There`s one key difference between kids who excel at math and

advertisement
POWER OF MYTH
There’s one key difference
between kids who excel at
math and those who don’t
By Miles Kimball and Noah Smith October 27, 2013
Miles Kimball is an
economics professor at the
University of Michigan. He
blogs about economics,
politics and religion.
Noah Smith is an
assistant professor of
finance at Stony Brook
University. His blog is
Noahpinion.
“I’m just not a math person.”
+
We hear it all the time. And we’ve had enough. Because we believe that the idea of “math people” is the
most self-destructive idea in America today. The truth is, you probably are a math person, and by
thinking otherwise, you are possibly hamstringing your own career. Worse, you may be helping to
perpetuate a pernicious myth that is harming underprivileged children—the myth of inborn genetic math
ability.
+
Is math ability genetic? Sure, to some degree. Terence Tao, UCLA’s famous virtuoso mathematician,
publishes dozens of papers in top journals every year, and is sought out by researchers around the world
to help with the hardest parts of their theories. Essentially none of us could ever be as good at math as
Terence Tao, no matter how hard we tried or how well we were taught. But here’s the thing: We don’t
have to! For high school math, inborn talent is just much less important than hard work, preparation, and
self-confidence.
1
How do we know this? First of all, both of us have taught math for many years—as professors, teaching
assistants, and private tutors. Again and again, we have seen the following pattern repeat itself:
1
1. Different kids with different levels of preparation come into a math class. Some of these kids have
parents who have drilled them on math from a young age, while others never had that kind of
parental input.
2. On the first few tests, the well-prepared kids get perfect scores, while the unprepared kids get only
what they could figure out by winging it—maybe 80 or 85%, a solid B.
3. The unprepared kids, not realizing that the top scorers were well-prepared, assume that genetic ability
was what determined the performance differences. Deciding that they “just aren’t math people,”
they don’t try hard in future classes, and fall further behind.
4. The well-prepared kids, not realizing that the B students were simply unprepared, assume that they are
“math people,” and work hard in the future, cementing their advantage.
Thus, people’s belief that math ability can’t change becomes a self-fulfilling prophecy.
+
The idea that math ability is mostly genetic is one dark facet of a larger fallacy that intelligence is mostly
genetic. Academic psychology journals are well stocked with papers studying the world view that lies
behind the kind of self-fulfilling prophecy we just described. For example, Purdue University
psychologist Patricia Linehan writes:
+
A body of research on conceptions of ability has shown two orientations toward ability. Students with an
Incremental orientation believe ability (intelligence) to be malleable, a quality that increases with effort.
Students with an Entity orientation believe ability to be nonmalleable, a fixed quality of self that does not
increase with effort.
+
The “entity orientation” that says “You are smart or not, end of story,” leads to bad outcomes—a result
that has been confirmed by many other studies. (The relevance for math is shown by researchers at
Oklahoma City who recently found that belief in inborn math ability may be responsible for much of the
gender gap in mathematics.)
+
Psychologists Lisa Blackwell, Kali Trzesniewski, and Carol Dweck presented these alternatives to
determine people’s beliefs about intelligence:
+
1. You have a certain amount of intelligence, and you really can’t do much to change it.
2. You can always greatly change how intelligent you are.
They found that students who agreed that “You can always greatly change how intelligent you are” got
higher grades. But as Richard Nisbett recounts in his book Intelligence and How to Get It, they did
something even more remarkable:
+
Dweck and her colleagues then tried to convince a group of poor minority junior high school students that
intelligence is highly malleable and can be developed by hard work…that learning changes the brain by
forming new…connections and that students are in charge of this change process.
+
The results? Convincing students that they could make themselves smarter by hard work led them to work
harder and get higher grades. The intervention had the biggest effect for students who started out
believing intelligence was genetic. (A control group, who were taught how memory works, showed no
such gains.)
+
But improving grades was not the most dramatic effect, “Dweck reported that some of her tough junior
high school boys were reduced to tears by the news that their intelligence was substantially under their
control.” It is no picnic going through life believing you were born dumb—and are doomed to stay that
way.
1
For almost everyone, believing that you were born dumb—and are doomed to stay that way—is believing
a lie. IQ itself can improve with hard work. Because the truth may be hard to believe, here is a set of links
about some excellent books to convince you that most people can become smart in many ways, if they
work hard enough:
+
• The Art of Learning by Josh Waitzkin
• Moonwalking with Einstein by Joshua Foer
• The Talent Code by Daniel Coyle
• Talent is Overrated by Geoff Colvin
So why do we focus on math? For one thing, math skills are increasingly important for getting good jobs
these days—so believing you can’t learn math is especially self-destructive. But we also believe that math
is the area where America’s “fallacy of inborn ability” is the most entrenched. Math is the great mental
bogeyman of an unconfident America. If we can convince you that anyone can learn math, it should be a
short step to convincing you that you can learn just about anything, if you work hard enough.
+
Is America more susceptible than other nations to the dangerous idea of genetic math ability? Here our
evidence is only anecdotal, but we suspect that this is the case. While American fourth and eighth graders
score quite well in international math comparisons—beating countries like Germany, the UK and
Sweden—our high-schoolers underperform those countries by a wide margin. This suggests that
Americans’ native ability is just as good as anyone’s, but that we fail to capitalize on that ability through
hard work. In response to the lackluster high school math performance, some influential voices in
American education policy have suggested simply teaching less math—for example, Andrew Hacker has
called for algebra to no longer be a requirement. The subtext, of course, is that large numbers of
American kids are simply not born with the ability to solve for x.
+
We believe that this approach is disastrous and wrong. First of all, it leaves many Americans ill-prepared
to compete in a global marketplace with hard-working foreigners. But even more importantly, it may
contribute to inequality. A great deal of research has shown that technical skills in areas like software are
increasingly making the difference between America’s upper middle class and its working class. While
we don’t think education is a cure-all for inequality, we definitely believe that in an increasingly
automated workplace, Americans who give up on math are selling themselves short.
+
Too many Americans go through life terrified of equations and mathematical symbols. We think what
many of them are afraid of is “proving” themselves to be genetically inferior by failing to instantly
comprehend the equations (when, of course, in reality, even a math professor would have to read closely).
So they recoil from anything that looks like math, protesting: “I’m not a math person.” And so they
exclude themselves from quite a few lucrative career opportunities. We believe that this has to stop. Our
view is shared by economist and writer Allison Schrager, who has written two wonderful columns in
Quartz (here and here), that echo many of our views.
+
One way to help Americans excel at math is to copy the approach of the Japanese, Chinese, and
Koreans. In Intelligence and How to Get It, Nisbett describes how the educational systems of East Asian
countries focus more on hard work than on inborn talent:
+
1. “Children in Japan go to school about 240 days a year, whereas children in the United States go to
school about 180 days a year.”
2. “Japanese high school students of the 1980s studied 3 ½ hours a day,
and that number is likely to be, if anything, higher today.”
3. “[The inhabitants of Japan and Korea] do
not need to read this book to find out that intelligence and intellectual accomplishment are highly
malleable. Confucius set that matter straight twenty-five hundred years ago.”
4. “When they do badly at
something, [Japanese, Koreans, etc.] respond by working harder at it.”
5. “Persistence in the face of
failure is very much part of the Asian tradition of self-improvement. And [people in those countries] are
accustomed to criticism in the service of self-improvement in situations where Westerners avoid it or
resent it.”
1
We certainly don’t want America’s education system to copy everything Japan does (and we remain
agnostic regarding the wisdom of Confucius). But it seems to us that an emphasis on hard work is a
hallmark not just of modern East Asia, but of America’s past as well. In returning to an emphasis on
effort, America would be returning to its roots, not just copying from successful foreigners.
+
Besides cribbing a few tricks from the Japanese, we also have at least one American-style idea for making
kids smarter: treat people who work hard at learning as heroes and role models. We already venerate
sports heroes who make up for lack of talent through persistence and grit; why should our educational
culture be any different?
+
Math education, we believe, is just the most glaring area of a slow and worrying shift. We see our country
moving away from a culture of hard work toward a culture of belief in genetic determinism. In the debate
between “nature vs. nurture,” a critical third element—personal perseverance and effort—seems to have
been sidelined. We want to bring it back, and we think that math is the best place to start.
Download