PKU6 VALUES and WORLDVIEWS Easternizing in Recent W Thought COMPARISON TEXT: “Easternizing” in Recent Western Thought Introduction: Since its first modern encounter with the West, beginning with the Portuguese in the early 1500s, China has undergone a long and intricate dance of fascination and resistance in relation to the West. In its most recent phase, “Westernizing” in China, though extensive, may still amount to only a relatively superficial manifestation of cultural borrowing and assimilation. How can this be? After all, visitors to China today see skyscrapers that outrival most modern Western cities; restaurants run by McDonald’s, Starbucks, and KFC plus the latest model German cars – and traffic jams to match. The answer is: some such changes are culturally significant, most are not. If Westernizing in China only shows up in McDonald’s fast food or Mercedes cars, it must be understood as highly visible – but not necessarily indicative of important new directions. In that superficial sense, China was very visible in 18th-century European ceramics and decorative arts collectively identified as chinoiserie. European taste for exotic imports allowed China to export an impressive quantity of goods, but its impact on Western cultures was neither enduring nor significant. From a comparative perspective, however, it is possible today to point to something quite new: not a convergence of taste but a growing convergence between longstanding cultural assumptions. From this perspective, we can say that conceptual distances between China and the West appear, over recent decades, to be diminishing. But the reasons behind this phenomenon probably have little to do with Western imports of Chinese artifacts or practices, such as traditional Chinese medicine or the use in Western business schools of Sunzi’s book of stratagems. Rather, challenges arising from within Western premises themselves have opened the door to fresh insights. These often bear startling resemblances to ideas traditionally prevailing in China. So far, these developments are not perceived in the West as Chinese in origin. Perhaps that is just as well, since such a perception might encourage a reflex rejection of “foreign influence.” Instead, these new perspectives result from one of the strengths of the Western intellectual tradition: an insistent self-critical capacity that over time may call any assumption into question. At present the net result is an emerging convergence of Western with long-standing Chinese views of the world. In time these may encourage collaborative action – desperately needed in response to the emerging ecological crisis, the most pressing problem facing the world for this and generations to come [link: p 653]. To keep in mind as you read: If these research results represent a Western trend, why has it not been recognized and publicized? Text: Compared to the mass importation of ideas and practices from the West to China, those flowing the other way seem at times random and scattered. There are many reasons for this, perhaps most powerful being the self-satisfaction of the West, built up during its centuries of world domination. During that time, only a few in the West recognized non-Western worlds as possessing valuable civilizations. Ironically, this very Western hegemony was itself enabled by the four great inventions originating in China: paper and printing, gunpowder, and the maritime compass [link: text]. But, until recently, their Chinese origins were either unknown or else simply ignored. In our own time, one can name practices imported from China that function in a similar way: enriching Western life but not transforming it. One might cite the vogue of taiji and other martial arts or for traditional Chinese medicine, particularly acupuncture and to a lesser extent, qigong. TCM may have more impact over time; but so far it has largely been associated with preventive medicine, a idea new to most Westerners (and their insurance companies). More important kinds of cultural interpenetration involve the way people think. In Encounter 6.2 we have already glanced at two Western bridge persons who were unusually open to Chinese phenomena: the Swiss psychiatrist Carl Gustav Jung (18751961) and Joseph Needham (Li Yuese, 李约瑟, 1900-1995). During the 20th century, Jung and Needham were explicitly working on interfaces between the Chinese and Western traditions. But now more recent developments in the West may prove to be even more influential, partly because they make no direct mention of China. Among the many possible examples of recent Western thinking which seems to confirm ancient Chinese wisdom, is that of the Higgs Boson/Field research that confirms energy as fundamental of everything that exists [link: p 424]). In the following sections, we introduce three other areas of overlapping perspectives, each drawn from three widely distant fields of inquiry: social psychology, cognitive linguistics, and the philosophy of values. The fact that similar modes of thinking show up in such disparate endeavors suggests an emerging broader movement, not yet perceived as a trend. “The West Is WEIRD” The larger perspective implied by these overlaps in thinking might well be summarized in the phrase advanced by three social scientists in Canada’s University of British Columbia in Vancouver: “The West is weird.”” Writing in 2010, they argue that “behavioral scientists routinely publish broad claims about human psychology and behavior in the world’s top journals based on samples drawn entirely from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic [WEIRD] societies.” [Joseph Henrich, Steven J. Heine, Ara Norenzayan, “The Weirdest People in the World?” Behavioral and Brain Sciences (2010), 33: 61-135.] Studies by Western social scientists, they claim, “often implicitly assume that either there is little variation across human populations, or that these ‘standard subjects’ are as representative of the species as any other population.” Are such assumptions justified? Not at all. Their review of comparative databases from across the behavioral sciences suggests that there is in fact substantial variability in experimental results across different populations. More significantly, the authors conclude that WEIRD subjects are unusual compared with the rest of mankind – in fact, they are “frequent outliers.” In such diverse domains as visual perception, fairness, cooperation, spatial reasoning, categorization and inferential induction, moral reasoning, reasoning styles, self-concepts and related motivations, these authors find that “members of WEIRD societies, including young children, are among the least representative populations one could find for generalizing about humans.” What these authors are explicitly challenging is the ethnocentric belief that Western assumptions about psychology, motivation, and behavior may be used to generalize about the behavior of all humans. They argue that Westerners are, in fact, the exotic ones. But, being rich and powerful, Westerners have tended to regard their assumptions as “universal” – and thus to dominate the discourse about how other people think and live. Here is yet another reason why Westerners need to disengage themselves from any automatic privileging of their ways of thinking about the world. In other words, Westerners need to change their perspective: not only in the social sciences but throughout all those disciplines through which intelligent observers seek to understand and manage their lives. Social Psychology Take, for instance, a recent series of experiments carried out by Hajo Adams and Adam Galinsky that demonstrate the impact on thinking of what one wears [“Enclothed Cognition,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48 (July 2012), 918-925]. In a first experiment, 58 undergraduates were randomly assigned to wear a white lab coat or street clothes. Then they were given a test for selective attention; that is, their ability to maintain focus on a primary task while screening out potentially distracting secondary factors. Result: those who wore the white lab coats made about half as many errors on trials as those who wore ordinary clothes. In the second experiment, 74 students were randomly assigned to one of three options: wearing a “doctor’s coat,” wearing an “artistic painter’s coat,” or simply seeing a “doctor’s coat.” Then they were given a test for sustained attention. Those who wore what they were told was the “doctor’s coat” – which was in fact identical to the painter’s coat – had acquired heightened attention, spotting more differences between superficially identical pictures. Those who wore the painter’s coat or were primed with merely seeing the “doctor’s coat” found fewer discrepancies between the images. Implication: for a doctor’s cost to influence your psychological processes you have to understand its symbolic significance – and also to see it on your body and feel it on your skin. Conclusion: our minds are literally invested in what we wear. Clothes invade the body and brain, putting the wearer into a different psychological state. But the effect occurs only if you actually wear the coat and recognize its symbolic meaning: the implication is that physicians tend to be careful, rigorous, and good at paying attention. These experiments were conceived as part of a growing scientific specialization called embodied cognition. According to the authors of this study, there is a huge body of work in this field, demonstrating such varied correlations as how the experience of washing your hands is associated with moral purity; how people are rated personally warmer if they hold a hot drink in their hand, and colder if they hold an iced drink – or how carrying a heavy clipboard will make you feel more important. In other words, our thought processes are based on physical experiences as affected by associated abstract concepts. And those experiences include the clothes we wear. If these observations can be confirmed with non-Western subjects, it will show that we humans think not just with our brains but with our bodies and our social stereotypes as well. As Daniel Kahneman demonstrated in Thinking Fast and Slow (2011), humans very often make up their minds paying little attention to what they should know rationally. What is also important in these experiments is the new emphasis on not merely what we think but how we think. Such an emphasis would come as no surprise in a Chinese context because xin [link: p 437] has long described the human heart-mind as rooted in intuitive, social responses. The Chinese view is well articulated by comparative philosopher Wu Kuang-ming in On Chinese Body Thinking: A Cultural Hermeneutic (Leiden: Brill, 1997). Second-Generation Cognitive Linguistics Cognitive linguistics propose a fresh grounding for our words and ideas. In their focus on words and word usage these concerns are linguistic, but cognitive in their concern for how humans know and how they express what they know. George Lakoff (Professor of Linguistics at the University of California, Berkeley) and Mark Johnson (Professor of Philosophy at the University of Oregon) in their Philosophy in the Flesh (1999) argue that our ideas of things come not from direct connection with something outside us, as naïve realism would like to believe. Nor are they grounded in something a priori in our minds, as Immanuel Kant once posited. Instead our bodily experience of moving around in the world, a process that starts with birth if not before, gives rise to words and our understanding of them. What adults call space is something that all children discover early on, as they experience moving around on some sort of horizontal plane. Even before they have words to express that fact, they learn that things fall down unless something holds them up. Such experiences allow them to think of things in terms of up or down, hence to situate things on a vertical axis. Time turns out to be almost impossible to conceive except in terms of something moving through space. Time is required for something to move from one position to another. Once a person has grasped the notion of time, it becomes possible to imagine it as frozen when something does not move. In short, Lakoff and Johnson claim that all our more complex ideas are composed out of such basic experiences of the physical world. These are extended or combined by metaphorical extension; that is, by projecting or mapping familiar characteristics onto unfamiliar domains in order to make sense of them. For example, all children experience what it feels like to stand straight and walk tall. Later on, this bodily experience will allow them to think of what it might mean to be “morally upstanding.” The larger significance of this view of language is spelled out by Mark Johnson in a more recent book entitled The Meaning of the Body: Aesthetics of Human Understanding (2007). In this view truth is a largely irrelevant notion in human terms, because it can only apply to a limited range of propositions of a type that has been overprivileged in Western thought, especially in Anglo-American analytical philosophy. What is more important, says Johnson, is meaning, a much larger and messier concept, arrived at by extrapolating bodily experience into ever new domains. To make something meaningful is, in Johnson’s terms, to connect it with one’s past experiences and one’s ongoing concerns. In that way, things will make sense because one will have established connections between different aspects of one’s life experience. Meaning, then, is always individual and personal, though everyone will share many meanings with other members of one’s home culture (and also some meanings with all other humans). If we accept the embodied-experience origins of human understanding, there are several consequences, none of which will seem surprising to individuals acculturated as Chinese. One is that objectivity must be abandoned as a goal and as a criterion for useful knowledge. In addition to the basic experiences shared by all children, each child grows up in a cultural environment that will privilege certain metaphorical transfers of meaning over others. In addition, there is no inherent limit to the number or the complexity of the metaphors that humans may generate. Creativity remains possible in each person’s evolving experience. In every culture there are individuals generating new connections and extensions, writers and other artists prominent among them. Even journalists and advertisers add new connections, some of which will survive to become part of standard usage. Values: Against Fairness Similar reorientations of Western thinking apply in our third domain of overlap: thinking about values. In 2012 Stephen Asma, who teaches philosophy and Buddhism in Chicago, published an exemplary reassessment of “equality” as a Western value in Against Fairness [University of Chicago Press]. Here Asma, seeking scientific confirmation for what constitutes the most widespread of human impulses, pursues ethical matters into far-flung disciplines usually reserved for specialists. In doing so, Asma is willing to challenge central Western orthodoxies – even those values regarded as crucial to civil society by most of his audience, regardless of their political loyalties. Asma’s basic assertions show up, in yet another context, how exceptionally the West, through its privileging of Enlightenment concepts like “equality,” has become “WEIRD.” Equality is not promoted worldwide. Most of the world has a myriad ways of playing favorites. Most people routinely expect to defend family against all comers. When possible, nepotism is deployed: one expects and respects the inequalities involved in hierarchies. Asma grounds these widespread practices as biological, citing recent research on how bonding affects newborns, not just in various animal species but in humans themselves. We humans are hardwired, he argues, to privilege intimate family members and to build out from there as our lives become more complex, a view most Chinese readers would find not merely congenial but “natural.” In the West, while Enlightenment ideology enjoins us to treat everyone equally, our deeper human instincts keep disrupting such egalitarian high principles. By testing the utilitarian ethics familiar to us since the time of Jeremy Bentham [link: text], Asma replays thought experiments that demonstrate just how ambivalent most Westerners remain on issues such as equality. If, for example, in a crisis situation switching a streetcar to another track would result in fewer deaths than not doing so, most people think they would flip it. But if the only person to be sacrificed were in some sense precious, reliance on a mere utilitarian calculus of numbers would begin to lose its potency. For Asma, this research shows how we humans instinctively favor those closer to us over those who seem more distant. In a direct confrontation, favoritism typically trumps egalitarianism. In taking on Western beliefs that presently enjoy iconic status, Asma shows great intellectual courage. As, for example, when he writes: “Christianity tells us, ‘God so loved the world that he sacrificed his only son for us.’ It may be pious and mystically beautiful to sacrifice your son for others, but it’s also transcendentally bad parenting.” The studied irreverence here is not gratuitous. Asma is here deliberately taking aim at the transcendental perspective so crucial to seminal thinkers such as Plato and to Westerners ever since. In doing so, Asma tests the divine family as if it were any other family. He asks, How good is the care that a family gives to the child it brings into the world? Does consciously sacrificing that precious child make for good models of parenting – or for any other intimate human transaction? The goal of this clearly contrarian argument is clearly not to remake Westerners as Chinese. Asma takes a wider view, aiming to show the degree to which egalitarian ideology in the West, particularly in the USA, needs to be contested as inconsistent with a wider and deeper understanding of humanity. As everyone in Western society knows from experience, equality of opportunity can never guarantee equality of outcomes. Hierarchies exist in all enterprises, although their existence may, by many Westerners, be simply denied. Acknowledging such realities can free Westerners from all kinds of contemporary hypocrisies and ineffective social and political policies. Freed from such ideologies, we humans can then get on with the really challenging business of learning how to live together [link: ecological crisis, p 648]. Although Asma does not invoke Asian social models, what is identifiably Chinese here involves his style of reasoning. Time and again, Asma moves away from abstract principles to a grounding in the details of actual situations: a pragmatic impulse that helps us become conscious of just how complex and often contradictory our own Western values may become when we try to put them into practice. By stepping out of certifiably WEIRD values into a larger world perspective, Asma’s study reinforces the commonality of all humans. These tend to belong, as he points out, not to the West, but to the Rest. Thus in a guanxi culture, so typical in Asia, all relations are deemed personal, as embedded in specific situations. Asian children learn this early; in China, as we have seen, most Chinese parents start early and strongly to enjoin obedience and respect [link: xiao, p 146]. Comparative Perspectives on “Easternizing” From these examples it is possible to say that Western thought – out of its own internal self-critiques – is in the process of generating ways of looking at things that tend in some instances to run remarkably parallel to long-established Chinese views. In the other direction, “Westernizing” as perceived in China tends to be highly visible. In China public awareness of “Westernizing” encourages both debate and nationalistic resistance against what seems to some people an excess of imported ideas and practices. On the other side, the “Easternizing” of the West is more subtle and indirect – if only because it takes place outside a public consciousness of cultural importation. Since this is a trend neither widely perceived nor debated, it might in the long run have even more impact, precisely because such views do not activate reflexes of resistance. It is to be hoped that, insofar as the new perspectives gain credibility, the West will become more open to abandoning its self-righteous assumptions that its own civilizational constructs apply universally. Perceiving that the West is WEIRD is a necessary step in that direction. A further step would involve an enhanced recognition of the legitimacy and worth of nonWestern civilizations, particularly that of China. Of course, all cultural convictions are contingent on the civilizations which generate and support them. In the end, there is no ground which allows one civilization to claim inherent superiority as compared to others. Each is the right civilization for its own people, given their origins and trajectory through time. Hence, the appropriate ground of interaction between two disparate civilizations is mutual recognition and respect, leading to negotiation and compromise – just as the Chinese tradition has long affirmed [link: Zhao Tingyang, p 631]. The implications for our time of ecological crisis provide the focus of the following document. Study questions: 1. What difference does it make whether imported ideas are identified as “from foreign origins” or not? 2. Given that all cultures lean toward ethnocentrism, is it realistic to hope that the West will treat Chinese civilization more respectfully as time goes on? 3. Will economic hard times tend to bring the two civilizations closer together or to push them further apart? What about ecological issues? 4. How crucial is learning about another civilization in granting it respect? Is there a possibility that it would lead only to further self-congratulation as to the values of one’s own world? Give examples to support your argument. Excerpt from Comparing Civilizations: China and the West (Shanghai: Fudan University Press, not yet in print), Encounter 6.2