Seddon, Botero et al. 2 Electronic Supplementary Material Sexual selection accelerates signal evolution during 4 speciation in birds 6 8 10 Contents Supplementary Methods (Appendix S1) 12 Supplementary Figure Evolutionary relationships of study species (Fig S1) 14 Supplementary Tables 16 Phenotypic traits (Table S1) Factor loadings for plumage reflectance data (Table S2) 18 Statistical tables (Tables S3 to S8) Supplementary References 20 External Database as an Excel file (Appendix S2) Seddon, Botero et al. 22 24 26 Appendix S1: Supplementary Methods Study species Species pairs (sister species and clade sisters) with published data on spectral 28 reflectance [1] were identified from published phylogenetic trees of families or genera generated using protein coding mtDNA in which > 70% of taxa had been 30 sampled and where node support was high (either posterior probability > 95%, or maximum likelihood bootstrap > 70). More recent phylogenetic studies took 32 precedence unless earlier studies included more taxa with a different resolution of sister relationships. When several molecular phylogenies were presented within a 34 paper, we only selected sister pairs resolved in all trees. In situations where nodal support conflicted between different methods of phylogenetic reconstruction, 36 maximum likelihood bootstrap values took precedence. Consensus trees and trees based on concatenated molecular datasets were presumed to depict the most 38 reliable phylogenetic relationships and thus, whenever possible, we assessed nodal support based on the values given in these trees. 40 When selecting species from clades, we paired the focal species with whichever member of its sister clade had plumage reflectance data. However, where 42 more than one clade member had reflectance data, we used range maps to select the species with the closest possible breeding range to the focal species. By 44 choosing the geographically closest clade member, we selected lineages most likely to have split recently, assuming historical species ranges can be inferred from 46 present day distributions and provide an indication of the mode of speciation. To minimize the influence of species interactions on phenotypic divergence (e.g. 48 character displacement), we excluded all cases where one or more unsampled clade members were sympatric with either the focal species or the sampled clade 50 member. In other words, a criterion of selection was that none of the breeding ranges of the other clade members overlapped geographically with the focal species 52 or the clade member included in the main analysis. These criteria automatically restricted the sample of sister clades to small, relatively young clades (≤ 5 species). Seddon, Botero et al. 54 We then categorized species pairs as sympatric or allopatric based on published datasets [2-4]. Remaining species were assigned to these categories 56 using high quality geographic range polygons, following the methods of Weir & Price [3]. 58 Sample size 60 Our final sample of species comparisons (Appendix S2) contained 84 species pairs, including 39 true sister species pairs and 45 clade sisters (a focal species paired 62 with one member of their sister clade). However, because of differences in data requirements and availability, sample size varied across our models (Table S1). 64 Data for both plumage and morphology were available for 69 pairs (hence sample size of Analysis 1 [A1]). Meanwhile, data on plumage were available for all 84 66 species pairs, but the models of diversification rate (Analysis 3 [A3]) could only be run using true sister pairs (n = 39). This was not a constraint for A1 and A2. In 68 addition, we removed 17 clade sisters from A2 as they were phylogenetically nested (they shared one species with another species pair in our dataset) and were 70 therefore unsuitable for evolutionary rates models. We retained these 17 species pairs in linear mixed effect models (LMMs) as all combinations of species were 72 unique, and thus we considered each divergence event to be independent. We included species name as a random effect in the mixed models to control for the 74 inclusion of these repeated measures (see below). 76 Quantifying phenotype Morphological traits. We measured beak, tarsus, and wing length from museum 78 specimens using digital callipers. Beaks were measured (to the nearest 0.01 mm) as length from the anterior edge of the nostrils to the tip; tarsus length was measured 80 down the back of the leg from the middle of the ankle joint (i.e. the notch between the tibia and tarsus) to the end of the last scale of the acrotarsium (usually the last 82 undivided scale); wing was measured as the distance from the carpal joint to the longest primary of the unflattened wing. To ensure consistency, all measures for Seddon, Botero et al. 84 members of a pair were taken by one researcher. Body mass data were compiled from Dunning [5]. 86 Plumage traits. All spectrophotometer measurements were collected using an 88 Ocean Optics (Dunedin, Florida) USB2000 spectrophotometer and a PX-2 pulsed Xenon light source with the spectrophotometer probe at 90° to the plumage. 90 Measurements were standardized to a WS-1 white standard, considered >98% reflective from 250−1500 nm wavelengths. 92 For each reflectance reading, we averaged the reflectance data into bins covering 20 nm of the spectrum. We quantified colour using standard descriptors of 94 reflectance spectra: brightness and hue/chroma [7]. We calculated brightness or intensity by summing its reflectance from 320 to 700 nm, the approximate visible 96 spectrum of most avian species [8]. Because a spectrum consists of reflectance at each wavelength that is highly correlated, we then used a PCA to collapse these 98 reflectance variables into a few independent variables that summarize spectrum shape [6, 7], a standard method to handle spectral data [9-13]. We first used 100 brightness to standardize all reflectance scans before PCA. The resulting principal components (PC) values were thus indices of chroma and hue [6], independent of its 102 brightness. We then performed a principal component analysis (PCA) using the standardized reflectance values from each specimen (19 values for each specimen 104 based on 20 nm bins). Although multiple methods have been previously used to analyze spectral data, including those that take into account the spectral sensitivity 106 of each cone type, the reflectance of the sample, the background against which the sample is viewed, and the irradiance spectrum of the ambient light [7, 14-17], when 108 different methods have been compared, they have yielded qualitatively similar estimates of colour [17] and dichromatism [1]. We chose PCA analysis for its 110 simplicity and because it yields separate values that represent the shape of the spectrum and chroma (e.g. purity of colour). In our analyses, the first two principal 112 components explained more than 75.69% of the variation in the data. We found that principal component 1 (PC1) was positively correlated with reflectance in the 400– 114 480nm range and accounted for 50.54% of the variation in the data; PC2 was Seddon, Botero et al. positively correlated with reflectance in 320–380nm range, and accounted for 116 25.14% of the variation in the data. Therefore, we interpreted PC1 to represent chroma in short wavelength and PC2 to represent chroma in UV. For PC1 and PC2, 118 we calculated the average for males and for females of each species for each body region. For factor loadings, see Table S3. To calculate dichromatism scores, for 120 each body region, we calculated the Euclidean distance between PC scores for males and females (y-axis) separately for PC1 and PC2. We then summed the 122 differences between males and females for each PC across all six body regions to produce the overall dichromatism score. 124 Dichromatism as an index of sexual selection 126 Sexual dichromatism is not a perfect index of sexual selection, not least because a variety of other mechanisms can result in sex-differences in plumage colouration 128 and conspicuousness, such as natural selection for female crypsis in species with female-only incubation [reviewed in 18]. However, in the absence of detailed long- 130 term behavioural studies in which direct measures of sexual selection are obtained (e.g. relative rate of reproduction), dichromatism is the best proxy currently available 132 for the purposes of comparative analyses. It can be easily estimated in all bird species (unlike other indices such as relative testes size or rates of extra-pair 134 paternity which rely either on invasive sampling or intensive behavioural research). Moreover, a number of studies have revealed strong positive associations between 136 dichromatism and other indices of sexual selection such as testes size, degree of polygyny, and frequency of extra-pair paternity [19-21]. Consequently, dichromatism 138 has been used as a proxy for sexual selection in a large number of studies, including those examining the effects of sexual selection on speciation in birds [22-28], lizards 140 [29], insects [30], and fish [31], as well as in comparative studies of the effects of sexual selection on extinction [32-34], mortality [35], immune defense [36], signal 142 144 evolution [37], molecular evolution [38] and even response to climate change [39]. Seddon, Botero et al. 146 Classifying monomorphic taxa as mutually ornamented For taxa where quantitative plumage data indicated a lack of plumage dichromatism, 148 we visually assessed whether this was due to mutual ornamentation (both males and females are ornamented) or not (both males and females lack plumage 150 ornaments). Using photographs or illustrations from field guides or taxonomic monographs, we identified a taxon as ‘mutually ornamented’ in cases where both 152 males and females had colourful (not brown) or iridescent plumage patches, stripes or spots in any region of the body (e.g. bridled titmouse Baeolophus wollweberi). We 154 classified a taxon as ‘monomorphic-dull’ when both males and females had drab, uniform or pale colouration lacking ornamentation in the form of patches of colour, 156 stripes or spots (e.g. oak titmouse Baeolophus inornatus). 158 Analytical approach 160 Analysis 1: Effect of sexual selection on extent of phenotypic divergence We first used linear mixed effect models (LMMs) with maximum likelihood estimation 162 to investigate whether extent of phenotypic divergence varies according to levels of sexual selection (prediction 1) and whether the effects of sexual selection on 164 phenotypic divergence differ between the sexes (prediction 2). We modelled the maximum and total extent of phenotypic divergence (dependent variable) in relation 166 to several predictors including the index of sexual selection (mean value of sexual dichromatism within a pair), sex, and the interaction between sex and dichromatism. 168 The variable 'sex' was included as a factor in the model to indicate the sex associated with the trait diverging most in a particular pair of species (e.g. tarsus 170 length or back colour) came from males or females. In our dataset, some species (n = 17) were represented in more than one 172 pairing (Appendix S2). The non-independence of these data points arising from using the same species was taken into account by fitting both focal species (labeled 174 as Species 1 in analysis tables) and the species they were compared to (labeled Species 2 in analysis tables) as random effects in our LMMs. In all analyses, only 176 unique combinations of species were included. To control for phylogenetic inertia in Seddon, Botero et al. the extent of phenotypic divergence between pairs of species, we included 178 taxonomy as a nested random effect [44]. Mixed-effect models including taxonomy (Family [Genus]) had a significantly lower log-likelihood score than the model 180 excluding taxonomy (Table 1). The significance of fixed effects was examined using Wald type F-tests [45]. Significance values derive from having all significant terms (P 182 < 0.05) fitted in the final model together; statistics associated with non-significant terms were derived from having all significant terms in the model and each non- 184 significant term (P > 0.05) fitted individually. The significance of random effects was tested using log-likelihood ratio tests with all fixed effects and their interactions 186 included in models [46]. Although our LMM approach controlled for phylogenetic inertia at higher 188 taxonomic levels (family and genus) it nonetheless assumed that species pairs are equally related to one another and that the phylogenetic signal of phenotypic 190 divergence is weak. To test this assumption we estimated lambda (λ), which measures the degree to which traits co-vary across a tree in line with Brownian 192 motion (Freckleton et al. 2002). A λ of 1 corresponds to the Brownian model, λ of 0 indicates a lack of phylogenetic structure, and λ values between 0 and 1 indicate the 194 degree of trait lability [47]. To determine whether λ values departed significantly from a Brownian model, we compared the fit of the two models using a likelihood ratio 196 test. We found than both total phenotypic divergence (λ = 0.92) and maximum phenotypic divergence (λ = 0.90) departed from a strict Brownian model. The 198 implication is that the extent to which phenotypes diverge among our species pairs may have been influenced by shared ancestry. To correct for this, we used the 200 phylogenetic generalized least squares (PGLS) comparative method described in Freckleton et al. [48], using the maximum likelihood tree (Fig. S1) as our 202 phylogenetic hypothesis. We present results from both the LMM and PGLS models because the LMMs are robust to analysis of repeated measures and can therefore 204 be used on all unique species pairs in our dataset, but assume λ = 0, whereas the PGLS approach estimates λ and then uses it to adjust the internal branch lengths 206 such that the data meet the assumption of Brownian motion [48]. Seddon, Botero et al. For both the LMMs and PGLS models, response variables and predictors 208 were transformed to ensure model residuals were normally distributed and had homogeneous variance: maximum and total phenotypic divergence were Box-Cox 210 transformed; mean dichromatism and mean body mass were log-transformed. Parameter estimates are presented as mean ± SE. 212 Analysis 2: Effect of sexual selection on evolutionary rates of phenotypic divergence 214 The analyses presented in the main text indicate that sexual selection has different effects on phenotypic evolution of males and females (Tables S5 and S6). To 216 evaluate the strength of these differences, we compared models in which rates of phenotypic divergence were estimated separately for each sex to models in which 218 the data for both sexes were analyzed together. Joint AIC values for sex-specific models were computed by adding the likelihood values of models in Tables S5 and 220 S6 and adjusting the number of parameters accordingly. For all traits, sex-specific models were clearly better supported than models in which data from both sexes are 222 combined (Table S7). 224 Analysis 3: effect of sexual dichromatism on diversification Birth-death trees with the correction for the lagtime to species recognition were 226 simulated in the R package PhyloGen [49]. We simulated with λ ranging from 0 to 0.15 in 0.01 intervals, and from 0.15 to 0.90 in 0.05 intervals. For λ ≤ 0.4, μ ranged 228 from 0λ, 0.05λ, 0.1λ, 0.2λ…0.9λ, 0.95λ, 0.99λ. For λ > 0.4, the same rates of μ were used provided λ-μ < 0.45 (a necessary computational restriction given excessively 230 large trees sizes when λ-μ > 0.45). For each set of λ and μ 21 values of φ (φ = 0, 0.1, 0.2…1.9, 2.0) were used. 232 We adopt a simulation approach for this analysis because while there are methods available for estimating speciation/extinction rates from phylogenies while 234 correcting for the lag-time to speciation, no such method exists for sister species data. Moreover, the key advantage of the simulation method over the analytical 236 238 method is that it does not assume the survival of lineages. Seddon, Botero et al. Supplementary Figure 240 242 Figure S1 Maximum clade credibility tree illustrating the evolutionary relationships of 244 the passerine bird species included in this study Seddon, Botero et al. 246 248 250 Supplementary Tables Table S1 Phenotypic traits measured and analyses in which they were included Class of phenotype Morphology Plumage Specific trait Beak length Tarsus length Wing-chord length Crown (SW chroma) Crown (UV chroma) Throat (SW chroma) Throat (UV chroma) Back (SW chroma) Back (UV chroma) Belly (SW chroma) Belly (UV chroma) Tail (SW chroma) Tail (UV chroma) Wing-coverts (SW chroma) Wing-coverts (UV chroma) Number of species pairs included 252 Analysis Analysis Analysis 1 2 3 X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X X 69 52 39 Seddon, Botero et al. Table S2 Factor loadings from principal components 254 analysis on plumage reflectance data Wavelength (nm) 320 340 360 380 400 420 440 460 480 500 520 540 560 580 600 620 640 660 680 256 258 260 262 264 266 268 270 272 274 276 PC1 -0.123 -0.128 -0.081 0.011 0.109 0.173 0.194 0.201 0.202 0.065 -0.014 -0.075 -0.088 -0.041 -0.033 -0.041 -0.023 -0.020 -0.043 PC2 0.253 0.288 0.251 0.133 0.010 -0.089 -0.123 -0.141 -0.164 -0.004 0.040 0.080 0.049 -0.067 -0.106 -0.104 -0.127 -0.115 -0.082 Seddon, Botero et al. 278 Table S3 PGLS models of (a) total and (b) maximum phenotypic divergence between species pairs in relation to the intensity of sexual selection within species (dichromatism), sex, and other potentially confounding variables (n = 52 pairs). 280 (a) Total phenotypic divergence Fixed effects Dichromatism Sex Dichromatism * Sex Evolutionary age Sympatry Body mass Final model AIC AICc Parameter Estimate (b) 1.696 -3.437 5.396 1.099 0.081 2.660 Lambda 0.92 534.80 535.97 SE t P 1.461 0.863 1.340 1.120 0.749 1.348 F1,51 7.88 1.160 -3.981 4.026 0.981 0.108 1.973 R2 0.32 0.25 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.33 0.91 0.05 adj. R2 0.29 SE t P 0.369 0.229 0.349 0.022 0.196 0.335 F1,51 3.18 0.12 -2.88 2.84 0.40 1.51 1.50 R2 0.16 0.904 0.005 0.005 0.69 0.14 0.14 adj. R2 0.11 (b) Maximum phenotypic divergence Fixed effects Dichromatism Sex Dichromatism * Sex Evolutionary age Sympatry Body mass Final model AIC AICc 282 284 Parameter Estimate (b) 0.04 -0.66 0.99 0.01 0.30 0.50 Lambda 0.90 260.3 261.5 Seddon, Botero et al. Table S4 PGLS models of (a) total and (b) maximum phenotypic divergence 286 between species pairs in relation to the intensity of sexual selection within species (dichromatism), sex, and other potentially confounding variables, excluding mutually 288 ornamented species (n = 45 species pairs). (a) Total phenotypic divergence Parameter Fixed effects Estimate (b) Dichromatism 2.58 Sex -3.08 Dichromatism * Sex 4.32 Evolutionary age 1.40 Sympatry 0.16 Body mass 2.46 Lambda Final model 0.92 AIC 467.59 AICc 468.99 (b) Maximum phenotypic divergence Parameter Fixed effects Estimate (b) Dichromatism 0.16 Sex -0.59 Dichromatism * Sex 0.75 Evolutionary age 0.15 Sympatry 0.34 Body mass 0.43 Lambda Final model 0.88 AIC 233.47 AICc 234.87 290 SE t P 1.62 1.01 1.59 1.25 0.82 1.38 F1,44 4.78 1.59 -3.04 2.72 1.12 0.19 1.78 P <0.0001 0.12 <0.0001 0.01 0.27 0.85 0.08 adj. R2 0.21 SE t P 0.37 -2.16 1.77 0.47 1.56 1.22 P 0.05 0.71 0.03 0.08 0.64 0.12 0.23 adj. R2 0.08 0.42 0.27 0.43 0.32 0.22 0.36 F1,51 2.24 Seddon, Botero et al. 292 294 296 298 Table S5 Evolutionary divergence in male traits in relation to sexual selection. Comparison of support for models in which the rate of evolutionary divergence in traits is assumed to be independent of the strength of sexual selection (constant rate model, CR) or linearly associated with the strength of sexual selection (variable rates model, VR). For each model type we explore results under a Brownian motion (BM) model of evolution and an Ornstein-Uhlenbeck (OU) process. In the variable rates OU model, we allow the possibility that sexual selection is also linearly associated with the constraint parameter, α. Bold denotes models that are unambiguously supported by the data in a given candidate set (i.e., Akaike weights > 70% and ΔAICc > 2 when compared to the next best supported model). Trait Beak length Tarsus length Wing-chord length Crown SW chroma Crown UV chroma Model type Rate_0 ± SE † CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.032 ± 0.006 0.009 ± 0.005 0.148 ± 0.149 0.013 ± 0.021 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.025 ± 0.005 0.020 ± 0.008 0.031 ± 0.013 0.039 ± 0.01 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.020 ± 0.004 0.017 ± 0.007 0.020 0.017 ± 0.007 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.615 ± 0.121 0.745 ± 0.184 216.689 0.000 CR-BM VR-BM 0.442 ± 0.087 0.000 ± 0.044 α_0 ± SE † Slope for rate ± SE Slope for α ± SE AICc Akaike weight 0.044 ± 0.062 -50.24 -58.85 -58.69 -59.35 0.00 0.31 0.29 0.40 -0.032 -63.59 -61.86 -61.99 -58.25 0.52 0.22 0.23 0.04 0.000 ± 0.017 -75.22 -73.31 -73.06 -68.86 0.57 0.22 0.19 0.02 33.118 103.51 104.60 73.72 71.64 0.00 0.00 0.26 0.74 86.28 59.79 0.00 0.00 0.005 ± 0.002 0.967 ± 1.074 0.131 ± 0.257 0.012 ± 0.010 0.001 ± 0.002 0.075 ± 0.112 -0.002 0.221 ± 0.064 0.001 ± 0.001 0.001 0.000 ± 0.017 0.001 ± 0.002 -0.028 ± 0.019 121.623 249.237 184.007 0.061 ± 0.016 Seddon, Botero et al. Throat SW chroma Throat UV chroma Back SW chroma Back UV chroma Belly SW chroma Belly UV chroma CR-OU VR-OU 3.423 ± 3.134 0.000 ± 0.736 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.540 ± 0.106 0.000 2.544 ± 2.197 0.000 ± 0.599 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.518 ± 0.102 0.015 ± 0.120 4.660 ± 5.192 1.622 ± 3.081 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 1.198 ± 0.235 1.141 ± 0.349 29.431 ± 125.33 0.000 ± 0.216 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.494 ± 0.097 0 ± 0.118 5.101 ± 6.799 0.747 ± 4.398 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.765 ± 0.150 0.909 ± 0.251 19.561 ± 71.812 0.000 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.411 ± 0.081 0.000 1.792 ± 1.299 0.000 2.905 ± 2.684 1.347 ± 2.231 0.427 ± 0.365 0.040 ± 0.155 51.28 40.72 0.01 0.99 0.048 ± 0.124 96.78 91.69 85.89 82.15 0.00 0.01 0.13 0.86 0.022 ± 0.319 94.62 83.59 58.94 56.57 0.00 0.00 0.23 0.77 0.000 ± 0.779 138.20 140.31 94.69 81.00 0.00 0.00 0.00 1.00 0.000 ± 0.630 92.15 77.14 49.80 40.35 0.00 0.00 0.01 0.99 0.616 114.83 116.45 79.90 73.56 0.00 0.00 0.04 0.96 0.006 82.49 73.31 64.93 53.58 0.00 0.00 0.00 1.00 0.145 1.041 ± 0.978 1.330 ± 0.983 2.121 ± 1.01 0.118 ± 0.052 3.430 ± 3.86 0.779 ± 1.221 3.642 ± 5.643 0.013 ± 0.062 11.032 ± 46.995 15.842 9.166 ± 7.168 0.094 ± 0.044 4.502 ± 6.037 6.809 ± 16.531 1.519 ± 3.324 -0.03 ± 0.032 9.742 ± 35.800 12.217 7.430 0.083 1.103 ± 0.854 1.673 0.606 Seddon, Botero et al. CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.347 ± 0.068 0.313 ± 0.086 1.150 ± 0.607 0.587 ± 0.762 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.206 ± 0.04 0.171 ± 0.063 0.595 ± 0.448 0.590 ± 0.647 Wing coverts SW chroma CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.407 ± 0.08 0.000 ± 0.101 1.055 ± 0.555 0.000 Wing coverts UV chroma CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.307 ± 0.06 0.206 ± 0.095 0.684 ± 0.34 0.000 ± 0.625 Tail SW chroma Tail UV chroma 300 0.322 ± 0.495 73.81 75.66 60.40 63.76 0.00 0.00 0.84 0.16 -0.016 ± 0.093 46.52 48.22 41.16 45.43 0.06 0.02 0.82 0.10 0.000 82.03 59.71 76.26 57.27 0.00 0.23 0.00 0.77 0.073 ± 0.118 67.31 67.69 63.81 66.16 0.11 0.09 0.61 0.19 0.007 ± 0.014 0.734 ± 0.425 0.044 ± 0.763 0.289 ± 0.467 0.008 ± 0.013 0.514 ± 0.482 -0.001 ± 0.102 0.582 ± 0.692 0.078 ± 0.031 0.450 ± 0.296 0.412 0.194 0.022 ± 0.024 0.342 ± 0.234 0.173 ± 0.337 0.245 ± 0.310 Seddon, Botero et al. 302 304 306 308 Table S6 Evolutionary divergence in female traits in relation to sexual selection. AICc values are used to compare the support for models in which the rate of evolutionary divergence in traits is assumed to be independent of the strength of sexual selection (constant rate model, CR) versus linearly associated with the strength of sexual selection (variable rates model, VR). For each model type we explore results under a Brownian motion (BM) model of evolution and an OrnsteinUhlenbeck (OU) process. In the variable rates OU model, we allow the possibility that sexual selection is also linearly associated with the constraint parameter, α. For each trait, bold denotes the model that is unambiguously best supported by the data among the four alternatives (i.e., Akaike weights > 70% and ΔAICc > 2 when compared to the next best supported model). Trait Beak length Tarsus length Wing-chord length Crown SW chroma Crown UV Model type Rate_0 ± SE† CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.087 ± 0.017 0.099 ± 0.024 0.239 ± 0.12 0.235 ± 0.197 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.065 ± 0.013 0.075 ± 0.017 0.157 ± 0.084 0.177 ± 0.102 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.068 ± 0.013 0.064 0.126 ± 0.062 0.167 ± 0.003 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 1.185 ± 0.232 0.92 78.014 0.000 CR-BM 0.405 ± 0.079 α_0 ± SE † AICc Akaike weight 0.311 ± 0.349 1.65 3.03 -5.61 -5.91 0.01 0.01 0.45 0.53 0.018 ± 0.085 -13.04 -12.11 -17.38 -13.97 0.08 0.05 0.73 0.13 0.016 ± 0.020 -11.04 -14.06 -11.86 -14.57 0.08 0.35 0.12 0.45 683.890 137.61 132.84 96.03 99.34 0.00 0.00 0.84 0.16 81.76 0.00 Slope for rate ± SE Slope for α ± SE -0.003 ± 0.003 0.506 ± 0.303 0.000 ± 0.588 0.041 ± 0.067 -0.002 ± 0.002 0.388 ± 0.275 -0.003 ± 0.011 0.344 ± 0.304 -0.003 0.230 ± 0.191 -0.008 0.201 ± 0.108 -0.043 28.511 976.495 2791.748 Seddon, Botero et al. chroma Throat SW chroma Throat UV chroma Back SW chroma Back UV chroma Belly SW chroma Belly UV chroma Tail SW VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.226 ± 0.117 2.250 ± 1.948 0.054 ± 2.303 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.592 ± 0.116 0.638 ± 0.164 1.961 ± 1.558 0.000 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.588 ± 0.115 0.548 ± 0.150 77.087 145.401 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 2.523 ± 0.495 2.940 ± 0.586 50.957 42.107 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.932 ± 0.183 0.947 ± 0.253 20.889± 117.423 15.075 ± 93.281 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.906 ± 0.178 1.063 ± 0.224 12007.112 0.000 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.409 ± 0.080 0.289 ± 0.106 3.884 ± 7.870 0.000 ± 19.819 CR-BM 0.484 ± 0.095 0.040 ± 0.033 1.403 ± 1.280 0.952 ± 1.698 1.152 ± 1.828 0.266 ± 0.684 80.84 65.36 66.10 0.00 0.59 0.41 0.119 101.57 103.59 95.33 94.30 0.02 0.01 0.37 0.61 0.000 101.18 103.20 82.50 85.10 0.00 0.00 0.79 0.21 0.821 176.91 172.18 99.41 102.98 0.00 0.00 0.86 0.14 0.000 ± 2.501 125.12 127.27 69.13 71.12 0.00 0.00 0.73 0.27 2.335 123.66 123.51 76.50 76.92 0.00 0.00 0.55 0.45 2.311 ± 11.185 82.31 82.59 55.75 57.73 0.00 0.00 0.73 0.27 91.03 0.00 -0.010 ± 0.022 0.622 ± 0.587 0.832 1.038 0.009 ± 0.024 36.539 101.918 15.228 -0.131 ± 0.029 17.449 11.164 -0.100 -0.003 ± 0.037 12.804± 71.974 14.152 ± 81.537 1.715 ± 3.685 -0.039 ± 0.016 6291.482 9.621 9.889 0.026 ± 0.025 3.029 ± 6.300 5.456 ± 25.582 6.125 ± 36.006 Seddon, Botero et al. chroma Tail UV chroma Wing coverts SW chroma 312 0.507 ± 0.117 1.500 ± 0.751 1.200 ± 1.096 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.307 ± 0.060 0.232 ± 0.099 3.103 ± 3.932 1.243 ± 2.547 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.304 ± 0.060 0.291 ± 0.128 1.303 ± 0.945 0.000 -0.005 ± 0.013 0.650 ± 0.363 0.000 ± 0.657 0.400 ± 0.473 0.401 ± 0.426 93.05 79.56 81.84 0.00 0.76 0.24 0.259 ± 0.7 67.44 68.89 50.98 54.23 0.00 0.00 0.84 0.16 0.243 66.85 69.00 54.15 57.39 0.00 0.00 0.83 0.16 0.721 ± 2.461 73.37 73.46 49.49 53.47 0.00 0.00 0.88 0.12 0.017 ± 0.023 2.640 ± 3.429 0.605 ± 1.235 1.967 ± 3.335 0.003 ± 0.026 0.974 ± 0.767 0.455 0.288 CR-BM 0.344 ± 0.068 VR-BM 0.209 ± 0.106 0.031 ± 0.028 CR-OU 3.660 ± 7.862 3.226 ± 7.097 VR-OU 0.000 ± 5.869 0.996 ± 2.869 0.408 ± 5.768 † Parameter estimate when the index of sexual selection is equal to zero Wing coverts UV chroma 310 VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU Seddon, Botero et al. 314 316 318 320 Table S7 Evolutionary divergence in traits when male and female data are combined. As in Tables S5 and S6 we present here the results for models in which the rate of evolutionary divergence in traits is assumed to be independent of (constant rate model, CR) or linearly associated with the strength of sexual selection (variable rates model, VR), each under a Brownian model of evolution, BM, and an Ornstein-Uhlenbeck process, OU. In the variable rates OU model, we allow the possibility that sexual selection is also linearly associated with the constraint parameter, α. AICc values are used here to compare the support for models derived from sexes-combined versus sex-specific data. Model variants for each trait are compared to their equivalent models in Tables S5 and S6 and bold highlights traits for which combining data for both sexes yields a better supported model than either of the sex-specific alternatives. Trait Beak length Tarsus length Wing-chord length Crown SW chroma Crown UV chroma Model type Rate_0 ± SE † CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.059 ± 0.008 0.057 ± 0.012 0.173 ± 0.068 0.096 ± 0.112 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.045 ± 0.006 0.05 ± 0.009 0.089 ± 0.031 0.098 ± 0.035 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.044 ± 0.006 0.055 0.063 ± 0.019 0.079 ± 0.003 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.9 ± 0.125 1.08 ± 0.153 96.572 0.001 CR-BM VR-BM 0.423 ± 0.059 0.019 ± 0.065 α_0 ± SE † Slope for rate ± SE Slope for α ± SE AICc 0.272 ± 0.363 -38.28 -36.28 -55.49 -55.17 0.000 ± 0.030 -66.92 -65.81 -73.06 -69.65 0.002 ± 0.003 -69.73 -75.70 -70.12 -74.14 1705128 244.49 235.26 167.75 166.31 0.001 ± 0.002 0.547 ± 0.255 0.000 ± 0.578 0.044 ± 0.066 -0.001 ± 0.001 0.267 ± 0.144 0.265 ± 0.177 -0.002 ± 0.003 -0.002 0.122 ± 0.095 -0.004 0.117 ± 0.043 -0.049 ± 0.008 42.75 4748386 7280477 0.093 ± 0.028 166.01 141.02 Seddon, Botero et al. Throat SW chroma Throat UV chroma Back SW chroma Back UV chroma CR-OU VR-OU 3.071 ± 2.146 0.07 ± 0.753 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.566 ± 0.079 0 2.249 ± 1.349 0 CR-BM VR-BM VR-OU 0.553 ± 0.077 0.35 ± 0.095 8.926 ± 19.346 23.621 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 1.861 ± 0.258 2.235 ± 0.328 42.147 ± 293 0 CR-BM VR-BM 0.713 ± 0.099 0.507 ± 0.138 14.025 ± 36.428 7.592 ± 28.750 CR-OU CR-OU VR-OU Belly SW chroma Belly UV chroma CR-BM VR-BM VR-OU 0.835 ± 0.116 0.992 ± 0.155 21.805 ± 68.45 0 CR-BM VR-BM 0.41 ± 0.057 0.167 ± 0.097 CR-OU 2.226 ± 1.596 0.595 ± 0.468 1.198 ± 1.109 0.118 ± 0.18 113.51 101.77 0.096 196.34 192.84 177.42 167.15 0.166 0.81 ± 0.549 0.871 0.895 193.89 190.67 0.044 ± 0.024 5.139 ± 11.266 3.698 139.86 24.277 -0.149 ± 0.837 137.19 1.993 320.03 316.34 189.84 182.20 -0.09 ± 0.02 15.086 ± 101.379 12.747 15.802 220.29 219.18 0.045 ± 0.032 10.164 ±26.416 12.532 ± 38.562 116.43 2.080 ± 3.033 0.000 ± 1.230 236.75 235.84 -0.036 ± 0.013 11.218 ± 35.227 11.529 108.84 152.08 8.876 0.057 ± 0.031 1.368 142.00 162.68 156.10 Seddon, Botero et al. CR-OU VR-OU 2.345 ± 1.58 0 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.416 ± 0.058 0.416 ± 0.073 1.323 ± 0.478 0.872 ± 0.636 CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.256 ± 0.036 0.202 ± 0.058 1.702 ± 1.383 0.913 ± 1.205 Wing coverts SW chroma CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.355 ± 0.049 0.021 ± 0.071 1.109 ± 0.466 0 ± 0.456 Wing coverts UV chroma CR-BM VR-BM CR-OU VR-OU 0.326 ± 0.045 0.207 ± 0.071 1.273 ± 0.682 0 ± 0.754 Tail SW chroma Tail UV chroma 322 † 1.614 ± 1.138 3.584 1.801 0.315 117.11 105.33 0.379 ± 0.322 164.14 166.22 137.36 138.08 0.118 ± 0.281 113.92 114.82 89.30 92.15 0.029 ± 0.067 147.87 130.15 128.95 112.23 0.183 ± 0.227 138.74 136.96 112.99 113.79 0 ± 0.01 0.683 ± 0.273 0 ± 0.489 0.358 ± 0.332 0.012 ± 0.013 1.525 ± 1.311 0.233 ± 0.384 1.153 ± 1.325 0.082 ± 0.03 0.608 ± 0.297 0.539 ± 0.397 0.300 ± 0.201 0.027 ± 0.019 0.859 ± 0.511 0.304 ± 0.527 0.422 ± 0.428 Parameter estimate when the index of sexual selection is equal to zero Seddon, Botero et al. Table S8 Comparison of support for diversification rates models fitted to the data 324 with and without sexual dichromatism 326 Constant rate* Variable rate † 0 0.014 0 0.002 0 0.011 b at dichromatism = 0 0.25 0.20 b at dichromatism = 22 0.25 0.50 d at dichromatism = 0 0 0 d at dichromatism = 22 0 0.05 Net at dichromatism = 0 0.25 0.20 Net at dichromatism = 22 0.25 0.45 -73.55 -72.29 3 5 153.11 154.58 0 1.47 Model Slope birth rate (b) across gradient Slope death rate (d) across gradient Net slope Maximum Likelihood Estimate # Parameters in model AIC Δ AIC *b and d do not vary across dichromatism gradient 328 330 †b and d vary across dichromatism gradient Seddon, Botero et al. Supplementary References 332 1. 334 336 338 340 2. 3. 342 4. 344 5. 346 6. 348 7. 350 8. 352 9. 354 10. 356 358 360 362 364 366 368 370 372 374 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. Armenta J.K., Dunn P.O., Whittingham L.A. 2008 Quantifying avian sexual dichromatism: a comparison of methods. J Exp Biol 211(15), 2423-2430. (doi:10.1242/jeb.013094). Phillimore A.B., Orme C.D., Thomas G.H., Blackburn T.M., Bennett P.M., Gaston K.J., Owens I.P.F. 2008 Sympatric speciation in birds is rare: insights from range data and simulations. Am Nat 171, 646-657. Weir J.T., Price T.D. 2011 Limits to speciation inferred from times to secondary sympatry and ages of hybridizing species along a latitudinal gradient. Am Nat 177, 462-469. Weir J.T., Wheatcroft D. 2011 A latitudinal gradient in rates of evolution of avian syllable diversity and song length. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 278, 1713-1720. Dunning J.B. 2010 CRC Handbook of Avian Body Masses. Boca Raton, Florida, CRC Press. Cuthill I.C., Bennett A.T.D., Partridge J.C., Maier E.J. 1999 Plumage reflectance and the objective assessment of avian sexual dichromatism. Am Nat 153(2), 183-200. Endler J.A. 1990 On the measurement and classification of color in studies of animal color patterns. Biol J Linn Soc 41(4), 315-352. Hart N.S. 2001 The visual ecology of avian photoreceptors. Prog Retin Eye Res 20(5), 675-703. Bennett A.T.D., Cuthill I.C., Partridge J.C., Lunau K. 1997 Ultraviolet plumage colors predict mate preferences in starlings. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 94(16), 8618-8621. Endler J.A., Théry M. 1996 Interacting effects of lek placement, display behavior, ambient light, and color patterns in three neotropical forest-dwelling birds. Am Nat 148(3), 421-452. Hunt S., Cuthill I.C., Bennett A.T.D., Griffiths R. 1999 Preferences for ultraviolet partners in the blue tit. Anim Behav 58, 809-815. Macedonia J.M. 2001 Habitat light, colour variation, and ultraviolet reflectance in the Grand Cayman anole, Anolis conspersus. Biol J Linn Soc 73(3), 299-320. Stein A.C., Uy J.A.C. 2006 Plumage brightness predicts male mating success in the lekking golden-collared manakin. Behav Ecol 17, 41-47. Vorobyev M., Osorio D. 1998 Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 265(1394), 351-358. Endler J.A., Mielke P.W. 2005 Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biol J Linn Soc 86, 405-431. Vorobyev M., Osorio D., Bennett A.T.D., Marshall N.J., Cuthill I.C. 1998 Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. J Comp Physio 183(5), 621633. Stoddard M.C., Prum R.O. 2008 Evolution of avian plumage color in a tetrahedral color space: a phylogenetic analysis of New World buntings. Am Nat 172(5), 740740. (doi:10.1086/593138). Badyaev A.V., Hill G.E. 2003 Avian sexual dichromatism in relation to phylogeny and ecology. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 34, 27-49. 24 Seddon, Botero et al. 376 378 380 382 384 386 388 390 392 394 396 398 400 402 404 406 408 410 412 414 416 418 420 19. Owens I.P.F., Hartley I.R. 1998 Sexual dimorphism in birds: why are there so many forms of dimorphism? Proc Roy Soc Lond B 265, 397-407. 20. Møller A.P., Cuervo J.J. 1998 Speciation and feather ornamentation in birds. Evolution 52, 859-869. 21. Dunn P.O., Whittingham L.A., Pitcher T.E. 2001 Mating systems, sperm competition and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in birds. Evolution 55, 161-175. 22. Owens I.P.F., Bennett A.T.D., Harvey P.H. 1999 Species richness among birds: body size, life history, sexual selection or ecology? Proc Roy Soc Lond B 266, 933-939. 23. Seddon N., Merrill R.M., Tobias J.A. 2008 Sexually selected traits predict patterns of species richness in a diverse clade of suboscine birds. Am Nat 171, 620-631. 24. Barraclough T.G., Harvey P.H., Nee S. 1995 Sexual selection and taxonomic diversity in passerine birds. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 259, 211-215. 25. Phillimore A.B., Freckleton R.P., Orme C.D.L., Owens I.P.F. 2006 Ecology predicts large-scale patterns of phylogenetic diversification in birds. Am Nat 168, 220–229. 26. Morrow E.H., Pitcher T.E., Arnqvist G. 2003 No evidence that sexual selection is an 'engine of speciation' in birds. Ecol Lett 6, 228-234. 27. Kruger O. 2008 Engines of speciation: a comparative study in birds of prey. J Evol Biol 21, 861–872. 28. Sol D., Stirling D.G., Lefebvre L. 2005 Behavioral drive or behavioral inhibition in evolution: subspecific diversification in Holarctic passerines. Evolution 59, 26692677. 29. Stuart-Fox D., Owens I.P.F. 2003 Species richness in agamid lizards: chance, body size, sexual selection or ecology? J Evol Biol 16, 659-669. 30. Misof B. 2002 Diversity of Anisoptera (Odonata): Infering speciation processes from patterns of morphological diversity. Zoology 105(355–365). 31. Wagner C.E., Harmon L.J., Seehausen O. 2012 Ecological opportunity and sexual selection together predict adaptive radiation. Nature doi:10.1038/nature11144. 32. McLain D.K., Moulton M.P., Redfearn T.P. 1995 Sexual selection and the risk of extinction in oceanic islands. Oikos 74, 27-34. 33. Morrow E.H., Pitcher T.E. 2003 Sexual selection and the risk of extinction in birds. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 270, 1793-1799. 34. Sorci G., Møller A.P., Clobert J. 1998 Plumage dichromatism of birds predicts introduction success in New Zealand. J Anim Ecol 67(2), 263-269. 35. Promislow D.E.L., Montgomerie R., Martin T.E. 1992 Mortality costs of sexual dimorphism in birds. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 250(1328), 143-150. 36. Møller A.P. 1997 Immune defence, extra-pair paternity, and sexual selection in birds. 264, 561-566. 37. Garamszegi L.Z., Pavlova D.Z., Eens M., Møller A.P. 2007 The evolution of song in female birds in Europe. Behav Ecol 18(1), 86-96. 38. Nadeau N., Burke T., Mundy N.I. 2007 Evolution of an avian pigmentation gene correlates with a measure of sexual selection. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 274, 1807-1813. 39. Spottiswoode C.N., Tøttrup A.P., Coppack T. 2006 Sexual selection predicts advancement of avian spring migration in response to climate change. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 273, 3023–3029. 40. Drummond A.J., Rambaut A. 2007 BEAST: Bayesian evolutionary analysis by sampling trees. BMC Evolutionary Biology 7, 214. 25 Seddon, Botero et al. 422 424 426 428 430 432 434 436 438 440 41. Swofford D.L. 2002 PAUP*. Phylogenetic Analysis Using Parsimony (*and Other Methods). Sunderland, Massachusetts, Sinauer Associates. 42. Weir J.T., Schluter D. 2008 Calibrating the avian molecular clock. Mol Ecol 17, 2321– 2328. 43. Bromham L., Cardillo M. 2003 Testing the link between the latitudinal gradient in species richness and rates of molecular evolution. J Evol Biol 16, 200-207. 44. Freckleton R.P. 2009 The seven deadly sins of comparative analysis. J Evol Biol 22, 1367 – 1375. 45. Kenward M.G., Roger J.H. 1997 Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. Biometrics 53, 983-997. 46. Self S.G., Liang K.Y. 1987 Large sample properties of the maximum likelihood estimator and the likelihood ratio test on the boundary of the parameter space. . J Am Stat Assoc 82, 605-611. 47. Pagel M.D. 1999 Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401, 877-884. 48. Freckleton R.P., Harvey P.H., Pagel M.D. 2002 Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: A test and review of evidence. Am Nat 160, 712-726. 49. Weir J.T., Schluter D. 2007 The latitudinal gradient in recent speciation and extinction rates of birds and mammals. Science 315, 1574–1576. 26