Steve Garner The Ecology of Fear: Trait-Mediated Indirect Interactions Advanced Topics in Marine Ecology, Spring 2013 Extended Abstract Indirect species interactions occur at all levels of an ecosystem, from the individual to the community. Trait-mediated indirect interactions (TMIIs) are only a single type of indirect interaction but comprise the major compliment to density mediated indirect interactions (DMIIs) and in some examples play a greater role in shaping communities. Tri-trophic interactions (i.e. between three species) result from consumer-resource interactions and subsequent chain effects. It is important to distinguish the difference between TMIIs and DMIIs as DMIIs result from a change in the number of individuals where as TMIIs refer to changes in behavior in response to a stimulus, or lack there-of. The two are certainly intertwined and often confounded in that the response behaviors exhibited by prey are intended to reduce predation (and maintain or increase density) as far as compensatory declines in energy allocation will allow (Lima and Bednekoff, 1999). Despite selective pressures for favorable traits, individuals show a great deal of phenotypic plasticity in terms of both potential and realized behavioral responses. Here I review the literature relevant to the discussion of TMIIs, provide specific examples of several behaviorally mediated trophic interactions, and discuss the comparative importance of TMIIs to ecosystems using experimental evaluations of effect size. Abrams (1995), followed later by Dill et al. (2003) and Werner and Peacor (2003), defined TMIIs in terms of a three species food chain: “a species (the “initiator” of the effect) has a direct effect on another (the “receiver” of the effect) if a change in some property of the initiator species produces a change in a property of the receiver, and this change does not require a change in any property of any other species to occur.” However, “effects are indirect if a change in some property of another species (the “transmitter”) are caused by the change in the initiator’s property and are required to produce the change in the property of the receiver species.” This somewhat cumbersome definition can be visualized in Figure 1 taken from Dill et al., (2003). For example (of many), the presence of a prey species initiates the presence of a predator species which transmits an indirect effect of avoidance to a different potential prey species receiving the effect. Dill et al. (2003) expand on TMIIs by providing some examples of various types such as “competitor facilitation”, “apparent competition”, and “behavioral resource depression”. During “competitor facilitation” the presence of a predator (initiator) affects the behavior of its prey (transmitter) and causes that same prey to be more available to a competing predator species (receiver). A great example of this type of interaction occurs when large predators (i.e. dolphins and tunas) condense bait fish and force them to the surface where they become available to birds. In the context of Abrams work in the optimal foraging arena, facilitation-type TMIIs occur when the transmitter (prey) sacrifices foraging for refuge in the presence of the initiator (predator) thereby reducing predation on the prey species (receiver) of the transmitter (Dill et al. 2003; Griffin et al., 2012). More generally, any combination of three species in which species A feeds on species B and species B feeds on species C likely exhibit some degree of TMII in response to the presence of their respective predator species (either A or B or both). Abrams (e.g. 1984, 1990, and 1991) intensively explored the importance of direct versus indirect interactions in the context of optimal foraging theory, suggesting indirect effects would have a greater impact on long-lived species with iteroparous reproductive strategies. Abrams (1984) assumed that more active foragers are more vulnerable to predation and thus, the functional response of a species is dependent upon its adaptive behavior potential. Walters et al., (1981) demonstrated the ability to condition a succession of survival responses in the gastropod Apylsia californica to non-predatory cues, showing that even prey with relatively simple nervous systems are able to develop associations with negative stimuli. Investigations by Huang and Sih (1990) further demonstrated the importance of behavioral responses in consumer-resource dynamics. In a three species, one predator food-web (green sunfish, larval salamanders, and isopods) Huang and Sih (1990) determined that the presence of isopods increased the foraging activity of green sunfish (independent of density) which in turn increased refuge use by larval salamanders; without the presence of isopods the change in larval salamander refuge use in the presence of green sunfish was non-significant. They also found evidence of sexually dimorphic interspecies interactions, which they termed “indirect antagonism,” where increased refuge use by larval salamanders displaced female isopods from refuges making them more vulnerable to predation (Huang and Sih, 1990). Male isopods were much more active and were unaffected by the presence of salamanders. Chemical cues are the predominant initiators of response in both predators and prey, with hydrodynamics playing a key role in the ability of predators to detect prey scent trails (Weissburg and Zimmer-Faust, 1993). Favorable hydrodynamics result in quicker, more successful searches while turbulence and stagnation may completely prevent prey plume detection (Figure 2; Weissburg and Zimmer-Faust, 1993). Community-level responses are thought to be heavily dependent on the densities of species and their direct interactions. For example, predators may switch to a prey source when higher densities increase capture efficiency, and thereby reducing predation on that prey’s own prey. Certainly, TMIIs and DMIIs are intertwined as predators respond to changes in abundant prey, but several studies have found effect sizes associated with TMIIs much larger than those attributable to density only (Huang and Sih, 1990; Turner and Mittlebach, 1990; Wissinger and McGrady, 1993; Werner and Peacor, 2003). Werner and Peacor (2003) cited several examples: in a three species food web (Tramea->Erythmia->damselfly) the removal of the feeding apparatus of top predator dragonfly larvae (Tramea) resulted in a 61% decrease in predation upon damselflies by Erythemis, the primary prey of Tramea (Wissinger and McGrady, 1993), where as only a 21% decrease was observed without manipulation (density only effect). As mentioned above, Huang and Sih (1990) found TMIIs associated with sunfish, salamander larvae and isopods, with the effect size of the TMII 2.4 times greater than that attributable to density effects alone. Behavioral variability can disrupt seemingly simple experimental manipulations of short, linear food webs, preventing scientists from differentiating controlling factors from confounding effects. However, understanding behavioral interactions between predators, competitors, and prey helps clarify the functional role of various species in food webs rather than dismissing behavior as problematic uncertainty. The literature reviewed here clarifies the role of indirect interactions using sound experiments and emphasizes the importance of behavioral plasticity in shaping communities at the individual, genetic, and species levels. Literature Cited Abrams, P.A. 1984. Foraging time optimization and interactions in food webs. The American Naturalist. 124(1): 80-96 Abrams, P.A. 1990. The effects of adaptive behavior on the type-2 functional response. Ecology. 71(3): 877-885 Abrams, P.A. 1991. Strengths of indirect effects generated by optimal foraging. Oikos. 62(2): 167-176 Abrams, P.A. 1992. Predators that benefit prey and prey that harm predators: unusual effects of interacting foraging adaptations. The American Naturalist. 140(4): 573-600 Abrams, P.A. 1995. Implications of dynamically variable traits for identifying, classifying and measuring direct and indirect effects in ecological communities. The American Naturalist. 146: 112-134 Dill, L.M., M.R. Heithaus, C.J. Walters. 2003. Behaviorally mediated indirect interactions in marine communities and their conservation implications. Ecology. 84(5): 1151-1157 Griffin, B.D., B.J. Toscano, J. Gatto. The role of individual behavior type in mediating indirect interactions. 93(8): 1935-1943 Huang, C. and A. Sih. 1990. Experimental studies on behaviourally mediated, indirect interactions through a shared predator. Ecology. 71(4): 1515-1522 Lima, S.L. and P.A. Bednekoff. 1999. Temporal variation in danger drives antipredator behavior: the predation risk allocation hypothesis. The American Naturalist. 153(6): 649659 Soluk, D.A. and N.C. Collins. 1988. Synergistic interactions between fish and stoneflies: facilitation and interference among stream predators. Oikos. 52: 94-100 Turner, A.M. and G.G. Mittlebach. 1990. Predator avoidance and community structure: interactions among piscivores, planktivores, and plankton. Ecology. 71: 2241-2254 Walters, E.T., T.J. Carew, E.R. Kandel. 1981. Associative learning in Apylsia: evidence for conditioned fear in an invertebrate. Science. 211(4481): 504-506 Weissburg, M.J. and R.K. Zimmer-Faust. 1993. Life and death in moving fluids: hydrodynamic effects on chemosensory-mediated predation. Ecology. 74(5): 1428-1443 Werner, E.E. and S.D. Peacor. 2003. A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology. 84(5): 1083-1100 Wissinger, S. and J. McGrady. 1993. Intraguild predation and competition between larval dragonflies: direct and indirect effects of shared prey. Ecology. 74: 207-218 Figures Figure 1. Illustration of the response of the “receiver” (bottlenose dolphins) to the presence of the “transmitter” (tiger shark) whose presence is in response to the “initiator” (dugong) exceeding some threshold density in seagrass habitat. The “receiver” responds by reducing foraging in shallow habitats despite high food availability in favor of predator avoidance (Dill et al., 2003). Figure 2. Interactive effects of olfaction and hydrodynamics in mediating predation success. (A) Predatory success and search path efficiency as a function of flow speed. (B) Initial locomotory trajectories of unsuccessful searchers, and of crabs n the absence of prey, as a function of flow speed. The highest success rate in intercepting a plume occurs at low flow (1 cm/s).