SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS PARTICIPANT RECRUITMENT AND SCREENING 22 BD participants were recruited from local mental health trusts and specialist affective disorder clinics within Greater Manchester, United Kingdom. Data from 2 BD participants were not analysed (one did not complete all runs, and one due to neuroradiological abnormality) as were the data from two HC participants (one did not complete all runs, one due to technical failure). The final sample consisted of 20 participants in each group (see Table 1). The study was approved by the National Health Service Research Ethics Committee in the United Kingdom (NREC) and the University of Manchester Senate Ethics Committee. Participants were included if they met the criteria for BD-1 or BD-2, but did not meet the criteria for a mood episode (in the case of the BD group) or a psychiatric disorder, in the case of healthy controls. To ensure a representative sample, anxiety, and alcohol and substance use disorders, were not exclusion criteria. However, participants were excluded if they had used substances in the last four months or alcohol in the past 24 hours. TASK In addition to the task description (see main methods), we emphasise that the four options in each trial did not differ in value or probability, ensuring that all participants had sufficient numbers of trials in each condition, and that there were no between-groups differences in trial numbers. Hence there was equivalent signal-to-noise ratio in all between-group comparisons. The duration of the selection phase was tapered over the first trials of each run (5 sec for trials 1-2, 3 sec for trials 3-4 and 2 sec thereafter) to ensure that participants had time to acclimatise and respond. Participants were instructed to respond in this time, and informed that if no timely response was issued, a random choice would be made. The colour chosen was displayed in the centre of the Roulette wheel during anticipation and outcome. The duration of both of these phases were separately jittered between 3-4 seconds. The relatively short duration of the anticipation and outcome events was selected for the following, largely pragmatic, reasons. First, to promote attentional focus on the task as far as possible in an impulsive clinical population that may find sustained attention difficult. Second, we required a high number of total trials to yield a sufficient number of trials in the low-probability conditions within an acceptable total scan duration. The crosshair was either 1.5 sec (75% of trials) or 4 sec (25%) to facilitate a strong baseline in the model, with which to compare conditions of interest. FMRI ACQUISITION, PREPROCESSING AND MODELLING Echo planar images were acquired over eight runs of 150 volumes, using a Phillips 1.5 T scanner (TR = 2.45 s, TE = 47 ms; flip angle = 90°). Volumes comprised 30 slices (4mm, no gap; in-plane voxel dimensions 1.5 x 1.5 mm), collected in ascending order, and with standard field of view. SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, University College London) was used to pre-process and analyse the images. Each participant’s functional images were motion-corrected with realignment to the mean image and time-corrected to the middle slice. The functional images were then coregistered with the structural image, spatially normalised to MNI space, and smoothed with an 8 mm gaussian kernel. The data for all participants were analysed at the first-level using the general linear model, as implemented in SPM8 (Friston et al., 1994). Regressors were convolved with a haemodynamic response function, and contrast images computed. The realignment parameters were included as additional regressors at the first level to reduce residual effects of motion. REPRESENTATION OF EXPECTED VALUE AND PREDICTION ERROR: In a separate model, “Net” expected value (EV) and prediction error (PE) were additionally entered, as parametric modulators of the anticipation (EV only) and outcome (PE only) event regressors. Net EV was calculated trial-wise as the arithmetic product of reward probability and magnitude minus the arithmetic product of loss probability and magnitude (i.e. EV = EVGain - EVLoss). PE was calculated trial-wise as the difference between EV and the actual outcome (outcome minus net EV). This allowed the model to take into consideration the pattern of neural activity that would be expected based on the relative differences between the parameters of EV and PE terms. SUPPLEMENTARY RESULTS STATE EFFECTS OF AFFECTIVE SYMPTOMS To examine the effects of residual mood symptoms on activity in the 3 ROIs, the main analyses were repeated with Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HRSD) and Bech-Rafaelsen Mania Scale (MAS) scores entered as covariates. Interactions with these covariates were separately explored by means of partial correlations that controlled for the effect of the other covariate not being inspected. Analyses focussed on the bipolar disorder (BD) group because controls did not exhibit a sufficient range of HRSD and MAS scores (Table 1). ANTICIPATION: Activity in dlPFC ROI showed a non-significant trend for a Magnitude-MAS interaction [F(1, 17) = 3.55, p= .077]. Controlling for HRSD, MAS score was positively correlated with the difference between small and large magnitude trials [r(17) = .416, p=.077]. In the main analysis, an overall effect of magnitude signalled that trials with large stake were perceived as more relevant than those with smaller amounts at stake. The positive correlation with manic symptoms therefore suggests that the perceived relevance of large-stake prospects is amplified with increasing manic symptoms. For the NAcc, there was a probability-HRSD interaction [F(1, 17) = 5.25, p = .035], with no other significant effects or interactions (p≥.14). HRSD was negatively correlated with the difference between high minus low probability trials, controlling for MAS [r(17)= -.486, p=.035]. In the main analyses, the BD group showed a greater increase in NAcc for highprobability rewards (compared to low), consistent with a greater drive to obtain rewards when they are on offer. Hence the negative association between this effect and depressive symptoms suggests that state depression is associated with a reduced drive to obtain reward. The vmPFC ROI showed a main effect of MAS [F(1, 17) = 7.07, p = .017], such that higher MAS scores were associated with greater vmPFC activity. This is consistent with state mania driving up the perceived value of prospects related to reward in vmPFC, regardless of their probability. OUTCOME: For dlPFC, there were no effects of state symptoms (p≥.1). For NAcc, there was a main effect of HRSD [F(1, 17) = 3.99, p=.052], consistent with state depression reducing activations for rewards and potentiating the deactivation observed for losses (see Supplementary Figure 1). There were also trends for MAS-magnitude [F(1, 17) = 3.74, p=.07] and MAS-probability-magnitude [F(1, 17) = 3.37, p=.084] interactions, although these did not reach significance. The MAS-probability-magnitude interaction was explored by repeating the ANCOVA separately for low and high probability outcomes. The MASmagnitude interaction was significant for low-probability (p=.04) but not high-probability (p=.88) outcomes. Controlling for HRSD, MAS was positively correlated with the difference between low-probability outcomes of small and large magnitude [r(17) = .474, p=.04]. In the primary analysis (see main results), NAcc activation was greater for large relative to small magnitude outcomes. Collectively these findings suggest that mania is associated with a greater increase in NAcc activation for large relative to small outcomes, particularly when they are unexpected. For vmPFC, there was a Probability-MAS [F(1, 17) = 5.45, p = .03] interaction. Controlling for HRSD, the size of the difference between outcomes of different probabilities (low- minus high- probability) was negatively correlated with MAS [r(17)=-.49, p=.03]). In the main analysis, the effect of probability on vmPFC was dependent on valence: the response was strongest to unexpected rewards relative to expected, with a greater deactivation in response to losses. Together, these findings indicate that state mania is associated with a reduction in both the vmPFC activation in response to unexpected rewards and its deactivation in response to unexpected losses (relative to expected rewards and losses respectively). PARAMETRIC ANALYSIS OF EXPECTED VALUE AND PREDICTION ERROR ANTICIPATION: Parametric modulation by EV was observed in R. dlPFC [F(1, 38)=9.0, p=.005] but not NAcc or vmPFC (p ≥ .414). There were no group effects or interactions (p ≥ .155). OUTCOME: Parametric modulation by PE was observed in bilateral NAcc [F(1, 38)=51.4, p<.001], bilateral dlPFC [F(1, 38)=17.7, p<.001] and vmPFC [t(39)=4.91, p<.001]. The BDR group evidenced greater PE-modulated activity in vmPFC [t(38)=2.07, p=.046] but no difference in NAcc or dlPFC (p≥ .3). SUPPLEMENTARY DISCUSSION The supplementary results reported here extend the main analyses by exploring the influence of affective state on the neural processing of motivational information. In this way, these findings provide insight into how these processes are affected during episodes of depression and mania. In addition, we performed parametric analyses to isolate neuronal populations responding to expected value (EV) and prediction error (PE). During anticipation, all three regions of interest showed modulation by affective state. In the main analyses, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) activity differentiated gambles based on their relevance, with increased activation when anticipating large and likely rewards (relative to small and unlikely rewards, respectively). This indicates that there is greater attentional tracking of prospects when the potential reward is high, as these were most consistent with the long-term goal of maximising winnings. This attentional bias was positively associated with manic symptoms indicating that highly rewarding behaviours may be particularly enticing during mania. Mania was also associated with greater overall activation of the vmPFC, indicating an indiscriminate inflation of the perceived value of prospects related to reward. Importantly, both the dlPFC and vmPFC effects occurred irrespective of the likelihood of reward. Hence mania may be associated with an inability to devalue highly rewarding behaviours even when the chances of negative repercussions are high. This offers a plausible explanation for the elevated levels of risk-taking seen during manic episodes (APA, 2000). In contrast, anticipatory activity in NAcc was modulated by depressive symptoms. Main analyses showed that this region preferentially activated when rewards were likely as opposed to unlikely, and that this effect was stronger for BD patients. This was interpreted as signifying a greater drive to obtain reward, when rewards are likely to be available. This effect was negatively associated with depressive symptoms, indicating that state depression reduces the drive to obtain reward. This fits with clinical features of depression, including reduced interest in pursuing previously enjoyed activities and reduced motivation to strive for goals (APA, 2000). Outcome-locked NAcc activity was also separately modulated by affective symptoms. On the one hand, state depression was associated with a blunted response to rewards and a potentiated response to losses (Supplementary Figure 1), consistent with anhedonia and hypersensitivity to punishment being cardinal features of depression (APA, 2000, Eisner et al., 2008). On the other hand, manic symptoms were associated a trend for greater increase in NAcc activation elicited by large relative to small outcomes, particularly when unexpected. This indicates that mania is associated either with an increased valuation of large magnitude outcomes, or a reduced valuation of small magnitude outcomes. Both scenarios could explain the increased risk-taking evident during mania. Indeed the seeking out of more potent rewards may either be driven directly, through increased valuation of these large rewards, or indirectly by reducing the intrinsic value of everyday activities associated with smaller reward. Activity in regions of NAcc, dlPFC and vmPFC was modulated by PE, consistent with these regions being involved in outcome evaluation and reinforcement learning (e.g. Corlett et al., 2004, Pessiglione et al., 2006). Greater PE-modulated activity in vmPFC in the BD group tallies with the overall higher outcome-locked activity in this region (main results). Together, these findings suggest that BD patients may track motivational outcomes more intently, consistent with the inflated importance of goal-attainment in BD (Johnson, 2005) and our previous fMRI findings in non-clinical hypomania (O'Sullivan et al., 2011). The steeper parametric response may be driven by an increased response to unexpected and large rewards (relative to controls), conceivably amplifying the subjective value of novel rewards over those from everyday behaviours. In summary, affective symptoms separately modulate how rewards are processed in BD, via somewhat distinct neural mechanisms. Depression was associated with an attenuation of the ventral striatal reward system, culminating in a reduced drive to obtain rewards and a dampening of the hedonic impact when rewards were received. Mania, on the other hand, modulated cortical attention and valuation systems, and was associated with greater tracking and valuation of potentially highly rewarding options, regardless of the likelihood of success or failure. BD patients do not show marked deficits in encoding the expected value or prediction error signals, and instead show an increased tracking of motivational outcomes in an integrated valuation system. REFERENCES APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). 4th ed. Washington, DC: APA; 2000. Corlett PR, Aitken MRF, Dickinson A, Shanks DR, Honey GD, Honey RAE, et al. Prediction error during retrospective revaluation of causal associations in humans: fMRI evidence in favor of an associative model of learning. Neuron. 2004;44(5):877-88. Eisner LR, Johnson SL, Carver CS. Cognitive responses to failure and success relate uniquely to bipolar depression versus mania. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117(1):154. Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JP, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Human Brain Mapping. 1994;2(4):189-210. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A. Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009;324(5927):646-8. Johnson SL. Mania and dysregulation in goal pursuit: a review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25(2):241-62. O'Sullivan N, Szczepanowski R, El-Deredy W, Mason L, Bentall RP. fMRI evidence of a relationship between hypomania and both increased goal-sensitivity and positive outcome-expectancy bias. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(10):2825-35. Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442(7106):1042-5. Supplementary Figure 1. Processing of gain and loss outcomes in nucleus accumbens (NAcc) is modulated by depressive symptoms. Within euthymic bipolar disorder patients, a subgroup exhibiting relatively high levels of depressive symptoms evidenced both a dampened response to reward as well as an enhanced response to loss, compared to those exhibiting low levels of depressive symptoms. Note that mean scores on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale were in the ‘remission’ range for both the low (1.25; SD = 1.03) and high (6.40; SD = 1.91) subgroups. ‘fMRI signal’ represents the change from baseline in the blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal averaged across left and right NAcc. Supplementary Table 1. Activated foci for whole-brain contrasts of main effects greater than rest (crosshair) during anticipation of outcome. Anticipation p threshold Cluster threshold Probability (High > Low) 0.05 FWE 8 Region Rt Somatosensory Association Cortex / Extrastriate cortex Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (triangularis) Rt Frontal eye field Magnitude (Large > Small) 0.001 Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) Rt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex 0.001 Coordinates X Y Z Cluster size Z score 7/19 44 45 8 30 46 46 32 -66 10 40 10 38 32 20 60 423 44 13 13 5.74 5.23 4.83 4.92 44 47 47 8 48/44 44 44 -34 4 -50 12 26 22 32 16 32 0 -2 48 28 317 152 102 160 44 4.38 4.18 4.14 3.73 3.66 7 18 48/45 40 37 26 30 46 -38 56 -64 -88 26 -40 -56 48 4 28 38 -6 1043 95 224 86 58 5.07 4.31 4.16 3.93 3.92 44/6 6 -48 -34 8 2 34 60 98 23 3.77 3.62 20 Rt Frontal eye field Lt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) Parametric EV Brodmann Area 20 Rt Precuneus Rt V2 Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (triangularis) Lt Supramarginal Gyrus Rt Fusiform Lt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) / Premotor cortex Lt Premotor cortex Supplementary Table 2. Activated foci for whole-brain contrasts of main effects greater than rest (crosshair) at outcome. p threshold Cluster threshold Valence (Gain > Loss) 0.05 FWE 9 Probability (High > Low) 0.001 Magnitude (Large > Small) 0.001 Parametric PE 0.05 FWE Outcome Region Brodmann Area Coordinates X Y Z Cluster z size score Lt Ventral striatum Rt Ventral striatum Lt Ventromedial prefrontal cortex Lt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (triangularis) Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) Lt dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 25 25 10 47 45 44 9 9 -10 10 -8 -40 44 52 -24 -16 12 14 44 46 42 12 32 32 -10 -8 -2 0 -2 20 42 52 502 370 670 52 36 175 19 10 7.34 7.06 6.23 5.90 5.81 5.30 4.94 4.68 Rt Middle / Superior temporal gyrus Lt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex Lt Retrosplenial cingulate cortex 22/21 47 29 62 -40 -4 -50 22 -40 16 -6 10 38 64 41 3.82 3.62 3.50 Rt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex Rt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) Lt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex Rt Precuneus Lt Inferior frontal gyrus (opercularis) 47 44 47 7 48/44 44 46 -34 32 -48 26 14 20 -62 14 0 34 -2 44 28 96 82 83 39 33 3.90 3.81 3.71 3.54 3.51 25 25 -10 8 12 14 -12 -8 153 184 6.12 5.91 8/9/32 25 -16 -4 32 32 54 6 45 8 5.29 5.08 26 26 6 Lt Ventral striatum Rt Ventral striatum Lt dorsolateral prefrontal cortex/dorsal anterior cingulate Lt Subgenual cortex Supplementary Table 3. Activated foci for whole-brain contrasts of interactive effects greater than rest (crosshair) during anticipation and outcome. Anticipation P threshold Cluster threshold Probability x Magnitude 0.005 43 Brodmann Area Coordinates X Y Z Cluster size z score 47 38/47 30 42 38 16 4 -12 71 54 3.57 3.25 Rt Precuneus Lt Posterior cingulate 7 23 36 -8 -62 -50 44 28 177 39 4.77 3.66 Lt Fusiform gyrus Lt Inferior temporal gyrus Rt Posterior cingulate 37 20 23 -48 -38 2 -54 -26 -36 4 -4 34 86 28 56 3.85 3.72 3.51 Lt Inferior Temporal Gyrus Lt Posterior cingulate 20 23 -42 -2 -24 -36 -4 32 68 80 3.43 3.11 Region Rt Lateral orbitofrontal cortex Rt Temporal pole / orbitofrontal cortex Outcome Valence x Probability Valence x Magnitude Valence x Probablity x Magnitude 0.001 0.001 0.005 26 26 42