Preliminary Examination for Theology 2015 Moderators’ Report Sixty-five candidates sat the Preliminary Examination this year. Twenty-one gained distinctions while one failed to pass in one paper (New Testament Greek). Two candidates missed papers due to illness and will be given the opportunity to sit them at the end of Trinity. Three candidates elected to take four papers, with the remainder taking three. The highest average mark was 78.33; the lowest, 46 (from a candidate who just scraped bare a pass on two of the papers). 29 candidates’ average marks were in the 6069 range with a further 26 candidates’ averages in the 70-79 range, indicating a high quality of marks overall. Nine of the candidates with averages in the 70-79 range did not, however, meet the criteria for a distinction; this generally reflects candidates who performed very strongly on a language paper but only averagely well on their other papers. Most of those who gained distinctions revealed some real ability, although some were helped there by very high marks on a language paper (more on which at the end), not least since this was a year in which performance on both the Greek and the Hebrew papers was unusually strong. Both the lowest mark on an individual paper (34) and the highest (97) were obtained in Greek. In the main the process ran very smoothly. The Examination Schools proved most helpful in accommodating timetabling requests and, with the exception of one apparent misunderstanding, scripts were delivered to markers in a timely fashion. The support given by the faculty office, in particular Dr Kathrin Gowers, was exemplary and greatly eased the load of the Chair of Examiners. The Hebrew paper was afflicted with a few minor glitches (see detailed report below), but since candidates’ performance on that paper this year was exceptionally strong, this seems not to have had any significant negative impact. Five candidates received proctorial dispensation to sit all or part of the exam under special conditions. In addition two applications for consideration of factors affecting performance in examinations were received from the Proctors’ Office and were duly considered at the final moderators’ meeting. In one case the moderators considered the factors described to be sufficiently serious, and the marks sufficiently close to distinction standard, to merit awarding the candidate a distinction. In the other case the candidate was not sufficiently close to a borderline to allow any adjustment of the overall result to be made. As noted above the moderators also received a statement from the Proctors’ Office that one candidate who had failed to appear for one paper had good and sufficient reasons for their non-appearance and should therefore be given the earliest possible opportunity to sit the missed paper on another occasion. Arrangements will be made for this candidate to sit this paper at the time of the June re-sits. The reports which follow relate to individual papers. 1 Introduction to Philosophy 27 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 64.93 70+ 7 60-69 16 50-59 3 40-49 1 390 Section A: Logic 25 candidates sat the paper. 27 logic questions were answered: 2 candidates answered 2 questions each and 23 candidates answered 1 question each. The average mark per question was approximately 15 out of 25 or 60%. The distribution of marks out of 25 was: 21, 20 (x2), 19 (x2), 17 (x7), 16 (x2), 15.5, 15 (x2), 13.5, 13 (x2), 12 (x2), 11, 10, 9, 6, 3. Comments Question 1 Reasonably well done. Several candidates answered part (a) incorrectly, by confusing validity and soundness. There was some confusion about part (d): the statement was intended as having the overall form of a univer sal quantification but several candidates interpreted it as existential in form (there is some particular sentence that, when added to the premise set of any argument renders the argument valid). Of course, this statement is also true, but the universal/ existential confusion led to some poor justifications. Most candidates got the correct answer to parts (f)-(j) but there were some weak justifications. In particular, many candidates considered the corresponding conditionals, without relating this back to the original formulation. The easiest way to justify the falsity of these statements is to offer a valuation of the variables but, surprisingly, very few candidates took this approach. Question 2 Reasonably well done. In part (a), several candidates failed to distinguish the notions of (i) function; (ii) truth function; (iii) truth functional connective. In particular, there were several confusions of worldly and linguistic entities. Part (c) was generally well done, but many candidates made life difficult for themselves but not appealing to previous answers in later questions. For example, once the material conditional has been defined, it is straightforward to define the material biconditional in terms of the material conditional, but most candidates started again from scratch. Part (d) was generally poorly answered. Just a few candidates pointed out that both conjunction and the material conditional yield true when both inputs are true and so cannot express negation. Instead, most candidates just stated that conjunction and the material conditional are two-place whereas negation is one-place, which of course is not what prevents the former from expressing the latter. 2 Question 3 Very well done. The proofs given in response to part (a) were generally very good indeed. The mistakes were usually due to failing to respect the conditions that accompany the quantifier rules. Question (b) was generally poorly answered: only a very small number of candidates used the distinction between basic and derived rules of inference. Question 4 Reasonably well done. In part (a), several candidates failed to read the question correctly and gave their definitions in English, rather than £2, as requested. In part (b), most mistakes were made by failing to appreciate that some sets of properties were inconsistent. For example, in response to part (iv) of (b), very few candidates explained that a complete relation must also be reflexive. Question 5 There were very few responses to this question, but those who answered it generally did very well. There was a typo in the question: an extra ‘not’ appeared in the initial setup. This was noticed and announced early in the examination, however, and did not appear to have affected responses to the question. General Philosophy 28 candidates sat the paper. The questions were answered thus: 6a: 3 answers 6b: 0 7a: 5 answers 7b: 3 answers 8a: 3 answers 8b: 3answers 9a: 0 9b: 10 answers 10a: 0 10b: 7 answers 11a: 1 answer 11b: 8 answers 6b: 8 answers Overall: The questions were all answered reasonably well – with only a couple of exceptions. I have not examined this paper for several years, but the character of the essays have changed markedly over that time. There used to be less knowledge of standard moves, but there were real attempts to grapple with issues. This time I noticed that all the students were familiar with the same standard moves. I think this is evidence that they all attend the lectures. As a result, there is a general impression of good work. However, there seems less evidence that students are really thinking through the issues and showing a feel for the problem. Many seemed simply to be repeating the arguments they had been given. There was too much reliance on “my opinion”, and phrases like 3 2 “crazy argument” and “ludicrous” were peppering these papers. In short, too much assertion and not enough nuance. Another overall point to make concerns the a/b option. On the whole the b option question was 3 times as popular as the a option question (100 answers over all 3 papers for the b questions and 33 answers over all 3 papers for the a question). It was notable that, even when answering the a option very few candidates were familiar with the basic texts. Their references to Descartes, for example, were largely second hand, and knowledge of Locke’s text was non-existent. Knowledge of Hume was scant. In most cases there was almost no difference between the way the candidate answered the a questions and the way other students answered the b questions. I would recommend that there is a statement on the examination explaining why the a/b/ option exists. I have come across students who are simply unaware of the difference – despite the attempt this year to mention an author or give a quotation in each a question. The least popular questions were the a questions on God and evil, the a question on induction and the a question on freewill. All the b questions were answered by between 8 and 28 candidates. Although 3 questions had only 1 or 2 answers, there was a fair spread for the rest of the questions. 4b, on freewill, proved the most popular. 1a/b : There was a stronger understanding of the externalist moves with respect to scepticism (mostly using Nozick’s work) but little real assessment of the adequacy of this. Very few students exhibited a real understanding of the sceptical question. This may reflect the times, but it is still notable. 2a/b: The a question was answered with only the most rudimentary knowledge of Bk. II ch. xxvii of Locke’s Essay. Most candidates knew Reid’s objection, and some knew Butler’s. There was a poor grasp of the Williams’ argument in many of the paper, and a very passing knowledge of Parfit’s work. 3a/b: The standard arguments were rolled out in response to a (with very little understanding of Descartes) and in response to b most candidates discussed Jackson’s Mary argument. 4a/b: Very few candidates chose to answer a, and of these only there was a fair, but not great, knowledge of Hume’s text. The answers to the b question were mostly good. This question received the most responses (28 overall). 5a/b: Very few answers to the a version, but the b version proved popular. There was a fair–to-good grasp of the issues here. 6a/b: Only one person chose the a option, but many more chose the b option. This question received rather good responses. The standard moves were rehearsed, but they were done well. Arguably this question received the best answers. 4 Moral Philosophy Q1/Q12. Is Mill’s appeal to the ‘quality’ of pleasures necessary to successfully address the objection that utilitarianism is ‘a doctrine worthy only of swine’? This question was very popular, and was answered well on the whole. Some answers suffered as a consequence of insufficient knowledge of Mill’s text; too many relied heavily on Crisp’s discussion. Better answers noted that Mill himself seems to think there is an adequate response to the objection which does not make appeal to the notion of quality of pleasures: ‘utilitarian writers in general have placed the superiority of mental over bodily pleasures chiefly in the greater permanency, safety, uncostliness, &c., of the former—that is, in their circumstantial advantages rather than in their intrinsic nature. And on all these points utilitarians have fully proved their case’. (Utilitarianism, Ch. 2, Chapter 4) Q2/Q13. Is Mill’s account of the value of virtue in chapter 4 of Utilitarianism convincing? Students displayed mixed levels of knowledge of the passage in question. Very few demonstrated awareness of Mill’s earlier discussion of virtue in Chapter 2. Q3/Q14. Should we try to have whatever beliefs will best promote the general happiness? Very few students answered this question. Those who did tended not to address the question in hand, discussing motives and intentions instead of beliefs, or focusing on specifically moral beliefs, rather than beliefs in general. Q4/Q15. Can utilitarians give a convincing account of just punishment? The few students who answered this question did so very competently, focusing on the tension between utilitarianism being a forward-looking theory, and our common conception of just punishment as involving backward-looking elements. Q5/Q16. Does the most plausible version of rule-utilitarianism collapse into actutilitarianism? This was a very popular question, which was generally answered well, and sometimes very impressively. Students were generally clearer on which versions of RU do collapse into AU than on which ones might not. Many of the best answers successfully brought in detailed discussion of multi-level act-utilitarianism. One nice point, made by a few students, is that the word ‘collapse’ may be unduly negative. Q6/Q17. ‘Utilitarianism’s moral prescriptions simply stray too far from commonsense moral thought to be plausible.’ Do you agree? Answers to this question were mixed. The best ones tended to be well focused on specific counter-intuitive implications of utilitarianism. Most of the better ones questioned whether departure from common-sense morality need be a bad thing. Many answers would have benefited from better selection of examples where utilitarianism and common-sense moral thought most clearly come apart. Many students addressed this question largely by discussing Williams, especially the Jim and the Indians example, with 5 varying degrees of success. There were some apt references to the distinction between decision procedure and criterion of right action. A10294W1: The Christian Doctrine of Creation 38 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 66.34 70+ 5 60-69 33 50-59 0 40-49 0 390 All questions were attempted. The scripts were generally good, though perhaps candidates were aided by the fact that the questions did not vary much from previous years. The best candidates made an effort to integrate the systematic theology and the science and religion aspects of the paper. Candidates who earned high marks also offered constructive theological arguments of their own, and critiqued those offered by others. Future candidates should be aware that on any Oxford examination, candidates who do little more than capably summarize material fed to them in lectures or tutorials are unlikely to earn distinction-level marks. A10295W1: The Study of Old Testament Set Texts 12 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 66.67 70+ 2 60-69 10 50-59 0 40-49 0 390 The standard was quite high, ranging from 70%-63%. The average mark was 67%. Thirteen candidates were registered to sit the examination. One candidate was unwell and therefore absent. The gobbet questions were answered in a formulaic way by most of the candidates beginning with historical context and then addressing theological issues. Some candidates answered the questions in a disjointed order, attempting half of the gobbets (on Genesis) before doing section B essay one on Genesis and then returning to do the other half of the gobbets (on Amos) before returning to do the other half of section B (on Amos). Most of the candidates chose the gobbets on Cain and Abel and on the Tower of Babel for Genesis and for Amos on the formulaic introduction. Very few answered Section A (Genesis) gobbet B. In section B (Genesis) all of the questions were answered, although question 1b was not popular. Nobody attempted 2c or d. 6 The second marked was asked to look at three papers. These papers were borderline first. The marks were reconciled by taking into account issues of style, originality, knowledge of the text and level of engagement with scholars. A10301W1: The Study of a New Testament Set Text 29 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 64.86 70+ 7 60-69 18 50-59 4 40-49 0 390 In the main this paper was done reasonably well, with the great majority of scripts falling in the 60-69 range. While some individual candidates did better on some parts of the paper than others, in aggregate the average marks for the different sections of the paper show very little difference. Every short notes question, gobbet and essay question was attempted by at least one candidate, with the sole exception of question 10 (b), on source criticism. In question 1 Matthew and Pharisees were the most popular topics with Acts and Galatians the clear runners-up. The most popular essay questions by quite some margin proved to be those on the divinity of Jesus and the parable secret. Most candidates showed good command of the primary text, and quite a few were able to reference the opinions of named scholars, but what most distinguished candidates in the essay questions was their ability to marshal what they know into a cogent answer to the question asked. Too few candidates, for example, stopped to question what ‘divine’ might mean in the context of Mark or managed to address both the functioning of parabolic discourse and the theme of secrecy. Candidates who appeared simply to be trying to drown the examiner in a tsunami of verbiage were marked down accordingly, but there were mercifully few of those. The more successful gobbet answers were those that combined accurate contextualization with pertinent comment on aspects of the passage actually set. Weaker answers tended to talk too much about the material surrounding the passage rather than the passage itself; while there is a balance to be struck here, it clearly is not struck when the passage actually set is hardly mentioned at all. Very few candidates proved unable to contextualize the passages they chose to comment on, but in the few cases where this occurred the comment suffered accordingly. The strongest short notes answers were surprisingly good in relation to what students at this stage might reasonably be expected to know, whereas the weakest were either thin or muddled. More difficult to judge were some in the middle that appeared to be faithfully reproducing dubious material gleaned from some introductory text, raising the question whether in such cases the exercise becomes one of shallow draughts intoxicating the brain. 7 A10296W1: The History of the Church from Nero to Constantine 22 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 66.27 70+ 4 60-69 17 50-59 1 40-49 0 390 The scripts were generally good, though unadventurous. All questions other than Question 8 (‘Why did Donatism flourish in North Africa?’) were attempted. The best scripts showed an ability to marshal evidence from primary and secondary sources to argue for a view. More common were candidates who summarized what they had learned in lectures and tutorials with little critical analysis of their own. A10297W1: Introduction to the Study of Religions 25 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 63.92 70+ 4 60-69 19 50-59 1 40-49 1 390 25 candidates sat this paper. The most popular question was question 4 (““Rituals accomplish nothing. It is therefore pointless to study them in order to understand religions”. Discuss.”) (chosen by 20 candidates) followed by question 1 (“Do attempts to define the notion of religion have to presuppose that there is something all religions have in common?”) (11 candidates), 10 (“Describe the difference between investigating a question from a theological perspective and from the perspective of the study of religion.”) (10 candidates), and 6 (““Religion does not create social coherence. The social understanding of religion is therefore fundamentally flawed.” Discuss.”) (9 candidates). While most candidates showed adequate knowledge of the material covered, a persistent problem was the lack of close attention paid to the question. Almost all candidates answering question 6 completely ignored the question’s assumption that religions do not create social coherence and launched straight into a discussion of Durkheim. Even the better answers only tackled this question in a historical manner, usually by contrasting Durkheim with Marx. It is surprising that hardly any candidate considered this question to have any connection with current affairs. Similarly candidates answering question 4 usually ignored the assumed inefficacy of rituals, going directly into a discussion of van Gennep. There was a tendency amongst 8 candidates to off-load pre-conceived and somewhat generic essays without making any attempt to re-think matters in terms of the question asked. A second concern I have is the lack of analytical approach displayed in many of the essays. Answers almost always consisted of a paraphrase of the views of different thinkers (often in chronological order), giving little evidence of any deep engagement with the material, or the attempt to think through matters on one’s own in a systematic manner. In the future it will be necessary to alert candidates to the facts that a) dumping generic essays not really relevant to the question will not be rewarded by high marks, and b) the focus of the essay should be an account of the candidate’s thoughts on the matter informed by the knowledge acquired during the course, rather than a paraphrasing exposition of material covered in lectures and tutorials. A10298W1: New Testament Greek 27 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 78.4 70+ 20 60-69 2 50-59 2 40-49 2 391 In the main this paper was done very well, as is shown by the fact that a majority of candidates obtained a mark of 80 or more, with a third of the candidates managing 90 or more, whereas really poor scripts were rare. As ever, this mostly reflected whether or not candidates knew the material; as last year, the mark obtained on question 1 (the vocabulary list) was often a good predictor of the mark obtained overall. Many candidates once again threw away marks by not observing the rubric requirement to give the lexical form and English meaning when parsing, and there was fairly widespread carelessness over the application of breathing marks. Translations of the sentences from English into Greek also appeared to be marred by carelessness with too many elementary errors such as failing to put the subject of the sentence in the nominative, and instead merely copying τους ὀφθαλμους in the accusative from the Gospel passage in which it was found. Both the translations and the grammatical questions that followed were generally handled well, although in response to question 5(d) a surprisingly large number of candidates identified μονοι as an adverb (perhaps confusing it with μονον). There was once again a tendency for participles to be rendered with excessive literalism; it is understandable that candidates may be anxious to convince the examiner that they recognize participles when they see them, and no one was penalized just for that, but candidates occasionally lost marks where this approach distorted the meaning of what was being translated or where the translations in question 8 were unfairly criticised for being less wooden.. 9 A10299W1: Biblical Hebrew 11 candidates sat this paper. Average (mean) mark across paper: 85.00 70+ 10 60-69 1 50-59 0 40-49 0 390 Eleven candidates sat the examination. The standard was exceptionally high this year with an average of 85% and with marks ranging from 67%-96%. The second marker was asked to consider the highest and lowest scripts. Some of the scripts used the outdated KJV style, or approximations thereof (such as “thou may eatest”), to translate the Genesis text (Q1 a-c). Some problems emerged in relation to question 2 and 5 for all the students owing to a number of words and elements of grammar which were after the stated rubric of p. 123 of Weingreen, such as transliterations of well-known names such as Elijah, Elisha, Jehoshaphat, and a number of popular verbs, and a verbal suffix which occurs on p. 123. Both examiners were aware of this and candidates who did not manage to translate were not penalised. Indeed, it was pleasantly surprising that most of the candidates did not struggle with this. Question 2 also contained a printing error; the final pe ףwas not on the end of the word ‘Joseph’ (Q2. C). Most candidates guessed, but those who did not were not penalised. Question 3 also contained a printing error – the pointing was smudged. Therefore candidates were advised not to answer this question. As a result, each of the parsing questions which was answered (3 a, c, d, e) was weighed at 2.5%. Question 4 was uncomplicated and candidates did well. Overall, this paper was answered with confidence and accuracy, as is reflected by the high average and by the fact that only one student went below the first class mark. A10302W1: Sanskrit 2 candidates sat this paper. A10303W1: Qur'anic Arabic 2 candidates sat this paper. 10 Concluding Observations The Junior Pusey and Ellerton Prize in Biblical Hebrew was awarded to Eva Chapman of Trinity College (with a score of 96). The Gibbs prize for the best overall performance was awarded to Cesar Manivet of Oriel College, with an average score of 78.33. We are aware that the issue of scaling or capping the marks on language papers is already under discussion, but the Moderators would like to call the Faculty Board’s attention to how the exceptionally strong performances on language papers this year may have contributed to a relatively high number of distinctions, in particular in some cases where candidates’ other papers were not all that distinguished. It was not felt appropriate to depart from the immediately previous years’ practice (of not scaling marks) for the penultimate year of the preliminary examination in its current format. It is also recognized that the possibility of obtaining very high marks in a language paper to some extent counters the in-built disadvantage candidates in humanities subjects otherwise have compared with the marks often obtainable on science papers. On the other hand, it is clear that when it comes to distinctions and prizes, candidates for the single school, who all take a language paper, tend to have a built-in advantage over Philosophy and Theology candidates, who generally do not (39.47% Theology and Religion candidates obtained a distinctions compared with 23.08% of Philosophy and Theology candidates). While straightforward capping of language marks would be unfair on the strongest candidates (a candidate obtaining a raw mark of 97 should surely be more highly rewarded than one with a mark of 80 or 85), some kind of scaling of marks might be desirable, such that, for example, marks in excess of 40 (a bare pass) are scaled down to a maximum of 85 (this was the system that was employed some years back, although more recently it appears to have been abandoned on the basis of advice from the Proctors). The Chair once again thanks the Moderators and Assessors for all their time, support and good humour. Assessors have once again proved valuable both in setting papers that lie beyond the expertise of the Moderators (in Sanskrit and Arabic) and in providing a second opinion on borderline cases and a representative sample of scripts. Moderators: Assessors: Dr E.C.S Eve (chair) Dr A. Avramides Dr K. Southwood Prof. J. Westerhoff Dr W. Wood Dr A.Al-Akiti Prof. G. Flood Dr D. Lincicum Dr A. Teal Dr J. Williams 11