Robb & Grant 1997) and were able to spread themselves
338
more evenly amongst patches. As aggression has important associated fitness costs, such
339
as increasing energy expenditure, social stress and resource loss while defending (Li &
340
Brocksen 1977; Kim & Grant 2007; Kaspersson, Höjesjö & Pedersen 2010), the reduced
341
interference competition costs are likely responsible for the greater average growth,
342
irrespective of resource distribution, found in high-density trials. That said, while the
343
exploitative competition utilized at high densities is proposed to allow for a more even
344
distribution of resources amongst individuals (Grant 1993; Jobling & Baardvik 1994;
345
Boujard, Labbé & Aupérin 2002; Weir & Grant 2004), our results suggest that even
346
partitioning does not occur past a certain threshold. Indeed, the decrease in the equality of
347
resource accessibility at high densities, also found in other studies (Syarifuddin & Kramer
348
1996; Boujard, Labbé & Aupérin 2002), caused individuals to cease feeding, leading to
349
negative growth and increased growth variance, with increased spatial resource clumping
350
(Li & Brocksen 1977; Grant & Kramer 1990).
351
352
353
The influence and shaping of phenotype
While competitive ability is often suggested to be related to body size (Post,
354
Parkinson & Johnston 1999; Ward, Webster & Hart 2006; Kaspersson, Höjesjö &
355
Pedersen 2010), we found no significant relationship between an individual’s level of
356
growth and their initial mass, length, body or caudal width. Instead, body shape was the
357
important component in obtaining resources, as levels of individual and relative growth
16
358
correlated with pre-trial morphology. Here individuals with greater than average trial
359
growth had a morphology indicating greater swimming ability and turning stability:
360
relatively deeper heads/bodies and larger caudal fins (Gatz 1979; Webb 1984; Ojanguren
361
& Braña 2003; Peres-Neto & Magnan 2004). These individuals also had pectoral fins that
362
were more flatly angled, potentially indicating more efficient maneuvering (Drucker &
363
Lauder 2003) and reduced energy expenditure through holding position against current on
364
the substrate (Fausch 1984). As swimming ability has been proposed to relate to
365
competitive ability in both exploitative and interference contexts (Ward, Webster & Hart
366
2006), such phenotypic characteristics likely accounted for the greater growth of these
367
individuals. Post-trial morphology was even more highly correlated with trial growth,
368
potentially reflecting both the relative effects of the amount of growth on body shape in
369
and of itself (Currens et al. 1989; Borcherding & Magnhagen 2008), as well as potential
370
investment in plasticity (i.e. morphological modulation) by individuals who had greater
371
levels of growth (Olsson, Svanbäck & Eklöv 2007), acting to further increase their
372
competitive ability. Indeed, individuals with greater relative growth were those with
373
characteristics of benthic habitat use (more dorsally placed eyes and greater body height)
374
(Gatz 1979; Mason et al. 2007) and exploitative ability (increased swimming ability and
375
turning stability) (Webb 1984; Ojanguren & Braña 2003; Peres-Neto & Magnan 2004). In
376
contrast, individuals with less relative growth were characterized by more slender bodies
377
and longer heads, potentially indicative of under-nourishment (Currens et al. 1989;
378
Olsson, Svanbäck & Eklöv 2007; Borcherding & Magnhagen 2008).
379
380
Implications for population regulation in natural landscapes
17
381
Within natural landscapes, as density increases, mean growth is often found to
382
follow a negative power curve (Jenkins et al. 1999; Grant & Imre 2005), with growth
383
variance increasing (as per the SQH) (Newman 1993; Keeley 2001; Lobón-Cerviá 2010).
384
After density-dependent emigration and mortality, however, the resulting patterns are
385
much more variable; exhibiting negative power curves, negative linear relationships of
386
varying strength, to no relationship between average growth and density (Newman 1993;
387
Jenkins et al. 1999; Post, Parkinson & Johnston 1999; Keeley 2001; Imre, Grant &
388
Cunjak 2005), with little consistency in associated variance-density relationships (Imre,
389
Grant & Cunjak 2010). As, here, we found different degrees of growth variance at the
390
same population density as a function of the spatial distribution of resource patches, our
391
results have the potential to shed light on such inconsistencies in natural systems. For
392
example, landscapes with high densities and clumped resources, rather than distributed,
393
may be characterized by high growth variance and greater degrees of density-dependent
394
emigration and mortality. Hence the degree of spatial resource clumping may modify the
395
strength of the negative power curve of growth versus density.
396
While we found positive instead of negative density-dependent growth, such a
397
finding is common within experimental systems where food is given proportional to
398
density (Brown, Brown & Srivastava 1992). Within natural systems, site-resource levels
399
are assumed to be constant across densities (Kaspersson, Höjesjö & Pedersen 2010)
400
where low-density sites have a greater amount of resource relative to competitor number
401
than high density (Noël, Grant & Carrigan 2005; Ward et al. 2007). Should we have
402
provided resource levels more in line with natural systems, it is conceivable that we
18
403
would have also found negative density dependence with variance remaining higher at
404
high densities.
405
Our findings show the potential for the spatial structure of resource patches,
406
irrespective of other potentially varying characteristics (i.e. resource quality, patch size),
407
to influence population growth through changing the effective number of competitors
408
within the system, and therefore, the degree to which competitive ability (phenotype)
409
confers enhanced fitness. Future work should seek to understand how the spatial
410
distribution of patches varying in quality may interact to influence within-population
411
growth variance and the consequences of such variance for the regulation of natural
412
populations, potentially allowing for further insight as to the decisions, pressures and
413
structuring factors, which underlie patterns of individual distribution within landscapes.
414
415
Acknowledgements
416
We would like to thank the animal care staff at Concordia University who cared for
417
housed fish on a day-to-day basis and István Imre as well as an anonymous referee for
418
comments on the manuscript. This research was supported by an NSERC (Natural
419
Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada) grant to PPN.
420
421
Data Accessibility
422
Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.7nb71
423
(Jacobson, Grant & Peres-Neto 2015).
424
425
References
19
426
Abrahams, M.V. (1986) Patch choice under perceptual constraints: a cause for departures
427
from an ideal free distribution. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 19, 409-
428
415.
429
Anderson, W.G., McKinley, R.S. & Colavecchia, M. (1997) The use of clove oil as an
430
anesthetic for rainbow trout and its effects on swimming performance. North
431
American Journal of Fisheries Management, 17, 301-307.
432
Araújo, M.S., Guimarães, P.R.Jr., Svanbäck, R., Pinheiro, A., Guimarães, P., Dos Reis,
433
S.F. & Bolnick, D.I. (2008) Network analysis reveals contrasting effects of
434
intraspecific competition on individual vs. population diets. Ecology, 89, 1981-
435
1993.
436
Bolnick, D.I. (2004) Can intraspecific competition drive disruptive selection? An
437
experimental test in natural populations of sticklebacks. Evolution, 58, 608-618.
438
Borcherding, J. & Magnhagen, C. (2008) Food abundance affects both morphology and
439
440
behaviour of juvenile perch. Ecology of Freshwater Fish, 17, 207-218.
Boujard, T., Labbé, L. & Aupérin, B. (2002) Feeding behaviour, energy expenditure and
441
growth of rainbow trout in relation to sotcking density and food accessibility.
442
Aquaculture Research, 33, 1233-1242.
443
444
445
Brown, G.E. & Brown, J.A. (1993) Do kin always make better neighbours?: the effects of
territory quality. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 225-231.
Brown, G.E., Brown, J.A. & Srivastava, R.K. (1992) The effect of stocking density on
446
the behaviour of Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus L.). Journal of Fish Biology, 41,
447
955-963.
448
Currens, K.P., Sharpe, C.S., Hjort, R., Schreck, C.B. & Li, H.W. (1989) Effects of
20
449
different feeding regimes on the morphometrics of chinook salmon
450
(Oncorhynchus tshawytscha) and rainbow trout (O. mykiss). Copeia, 3, 689-695.
451
Drucker, E.G. & Lauder, G.V. (2003) Function of pectoral fins in rainbow trout:
452
behavioral repertoire and hydrodynamic forces. The Journal of Experimental
453
Biology, 206, 813-826.
454
Edelaar, P., Siepielski, A.M. & Clobert, J. (2008) Matching habitat choice causes directed
455
gene flow: a neglected dimension in evolution and ecology. Evolution, 62, 2462-
456
2472.
457
458
459
460
461
462
Fausch, K.D. (1984) Profitable stream positions for salmonids: relating specific growth
rate to net energy gain. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 62, 441-451.
Fretwell, S.D. & Lucas, H.L.Jr. (1970) On territorial behavior and other factors
influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheoretica, 19, 16-36.
Gatz, A.J.Jr. (1979) Ecological morphology of freshwater stream fishes. Tulane Studies
in Zoology and Botany, 21, 91-124.
463
Gotceitas, V. & Godin, J-G.J. (1992) Effects of location on food delivery and social
464
status on foraging-site selection by juvenile Atlantic salmon. Environmental
465
Biology of Fishes, 35, 291-300.
466
Grand, T.C. & Grant, J.W.A. (1994) Spatial predictability of resources and the ideal free
467
distribution in convict cichlids, Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum. Animal Behaviour,
468
48, 909-919.
469
470
471
Grant, J.W.A. (1993) Whether or not to defend? The influence of resource distribution.
Marine Behaviour and Physiology, 23, 137-153.
Grant, J.W.A. & Guha, R.T. (1993) Spatial clumping of food increases its
21
472
monopolization and defense by convict cichlids, Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum.
473
Behavioral Ecology, 4, 293-296.
474
475
476
Grant, J.W.A. & Imre, I. (2005) Patterns of density-dependent growth in juvenile streamdwelling salmonids. Journal of Fish Biology, 67, 100-110.
Grant, J.W.A. & Kramer, D.L. (1990) Territory size as a predictor of the upper limit to
477
population density of juvenile salmonids in streams. Canadian Journal of
478
Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 47, 1724-1737.
479
Grant, J.W.A., Gaboury, C.L. & Levitt, H.L. (2000) Competitor-to-resource ratio, a
480
general formulation of operational sex ratio, as a predictor of competitive
481
aggression in Japanese medaka (Pisces: Oryziidae). Behavioral Ecology, 11, 670-
482
675.
483
Imre, I., Grant, J.W.A. & Cunjak, R.A. (2005) Density-dependent growth of young-of-
484
the-year Atlantic salmon Salmo salar in Catamaran Brook, New Brunswick.
485
Journal of Animal Ecology, 74, 508-516.
486
Imre, I., Grant, J.W.A. & Cunjak, R.A. (2010) Density-dependent growth of young-of-
487
the-year Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) revisited. Ecology of Freshwater Fish, 19,
488
1-6.
489
Jacobson, B., Grant, J.W.A. & Peres-Neto, P.R. (2015) Data from: The interaction
490
between the spatial distribution of resource patches and population density:
491
consequences for intra-specific growth and morphology. Dryad Digital
492
Repository http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.7nb71.
493
Jenkins, T.M.Jr., Diehl, S., Kratz, K.W. & Cooper, S.D. (1999) Effects of population
494
density on individual growth of brown trout in streams. Ecology, 80, 941-956.
22
495
Jobling, M. & Baardvik, B.M. (1994) The influence of environmental manipulations on
496
inter- and intra-individual variation in food acquisition and growth performance
497
of Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus. Jounal of Fish Biology, 44, 1069-1087.
498
Kaspersson, R., Höjesjö, J. & Pedersen, S. (2010) Effects of density on foraging success
499
and aggression in age-structured groups of brown trout. Animal Behaviour, 79,
500
709-715.
501
502
503
Keeley, E.R. (2001) Demographic responses to food and space competition by juvenile
steelhead trout. Ecology, 82, 1247-1259.
Keene, J.L., Noakes, D.L.G., Moccia, R.D. & Soto, C.G. (1998) The efficacy of clove oil
504
as an anaesthetic for rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss (Walbaum).
505
Aquaculture Research, 29, 89-101.
506
Kennedy, M. & Gray, R.D. (1993) Can ecological theory predict the distribution of
507
foraging animals? A critical analysis of experiments on the Ideal Free
508
Distribution. Oikos, 68, 158-166.
509
Kim, J-W. & Grant, J.W.A. (2007) Effects of patch shape and group size on the
510
effectiveness of defence by juvenile convict cichlids. Animal Behaviour, 73, 275-
511
280.
512
Leavy, T.R. & Bonner, T.H. (2009) Relationships among swimming ability, current
513
velocity association, and morphology for freshwater lotic fishes. North American
514
Journal of Fisheries Management, 29, 72-83.
515
Li, H.W. & Brocksen, R.W. (1977) Approaches to the analysis of energetic costs of
516
intraspecific competition for space by rainbow trout (Salmo gairdneri). Journal of
517
Fisheries Biology, 11, 329-341.
23
518
519
520
521
522
Lobón-Cerviá, J. (2010) Density dependence contrains mean growth rate while enhancing
individual size variation in stream salmonids. Oecologia, 164, 109-115.
Lomnicki, A. (1988) Population ecology of individuals. Princeton University Press, New
Jersey.
Mason, N.W.H., Lanoiselée, C., Mouillot, D., Irz, P. & Argillier, C. (2007) Functional
523
characters combined with null models reveal inconsistency in mechanisms of
524
species trunover in lacustrine fish communities. Oecologia, 153, 441-452.
525
526
527
Newman, R.M. (1993) A conceptural model for examining density dependence in the
growth of stream trout. Ecology of Freshwater Fish, 2, 121-131.
Noël, M.V., Grant, J.W.A. & Carrigan, J.G. (2005) Effects of competitor-to-resource
528
ratio on aggression and size variation within groups of convict cichlids. Animal
529
Behaviour, 69, 1157-1163.
530
Ojanguren, A.F. & Braña, F. (2003) Effects of size and morphology on swimming
531
performance in juvenile brown trout (Salmo trutta L.). Ecology of Freshwater
532
Fish, 12, 241-246.
533
534
535
Olsson, J., Svanbäck, R. & Eklöv, P. (2007) Effects of resource level and habitat type on
behavioral and morpholoigcal plasticity in Eurasian perch. Oecologia, 152, 48-56.
Peres-Neto, P.R. & Magnan, P. (2004) The influence of swimming demand on
536
phenotypic plasticity and morphological integration: a comparison of two
537
polymorphic charr species. Oecologia, 140, 36-45.
538
539
540
Pinheiro, J.C. & Bates, D.M. (2000) Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. Springer,
Verlag New York, Inc.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & the R Development Core Team (2012)
24
541
nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-105.
542
Post, J.R., Parkinson, E.A. & Johnston, N.T. (1999) Density-dependent processes in
543
structured fish populations: interaction strengths in whole-lake experiments.
544
Ecological Monographs, 69, 155-175.
545
Robb, S.E. & Grant, J.W.A. (1998) Interactions between the spatial and temporal
546
clumping of food affect the intensity of aggression in Japanese medaka. Animal
547
Behaviour, 56, 29-34.
548
Rohlf, F.J. (2005a) tpsDIG2. Program for digitizing landmarks and outlines on image
549
files, scanner or video. [windows], version 2. Department of Ecology and
550
Evolution, State University of New York, Stony Brook (available from
551
http://life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph/).
552
Rohlf, F.J. (2005b) tpsRegr. Program for analyzing landmark data by thin-plate splines
553
using multivariate multiple regression of shape. [windows], version 1.31.
554
Department of Ecology and Evolution, State University of New York, Stony
555
Brook (available from http://life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph/).
556
Sheets, H.D. (2004a) CoordGen6. IMP-Integrated Morphometrics Package
557
IMPBasics.exe, version 6a. Coordinate Generating Utility, also file translator
558
to/from X1Y1, TPS file formats. Department of Physics, Casinus College, Buffalo,
559
New York (available from: http://www3.canisius.edu/~sheets/morphsoft.html).
560
Sheets, H.D. (2004b) PCAGen6. IMP-Integrated Morphometrics Package
561
IMPBasics.exe, version 6a. Principal components analysis based on partial warps
562
scores and principal axis scores. Department of Physics, Casinus College,
25
563
Buffalo, New York (available from:
564
http://www3.canisius.edu/~sheets/morphsoft.html).
565
Silver, P., Cooper, J.K., Palmer, M.A. & Davis, E.J. (2000) The arrangement of resources
566
in patchy landscapes: effects on distribution, survival, and resource acquisition of
567
chironomids. Oecologia, 124, 216-224.
568
Smith, T.B. & Skúlason, S. (1996) Evolutionary significance of resource polymorphisms
569
in fishes, amphibians, and birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 27,
570
111-133.
571
Syarifuddin, S. & Kramer, D.L. (1996) The effect of group size on space use and
572
aggression at a concentrated food source in blue gouramis, Trichogaster
573
trichopterus (Pisces: Belontiidae). Environmental Biology of Fishes, 46, 289-296.
574
Tregenza, T., Hack, M.A. & Thompson, D.J. (1996) Relative competitive success of
575
unequal competitors changes with overall density. Oikos, 77, 158-162.
576
Ward, D.M., Nislow, K.H. & Folt, C.L. (2009) Increased population density and
577
suppressed prey biomass: relative impacts on juvenile Atlantic salmon growth.
578
Transactions of the American Fisheries Society, 138, 135-143.
579
580
Ward, A.J.W., Webster, M.M & Hart, P.J.B. (2006) Intraspecific food competition in
fishes. Fish and Fisheries, 7, 231-261.
581
Ward, D.M., Nislow, K.H., Armstrong, J.D., Einum, S. & Folt, C.L. (2007) Is the shape
582
of the density-growth relationship for stream salmonids evidence for exploitative
583
rather than interference competition? Journal of Animal Ecology, 76, 135-138.
584
Webb, P.W. (1984) Body and fin form and strike tactics of four teleost predators
26
585
attacking fathead minnow (Pimephales promelas) prey. Canadian Journal of
586
Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 41, 157-165.
587
588
589
Weir, L.K. & Grant, J.W.A. (2004) The causes of resource monopolization: interaction
between resource dispersion and mode of competition. Ethology, 110, 63-74.
Wood, J.L.A., Grant, J.W.A. & Belanger, M.H. (2012) Population density and territory
590
size in juvenile rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss: implications for population
591
regulation. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences, 69, 1121-1128.
592
593
594
Zar, J.H. (2010) Biostatistical analysis, 5th edn. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, New
Jersey, USA.
Zelditch, M.L., Swiderski, D.L., Sheets, H.D. & Fink, W.L. (2004) Geometric
595
morphometrics for biologists: a primer. Elsevier Academic Press, New York,
596
USA.
597
598
599
600
601
602
603
604
605
606
607
27
608
Figure legends
609
610
Figure 1. Shape analysis and spatial resource distributions. (a) Young-of-the-year
611
rainbow trout with 22 landmarks used within morphological analyses, arrows denote
612
locations of body and caudal width caliper measurements. (b) Photo and schematics of
613
spatial resource distributions tested: distributed (D), semi-clumped (S) and clumped (C).
614
Flow denotes the direction of water movement through channels, water depth was c.
615
18cm, resource patch depth 1.1cm. All measurements within figure are in centimetre.
616
617
Figure 2. Results from mixed-model ANOVAs of growth outcomes of trials. (a) Growth
618
(Gind) outcome for trials as a function of spatial and density treatments. (b) Variance in
619
growth (Gvar) outcome for trials as a function of spatial and density treatments. Open
620
circles denote high density, filled low-density trials, error bars are 95% confidence
621
intervals.
622
623
Figure 3. Spatial feeding dynamics and aggressive interactions observed within video
624
recordings. (a) Feeding dynamics of individuals throughout the first 15 min of feeding.
625
Shading is based on the number of seconds within 1 min that at least ½ body length of at
626
least one individual was within the resource patch. (b) Results from the mixed-model
627
ANOVA of the number of chases per fish within the first 15 min of feeding as a function
628
of spatial and density treatments. Open circles denote high density, filled low-density
629
trials, error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
28
630
Figure 4. Morphological differences between individuals with negative or positive (less
631
or greater) growth relative to their trial average (Grel) as a function of pre- or post-trial
632
morphology. In order to highlight differences variation is depicted at 10x the observed
633
range.
634
635
Figures
636
637
Figure 1.
(a)
(b)
28.6
18.5
18.5
57.25
28.6
7.6
37
18.5
S
28.6
74
flow
114.5
18.5
638
D
C
28.6
639
640
641
642
29
643
Figure 2.
644
(a)
0.14
645
646
0.12
Growth
647
648
0.10
649
650
0.08
651
0.06
Distributed
Distributed
Semi-clumped
Semi-clumped
Clumped
Clumped
Spatial distribution
(b)
652
653
0.10
654
Growth variance
0.08
655
656
0.06
657
0.04
658
659
0.02
660
0.00
Distributed
Distributed
Semi-clumped
Semi-clumped
Clumped
Clumped
661
Spatial distribution
662
663
664
665
30
666
Figure 3.
(a)
Low density
High density
0
1-15
16-30
31-45
46-60
5min
10min
15min
5min
10min
15min
Chases per fish
(b)
Distributed
Semi-clumped
Clumped
667
668
669
670
31
671
Figure 4.
Negative
relativeGrowth
growth
Negative Relative
Positive
Positive relative
Relative growth
Growth
Pre-trial
Pre-trial
Post-trial
Post-trial
672
673
674
675
676
677
678
679
680
681
682
683
684
685
686
687
32
688
Tables
689
690
Table 1. F-values from mixed-model ANOVAs regarding the influence of treatment type
691
and morphology on within-trial growth, as well as treatment type on the number of chases
692
per fish observed within feeding recordings. Numbers in brackets are degrees of freedom
Table 1
693
Factor
Treatment
Morphology
Space (2)
Density (1)
SxD (2)
Pre-trial (40)
Post-trial (40)
* p≤0.05
** p≤0.001
Gind
Response
Gvar
Chases
0.248
6.230*
0.952
9.524*
6.414*
9.959*
695
6.068*
44.841**
696
4.025
Gind
Grel
697
2.744**
5.584**
1.628*
3.230**
698
694
699
700
701
702
703
704
705
706
707
708
709
33
710
Table 2. Correlation coefficients (r) from regressions of the relationship between
711
individual growth (Gind) and initial size variables for each spatial resource distribution:
712
distributed (D), semi-clumped (S) and clumped (C). Numbers in brackets are the
713
combined number of individuals in the replicates of that treatment type
Table 2
Massi
Lengthi
BodyWi
CaudalWi
D (26)
Low density
S (24)
C (22)
D (77)
0.368
0.470*
0.323
0.309
0.105
0.169
0.188
0.098
0.746*
0.733
0.688
0.659
0.486*
0.565*
0.405
0.229
High density
S (73)
C (77)
0.299
0.391**
0.350*
0.262
-0.006*
-0.022*
-0.003
0.072
* p≤0.05
** p≤0.001
714
Treatment
Morphology
Pos
34
Download