The Science of Biology Read the following passage. Highlight the key terms and underline the main ideas. It is one thing to describe and measure the growth of a plant toward light (discovery science). But what causes this phenomenon? How can scientists explain the plant's ability to detect and respond to the direction of light? Such questions about causes and explanations are at the center of hypothesis-based science. Methods of Hypothesis-Based Science You may have heard of "the scientific method." The steps of this idealized method are diagrammed in Figure 2-10. At one time or another in their research, scientists may use all or most of these steps. Often, however, the sequence of steps does not exactly match the generalized scheme of Figure 2-10. For example, a scientist may start to plan an experiment, but then decide that more observations are needed first. In other cases, observations are too puzzling to even suggest key questions until other lines of research bring more understanding. Or, at some point in a research project, scientists may realize they have been "barking up the wrong tree." That is, they've been asking the wrong questions. Many scientific inquiries are abandoned before any questions are answered, though the data from observations and experiments may prove useful later. And accidental discoveries, such as Fleming's findings about mold and bacteria, are certainly not products of a rigid, step-by-step scientific method. Science is actually less structured than most people realize. Figure 2-10 Although science rarely matches this step-by-step process exactly, scientific inquiry often includes the posing and testing of hypotheses. The Science of Biology Despite all the variation in scientific inquiry, one key element is common to all hypothesis-based science. In fact, this form of inquiry is named for this key element: the hypothesis. Forming and Testing a Hypothesis A hypothesis (plural, hypotheses) is a suggested answer to a well-defined scientific question—an explanation on trial. Most hypotheses are concerned with the causes of natural phenomena, such as the growth of a plant toward light. A hypothesis is often based on past experience or knowledge gained from discovery science or other sources. People use hypotheses almost instinctively, as a natural way to solve everyday problems. For example, suppose your flashlight stops working during a camping trip. That's an observation. The question is obvious: Why doesn't the flashlight work? A reasonable hypothesis based on past experience is that the batteries in the flashlight are dead. Scientists don't just propose hypotheses. They test these ideas by making additional observations or by designing experiments. A hypothesis allows you to make certain predictions. It is these predictions that scientists then test. Consider the case of the failed flashlight in Figure 2-11. Note that the prediction is written as an "If . . . , then . . ." statement. If a particular hypothesis is correct, and you test that hypothesis with a suitable experiment, then you should expect a certain result for the experiment. The Science of Biology Figure 2-11 Applying hypothesis-based science to the problem of a failed flashlight leads to a test in the form of an experiment. If the results of the test do not support the hypothesis, other hypotheses can be suggested and tested. What if the flashlight still doesn't work after changing the batteries? Here's where things get interesting. Keep in mind that in science, an incorrect hypothesis doesn't mean failure. It just means the hypothesis that led to the prediction was probably wrong. The inquiry continues with tests of alternative hypotheses. What would be your next step in the flashlight problem? Perhaps, you could test another hypothesis by replacing the flashlight's bulb. Even when experiments support a hypothesis, curiosity can send you in new directions. (How long do batteries last in a flashlight? Do some brands of batteries last longer than others?) Opportunities for creativity and new challenges make scientific inquiry as exciting as exploring a new landscape. As you reach The Science of Biology the top of a hill or round the corner of a trail, you may become curious to see the view from the next hill or around the next corner. Science generates new questions in a similar manner. A Case Study of Hypothesis-Based Science One way to learn more about how hypothesis-based science works is to examine a case study. In contrast to a made-up example, such as the flashlight problem, a case study is an in-depth examination of something that actually happened. For instance, students in law school train to become lawyers by analyzing the actual documents of past legal cases. Similarly, a case study of a research project that was published in a scientific journal will boost your understanding of how scientists go about their work. The rest of this section is a case study of an inquiry about what harmless snakes might gain by imitating poisonous ones. From Observations to Question and Hypothesis The story begins with some key observations. (Do you recognize this step as discovery science?) Many poisonous animals are brightly colored, with distinctive patterns in some species. This appearance is called warning coloration because it marks the animal as dangerous to potential predators. But there are also mimics. These imposters look like a poisonous species but are really harmless to predators. For example, a nonstinging insect called the flower fly is very similar in appearance to a stinging honeybee. The question that follows from these observations is: What is the function of such mimicry? In 2001, a team of biologists designed a simple but clever set of experiments to test a hypothesis that was first suggested over a century earlier. Here's the hypothesis: Mimics (such as the flower fly) benefit because predators confuse them with the actual harmful species. Researchers David and Karin Pfennig, along with one of their college students, tested this hypothesis by studying mimicry in snakes that live in North and South Carolina. A poisonous snake called the eastern coral snake is marked by rings of red, yellow, and black. Predators rarely attack these snakes. A nonpoisonous snake named the scarlet kingsnake mimics the ringed coloration of the coral snake. Testing a Prediction of the Hypothesis What is the explanation for this case of look-alike snakes? According to the mimicry hypothesis, the coralsnakelike appearance of kingsnakes repels predators. The hypothesis predicts that predators will attack snakes with the bright rings of red, yellow, and black less frequently than they will attack snakes lacking such warning coloration. To test this prediction, the researchers made hundreds of artificial kingsnakes out of wire and a claylike substance called plasticine. There were two types of artificial snakes: those with the red, yellow, and black ring pattern of coral snakes; and snakes with plain brown coloration. The researchers placed equal numbers of the two types of artificial snakes in various sites throughout North and South Carolina. After four weeks, the team retrieved the artificial snakes and counted how many had been attacked by looking for bite or claw marks. The most common predators were foxes, coyotes, and raccoons, but black bears also attacked some of the artificial snakes. The Science of Biology Designing a Controlled Experiment Why did the experiment include artificial snakes that were plain brown along with the ringed snakes? A quick answer is that the contrast in coloration was necessary to see if predators attack snakes based on their color. If all the snakes were the same, the number of attacks would indicate nothing at all about the effect of the colored rings. This point illustrates an important requirement for designing experiments that test hypotheses. If you want to test the effect of one condition, you need to provide a contrasting condition as well. A condition that can differ within the experiment is called a variable. In the artificial snakes, the variable is the presence versus the absence of the colored rings. Most often, experiments test the effect of a difference in just one variable. An experiment that tests the effect of a single variable is called a controlled experiment. By conducting a controlled experiment, scientists try to eliminate (control) other variables that could affect the outcome. This is not usually a simple task. For example, variables such as temperature or other weather conditions could influence the activities of predators in the snake experiment. In an ideal setting— such as a laboratory—scientists can keep temperature and other environmental conditions as constant as possible. But such control is usually impossible in a field experiment. And even in a laboratory, total regulation of all but one variable is often not practical. Eliminating Unwanted Variables What is the solution to the problem of unwanted variables? Researchers divide the subjects (the artificial snakes, for example) into two groups: a control group and an experimental group. Since the snake experiment was designed to test the effect of the colored rings, the artificial snakes with the colored rings were the experimental group. The brown snakes served as a control group by showing what happens in the absence of colored rings. Everything else about the two snake groups was the same. For example, both ringed snakes and brown snakes were made of the same materials. Both kinds of snakes were placed at random in the same locations. Conditions such as light, temperature, and appetite of the predators varied, but both kinds of snakes were subject to the same variations. In this way, the brown snakes controlled, or cancelled out, the effects of the unwanted variables, leaving colored rings as the only consistent difference between the two groups of snakes. Then any difference in the number of attacks on the ringed snakes compared to the brown snakes could only have been due to the difference in coloration. "If . . . , then . . ." Reasoning You should now be able to recognize in this case study the same type of reasoning you learned about in the flashlight example. Using the flowchart in Figure 2-15, you can follow the steps taken by the Pfennig team. Observations of snake coloration and attacks by predators led to a question. The Pfennigs posed a hypothesis that seemed reasonable based on other scientists' past research on different animal species. The Pfennigs then designed and performed a controlled experiment to test their hypothesis. The next step was analyzing the data. The Science of Biology Figure 2-15 This flowchart summarizes the inquiry process followed during the snake mimicry research. Organizing Data and Interpreting Results Scientific inquiry is far from over once the data have been collected. Often, the results of an experiment only begin to make sense after much analysis of the data. For quantitative data, note again that it is often helpful to put the data in the form of a table or graph. These efforts may reveal patterns that were not obvious when the "raw" data were first collected. The bar graph in Figure 2-16 summarizes the results of the artificial snake experiment. The graph also reinforces the purpose of using two groups of snakes. Of all attacks on artificial snakes, about 84 percent of attacks were on the plain brown snakes compared to about 16 percent for the snakes with colored rings. These data fit the prediction based on the mimicry hypothesis. The experiment supports the hypothesis that the kingsnakes' mimicry of coral-snake coloration helps protect against predators. The Science of Biology Figure 2-16 Results of mimicry experiments using artificial snakes show a dramatic difference in the frequency of attacks on plain brown snakes compared to the snakes with colored rings. The research on look-alike snakes provides an example of how scientists use hypothesis-based science to test their explanation of natural phenonena. Notice again how hypothesis-based science works along with discovery science. Questions about nature usually arise from the observations of discovery science. Hypothesis-based science is a process for testing the possible answers to such questions. The upcoming section broadens the view of science you've seen so far, showing how scientific ideas fit in with other ways of picturing and knowing the world around you. Concept Check 2.2 1. Write a hypothesis to explain why a door hinge squeaks. Then use your hypothesis to make a prediction that could be tested. 2. Why do experiments usually test only one variable at a time? The Science of Biology 3. In the snake experiment, what was the basis for the hypothesis that mimic species benefit from looking like unrelated harmful species? 4. What role did the brown artificial snakes have in the experiment?