BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Differences in brain coativity in prospective memory associated with varying APOE allele combinations and age: An event-related fMRI experiment. Candidate Number: 87533 Supervisor: Jennifer Rusted Word Count: 5, 956 (inc. footnotes) Psychology BSc School of Psychology, University of Sussex May 2013 Acknowledgements This study was funded by a BBSRC grant to Jennifer Rusted (BB/H000518/1) who supervised this project. Thanks to Torsten Ruest for the data collection and Simon Evans for study design and image preprocessing. The writer of this report was responsible for the PPI analysis – with guidance from Simon Evans – and completion of this manuscript. 1 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Abstract The apolipoprotein (APOE) ε4 allele is theorised to cause accelerated cognitive aging because of its antagonistic pleiotropic properties: the allele correlates with increased cognitive performance in young adulthood and earlier onset of AD in older adulthood. To better understand how APOE affects cognitive aging, the current study investigated differences in brain coactivation as a function of genotype (ε4+s vs. ε4-s) and age (middle age vs. younger adult) in addition to trial-type (ongoing trials vs. prospective memory (PM) trials). The study primarily focused on middle-aged participants who completed a computerised-PM task whilst in the fMRI scanner. Secondarily, a younger cohort was added to examine the genotype-specific effects of age on coactivation. The current study predicted there would be an increase in bilateral frontal coactivation as a function of age in ε4+s based on theories of normal cognitive aging and the antagonistic pleiotropic theory. The most significant finding was the genotype-specific effect of age: increased bilateral coactivation with the lInfFrontal (seed) region for middle-aged ε4+s compared to younger ε4+s when masked by the contrast, middle-aged ε4-s > younger ε4-s. For future research, it would be interesting to conduct a longitudinal study that focused on coactivation differences as a function of age, genotype and AD-conversion to better understand how coactivation differences predict AD. Keywords: APOE ε4, antagonistic pleiotropy, prospective memory, coactivation 2 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 1. Introduction 1.1 APOE ε4, The Brain and Alzheimer’s Disease Since the early 1980s—when beta-amyloid (Aβ) plaques and neurofibrillary tangles were first linked to the Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) pathology— there has been an ongoing debate regarding what causes AD (Selkoe, 2011). The conversation continues because no genetic factor, biomarker or environmental factor has been unequivocally linked to the cause of AD. In the mid-1990s, it was discovered that harbouring at least one apolipoprotein (APOE) ε4 allele, located on chromosome 19, increased the risk of AD and negatively correlated with the age of onset (Selkoe, 2011). In support of this discovery, Corder et al. (1993) found that 64% of patients within a sporadic AD sample and 80% within a familial sample had at least one ε4 allele, whilst only 14% of the general population have the allele (Bertram, McQueen, Mullin, Blacker, & Tanzi, 2007). Therefore, though harbouring the ε4 allele is not the direct cause of AD, the allele’s presence is correlated with AD1. The ε4 allele’s overrepresentation in the AD population has correlated with the presence of amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques (Fillipinni, 2011; Mann, 1991), neuronal atrophy (Buttini et al., 1999), and a loss of cortical choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity in key areas of the brain (Poirier et al., 1995). In terms of ε4’s relation to Aβ plaques, Aβ deposition is one of the first visible signs of the pathological process of AD, and APOE ε4 is known to inhibit the normal clearing process of Aβ plaques in the brain (Fillipinni, 2011; Mann, 1991). In addition, Buttini et al. (1999) discovered that mice harbouring at least one ε4 allele experienced increased dendritic and synaptic loss with age (i.e., increased cerebral atrophy). There are three types of APOE alleles: ε2, ε3, and ε4. Whilst ε4 is overrepresented in the AD population, ε2 is underrepresented, ie., ε2 may protect against the onset of AD. 1 3 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 1.2. APOE ε4 as an Antagonistic Pleiotropic Curiously, on average, younger adults with at least one ε4 allele have higher IQ scores and achieve higher levels of education than their ε3 and ε2 counterparts (Hubacek et al., 2001; Yu, Lin, Chen, Hong, & Tsai, 2000). More specifically, young ε4+s (participants with at least one ε4 allele) have been found to have a domain-specific advantage on frontal tasks (decision making, prospective memory performance, and verbal fluency) (Marchant, King, Tabet & Rusted, 2010). In turn, APOE ε4 is considered antagonistic pleiotropic (i.e., a gene which has different effects on evolutionary fitness at different ages): APOE ε4 is correlated with increased cognitive performance in younger adults and decreased cognitive performance in older adults (Marchant et al.,2010). Marchant et al. (2010) speculated that younger ε4+s’ increased cognitive performance caused accelerated cognitive aging/earlier age-related decline in older ε4+s’, which may be an additional explanation for the allele’s overrepresentation in the AD population. The antagonistic pleiotropy theory is supported by the metabolic function of APOE ε4 discussed earlier: high Aβ levels correlate with increased neuronal and synaptic activity in younger adults and reduced functional brain connectivity in healthy older adults (Wei, 2010). 1.3. The Effects of APOE ε4 on Cognition and Cognitive Aging Today, because APOE ε4 is the second leading risk factor for developing AD, after age, there has been motivation to determine how ε4 affects specific cognitive processes affected by AD and cognitive aging (i.e., prospective memory, visual attention, and memory encoding) throughout the lifespan (Rocchi, Pellegrini, Siciliano and Murri, 2003). Determining how the ε4 allele affects cognitive processes throughout the lifespan will help researchers more accurately understand how the ε4 allele facilitates cognitive aging and the development of AD in old age. 4 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 The ε4 allele appears to impede visual attention in middle-age. Greenwood, Sunderland, Friz and Prasuraman (2000) discovered that the effect of cue-validity (i.e., the cost of having an invalid cue) on reaction time is greatest for middle-age ε4+ participants compared to ε4-s (participants with homozygous ε3). Thus, the researchers concluded that the possession of an ε4 allele in middle-age is associated with changes in attention processing similar to that of someone with mild-AD (Greenwood et al., 2000). In terms of how ε4 affects memory (i.e., encoding) with age, Filippini (2011) discovered that aging was associated with decreased brain activity in ε4+s and increased brain activity in ε4-s. Furthermore, the over-activity of brain function initially found in young ε4+s was found to be disproportionately reduced with age even before the onset of measurable memory impairment (Filippini, 2011; Mondadori et al., 2006). A caveat in Filippini’s (2011) study is the broader age-range in the “older adults” group (50-78: 28 years) compared to the “younger adults” group (20-35: 15 years). Thus, in light of current agerelated theories of compensation and dedifferentiation (to be discussed in the next section), it is possible that that the large “older adult” age-range diluted a point of increased activation in ε4+s that occurred earlier on in their lifespan (i.e., middle-age) than non-carriers (Marchant et al., 2010). 1.4. Theories of Normal Cognitive Aging The three most prominent theories of normal cognitive aging include the PosteriorAnterior Shift in Aging model (PASA), the dedifferentiation model of cognitive aging, and the Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults model (HAROLD). These all involve forms of increased brain activation with age as compensation for neuronal loss and/or an inability to recruit specialised brain regions (Cabeza, 2001; Davis, Dennis, Daselaar, Fleck & Cabeza, 2008; Park & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). 5 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 1.4.1. The Posterior-Anterior Shift in Aging Model (PASA). The PASA model contends that cognitive aging is associated with a reduction in posterior activity (e.g., in the occipital lobe) and an increase in anterior activity (e.g., in the frontal lobe) as a compensatory mechanism of cognitive aging (Davis et al., 2008; Grady et al., 1994). Parker and ReuterLorenz (2009) argued that this increase in frontal activation, as a function of age, is a compensatory mechanism in response to neuronal changes caused by declining neural structure and function. 1.4.2. The Dedifferentiation Model. In opposition to PASA—and all other compensatory models— the dedifferentiation model suggests that when older adults carry out certain cognitive processes, they engage more generalized neuronal processes as a consequence of cognitive aging (Han, Bangen, & Bondi, 2009). In contrast to Han et al. (2009), Cabeza (2001) argued that compensation and dedifferentiation theories are not necessarily incompatible because dedifferentiation (combining neuronal pathways) can be thought to compensate for cognitive decline associated with cognitive aging. 1.4.3. The Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults Model (HAROLD). An example of how compensation and dedifferentiation models can be compatible is found in the HAROLD model (Cabeza, 2001). This model suggests that brain activity tends to be less lateralized in older adults compared to younger adults during memory tasks. Thus, the HAROLD effect can be explained by compensation models (i.e., bihemispheric involvement may help counteract age-related neurocognitive decline) and dedifferentiation models alike (i.e., the loss of lateralisation reflects a difficulty in recruiting specialized neural mechanisms). 6 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 1.5. Prospective Memory The current work specifically focused on how genotype and age affected prospective memory (PM)—an individual’s ability to create, rehearse and carry out an intended action— because of its relevance to everyday life and independent living (Burgess, Gonen-Yaacovi, & Volle, 2011; Burgess, Scott & Frith, 2003; Marchant et al., 2010; Rusted, Ruest & Gray 2011). In fact, a decline in PM is one of the first major complaints older adults with memory loss experience (Luo & Craik 2008). In addition, prospective memory is frontally mediated and thus useful for determining the extent to which harbouring an APOE ε4 allele accelerates age-related processes, which would be evidence for accelerated cognitive aging. 1.5.1. Prospective Memory and the Brain. Burgess et al. (2011) concluded that the rostral prefrontal cortex (rPFC), especially the lateral rostral PFC, Brodmann Area 10 (BA 10), plays a superordinate role during the many stages of PM. Thus, the researcher proposed the Gateway Hypothesis of Rostral PFC, which suggests that the main purpose of the rPFC is to control differences between attending to independent thought (inner mental life) and attending to the external world (stimulus-oriented attention). In addition, there has recently been evidence for a fronto-parietal role in PM which links into Burgess’s Gateway Hypothesis. More specifically, the fronto-parietal hypothesis suggests that parietal regions project to frontal areas to complete the more executive tasks associated with PM (Rusted et al., 2011; Simons et al., 2006). 1.5.2. Event-Based Prospective Memory (EBPM). There are two types of PM tasks that can be tested in the laboratory and that are susceptible to age-related decline: The eventbased PM task (EBPM) and the time-based PM task (TBPM) (Luo & Craik, 2008; Henry, MacLeod, Phillips &, Crawford, 2004). This study focused on the EBPM task—wherein 7 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 participants are asked to perform an action in response to an environmental cue. The specific EBPM task used in this study was embedded in a computer-based card-sort task (i.e., the ongoing task) first developed by Rusted, Sawyer, Jones, Trawley and Marchant (2009). Rusted et al. (2009) defined the task as attention-demanding and contended that PM detection engages general attention processes in addition to those under the control of the central executive system. 1.6. The Current Study The current study looked at the effect of trial-type (PM trials vs. ongoing trials), genotype (ε4+s vs. ε4-s) and age (younger adults vs. middle-age adults) on coactivation in the brain (based on the results of an fMRI analysis) whilst participants completed Rusted et al.’s (2009) card-sort task in the scanner. Differences in coactivation as a function of the independent variables were measured by a Psychophysiological Interaction (PPI) Analysis. Three seed regions were chosen which have been consistently implicated in PM: BA 10, the Left Frontal Cortex and the Right Inferior Parietal Cortex (Burgess et al., 2011; Rusted et al., 2011). The two major goals of this study were to find out (1) how differences in coactivation as a function of trial-type relate to current theories of PM and (2) how differences in coactivation as a function of genotype and genotype-specific age differences relate to current theories of cognitive aging. In terms of cognitive aging theories, the results of our study will help us to better understand how harboring an ε4 allele links to theories of normal cognitive aging and the development of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and/or mild AD. In contrast to Fillipinni (2011), this study focuses on middle-age (50-65) because it is concerned with neuronal changes as a function of age and genotype that specifically precede the onset of age-related 8 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 memory decline. Additionally, the middle-age cohort is an under-researched group compared to older adults. Previous research has already suggested that for certain memory tasks, middle-aged adults with an APOE ε4 allele demonstrated increased bilateral activation compared to noncarriers (Bondi et al., 2008; Bondi et al., 2006). This supports the HAROLD model and Marchant et al.’s (2010) contention that harbouring an ε4 allele is related to accelerated cognitive-aging (Cabeza, 2001). 1.6.1 Aims and Predictions. The primary aim of the current research was to look at the effect of trial-type and genotype on brain coactivity as measured by the Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) response – an indirect measure of brain activity— in the middleaged cohort and secondarily to include the younger cohort in order to examine genotypespecific age differences. Data from the younger adults have also been analysed elsewhere. Based on past research, this study predicted that there would be differences in coactivation as a function of trial-type and genotype. In terms of trial-type, we predicted that BA 10 would coactivate more with other frontal regions during the PM trials compared to the ongoing trials. In support of the fronto-parietal hypothesis, we predicted that more frontoparietal coactivation would occur during the PM trials compared to the ongoing trials. In terms of genotype, based on the PASA and HAROLD models of cognitive aging and the possibility that harbouring an ε4 allele accelerates cognitive aging, we hypothesised that there would be an increase in bilateral frontal coactivation in middle-aged ε4+s compared to middle-aged ε4-s. In addition, this study predicted that there would be coactivation differences as a function of genotype-specific age differences. More specifically, we predicted that the increase in bilateral frontal coactivation as a function of age would be more significant in 9 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 ε4+s compared to ε4-s because of ε4s role as an antagonistic pleiotropic. 2. Methods 2.1 Participants 98 “younger” adults and 93 “middle-age” adults were recruited via SONA (The University of Sussex’s online participant-recruitment service) or local adverts. Before participating, all participants provided written consent in accordance with the ethics procedures set out by the University of Sussex Schools of Psychology and the Life Sciences Research Ethics Committee. All volunteers were informed that they could withdraw from the study at any point and that all data would be kept anonymous and confidential. Volunteers were excluded on the basis of untreated high blood pressure, cardiac pathology, a history of psychiatric or neurological illness, pregnancy and presence of metallic implants including bridges or braces, or tattoos above the shoulder – making them unsuitable for the fMRI scanner. In addition, participants were excluded on the basis of smoking behaviour; this is because the final two sessions (out of three) of the study involved receiving a nicotine or placebo nasal spray. The sessions involving nicotine nasal spray are not analysed here. Volunteers’ medical and psychiatric histories were assessed by self-report questionnaires. Blood pressure and BMI were measured by the experimenter prior to the first session. In order to determine participants’ APOE genotypes, cheek swab DNA-samples were collected from every participant. KBiosciences then carried out the DNA analysis and determined APOE genotypes. Participants with at least one APOE ε2 allele were excluded from the study because research has shown that harbouring the ε2 allele might improve cognition, which would therefore reduce the reliability of the results (Bloss, Delis, Salmon & Bondi, 2010). After the selection procedure, 20 middle-age adults and 20 younger adults were identified as “ε4-“ (ε3 homozygous) and 21 middle-age adults and 20 younger adults 10 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 were identified as “ε4+” (either ε4 homozygous or ε4/ ε3 heterozygous). The final sample of middle-age adults contained 18 males and 23 females between the ages of 43-57 (M = 49.95 years, SD = 4.2). The final sample of younger adults contained 13 males and 27 females between the ages of 19 and 24 (M = 20.18 years, SD = 1.88). Participants that dropped out of the study prior to completion were excluded from the analysis. The study was conducted under double blind procedures (i.e., neither the participants nor the researchers knew the genotype group allocation) – a triangulation process prevented participants and researchers from determining participant genotypes. 2.2 Experimental task The computerised PM card-sorting task was first developed by Rusted et al. (2009) as a laboratory-based measure of event-related PM made of everyday stimuli. The task was created using MATLAB and involved two parts: the ongoing task and the PM task, which is not explained to the participant until after he/she was familiarised with the ongoing task. The ongoing task involved sorting computerised images of 52 regular playing cards. Using a 4 button-box in their right hand, volunteers were instructed to press button 1 for HEART cards, button 2 for SPADE cards (‘sort’ trials), and to make no response to CLUBS or DIAMONDS (‘withhold’ trials). Volunteers were also instructed to respond as quickly as possible. Before entering the fMRI scanner, participants were given the opportunity to practice the ongoing task and are introduced to the PM task. For the PM task, participants were asked to press button 3 for any occurrences of the number 7 card independent of suit. Whilst in the scanner, participants performed the ongoing task as practiced outside of the scanner but with the additional PM instruction. During the card-sorting task, card faces were displayed for 1s, after which the card back was displayed for 2s plus a variable jitter between .1 and 1s (M = .5 s, SD = .24 s). 11 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 During each of the 3 scanning-sessions, volunteers sorted 8 decks of cards with the PM intention over a total period of about 15 minutes. Per session, there was a total of 416 trials (32 were PM trials, 192 were sort trials and 192 were withhold trials). In order to maximise the estimate-ability of each event-type, ensure a delayed onset of PM-events, and ensure minimum separation between PM-events, card order was pseudo-randomised (Friston, Phillips, Chawla, &, Buchel, 1999). This involved randomising the task within the following parameters: (1) never having a PM card occur in the first 7 cards of the entire sequence, (2) always having at least 3 intervening cards between PM events and (3) having 1 PM trial (out of 4 PM trials per deck) occur once per quarter-deck (13 cards). Accuracy and reaction times for ongoing and PM trials were recorded for another behavioural analysis (not reported here). 2.3. Design This study was a mixed-subject design. The first independent variable was trial-type (two levels; repeated-measures): PM trials and ongoing card-sort trials. The second independent variable was genotype (two levels; independent-measures): ε4-s and ε4+s. Lastly, the third independent variable was age (two levels; independent-measures): middleage adults and younger adults. The dependent variable was activation (measured by the BOLD response) that covaried with brain activity in the seed region during PM trials (compared to ongoing card-sort trials) and as a function of the other independent variables. Coactivation was measured by PPI analyses that included three main seed regions: lateral rostral PFC (BA 10), the Left Frontal Cortex (which included both the Left Superior Frontal Cortex (lSupFrontal) and the Left Inferior Frontal Cortex (lInfFrontal)) and the Right Inferior Parietal Cortex (rInfParietal). 12 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 2.4. Procedure The participants that were selected to participate in this three-session study provided informed consent in accordance with University of Sussex Schools of Psychology and the Life Sciences Research Ethics Committee. During the first session, participants completed various pen and paper tasks (e.g., the National Adult Reading Test) and provided their family histories (i.e., prevalence of dementia and mental illness) for baseline measures that will not be discussed in this paper. In addition, participants were given the opportunity to practice the computer card-sort task outside of the scanner without the PM trials. During the second and third sessions, participants were again given the opportunity to practice the card-sort task outside of the scanner without the PM trials. Then, all of the participants self-administered either a nicotinic nasal spray or placebo (NB half were given nicotinic nasal spray during session 2 and the other half during session 3). In addition to genotype, the type of nasal spray administered also followed double-blind procedures. 18-20 minutes after administering the nasal spray, participants performed the card-sort task with the PM trials inside of the fMRI scanner. Only data from the placebo sessions were analysed in this paper. It is important to note, performance and reaction time did not differ significantly between the two final sessions; thus, although half of the results discussed were from the third rather than the second session, there was no evidence of practice effects. After the final session, participants were debriefed and compensated for their time. 2.5 fMRI recording and analysis fMRI datasets sensitive to BOLD contrast were acquired at 1.5 T (Siemens Avanto). The BOLD responses acquired were from the overcompensation phase of the hemodynamic response function— the point when blood flow increases to compensate for oxygen loss after neuronal activity occurs (Ward, 2010). Thus, although the fMRI analysis provided high 13 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 spatial accuracy, when neuronal activity actually occurred was a few seconds after the BOLD. To minimise signal artefacts originating from the sinuses, axial slices were tilted 30 from inter-commissural plane. Thirty-six 3 mm slices (0.75 mm inter-slice gap) were acquired with an in-plane resolution of 3 mm × 3 mm (TR = 3300 ms per volume, TE = 50 ms). Images were pre-processed using SPM8 (Ashburner et al. 2012). Raw T2 volumes were spatially realigned and unwarped, spatially normalised to standard space and smoothed (8 mm kernel). 2.5.1 The PPI Analysis. A Psychophysiological Interaction Analysis (PPI) was used to measure changes in coactivation (as measured by the BOLD response) between a “seed region” and all other regions of the brain as a result of the study’s independent variables (O’Reilly et al., 2012). A “seed region” was defined as a 5 mm (radius) sphere centred on the coordinates of peak activation. Peak activations were taken from a previous event-related fMRI analysis of the dataset. Three seed regions were chosen that have been consistently implicated in PM: the lateral rostral PFC (BA 10), the Left Frontal Cortex (lSupFrontal and the lInfFrontal) and the Right Inferior Parietal Cortex (rInfParietal) (Burgess et al., 2011; Rusted et al., 2011). The first step of the PPI analysis involved calculating the PPI regressor. The PPI regressor is a product of the BOLD response (the physiological factor) and task condition of interest (the psychological factor; i.e., PM trials contrasted against ongoing trials) at the seed region. The regressor therefore contains information from the time-course of neuronal activity at the seed region specific to the task condition of interest. The second step of the analysis involved including the PPI regressor in a new design matrix. This design matrix also included regressors for the physiological and psychological factors. Including these factors is important so as to account for the main effects of task and physiological correlation. Thus, we could be confident that coactivation attributed to the PPI regressor was 14 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 specific to the product of the physiological and psychological factors. Only trials where correct responses were made were included in the PPI analysis. In order to determine the effect of genotype on coactivity (for middle-age adults only), genotype (ε4+s and ε4-s) was entered into a second-level full factorial model. In order to genotype-specific effect of age on coactivity, genotype (ε4+s and ε4-s) and age (middle-age and younger adults) were entered into a separate model. The majority of analyses are presented at a threshold uncorrected for family-wise error at p < .001. The exception was the computation of genotype-specific age differences: in order to identify the genotype-specific effects of cognitive aging, exclusive masks were applied at a threshold uncorrected for family-wise error at p < .05. The contrasts compared young adults and middle-aged adults for each genotype. The contrasts were subsequently masked against each other in order to identify age-related changes exclusive to each genotype. A 50-voxel threshold was also applied. Regions of interest were defined using the Wake Forest University PickAtlas. Anatomical localisation of clusters was performed using the Talairach Daemon (University of Texas, USA), and the anatomy toolbox for SPM (Eickhoff et al, 2005). 3. Results 3.1. Seed Region: BA 10 3.1.1. Average Effect of Condition in the Middle-age Cohort 15 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Figure 1. BA 10 (seed) average effect of condition indicated by increased coactivation with the right and left caudate nucleus during PM trials. Significantly increased brain coactivity was found in the right and left caudate nucleus, when BA 10 was the seed region, during PM trials compared to ongoing trials (cluster k = 506, Peak MNI: x = 12, y = 14, z = 3, p < .001 unc.). Thus, there was an average effect of condition on BA 10 coactivity. Figure 2. BA 10 (seed) average effect of condition indicated by a significant increase in coactivation with the right cuneus during PM trials. In addition, there was significantly increased brain coactivity in the right cuneus with the BA 10 region (the seed region) during PM trials compared to ongoing trials (cluster k = 109, Peak MNI: x = 13, y = 78, z = 31, p < .001 unc.). Figure 3. BA 10 (seed) average effect of condition indicated by a significant increase in coactivation with the left anterior cingulate gyrus during PM trials. 16 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 There was significantly increased brain coactivity in the left anterior cingulate with the BA 10 region (the seed region) during PM trials compared to ongoing trials (see Figure 3) (cluster k = 79, Peak MNI: x = 0, y = 32, z = 17, p < .001, unc.). 3.1.2. Main Effect Genotype in the Middle-age Cohort. Since there was no significantly increased or decreased BA 10 coactivation in ε4+s compared to ε4-s, no main effect of genotype on BA 10 coactivation was observed. 3.2. Seed Region: Left Superior Frontal Cortex (lSupFrontal) 3.2.1. Average Effect of Condition in the Middle-age Cohort Figure 4. lSupFrontal (seed) average effect of condition indicated by a significant increase in coactivation with the left postcentral gyrus during PM trials. A significant difference in brain coactivity was found in the left postcentral gyrus with the lSupFrontal region (seed region) during PM trials compared to ongoing trials (cluster k = 71, Peak MNI: x = -50, y = -16, z = 52, p < .001, unc). Thus, there was a significant average effect of condition on lSupFrontal (seed region) coactivity. 17 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 3.2.2. Main Effect of Genotype in the Middle-age Cohort Figure 5. lSupFrontal (seed) main effect of genotype indicated by a significant difference in coactivation with the right superior frontal gyrus as a function of genotype. There was a significant increase in brain coactivity in the right superior frontal gyrus with the lSupFrontal region (the seed region) for ε4+s compared to ε4-s (see Figure 5) (cluster k = 51, Peak MNI: x = 14, y = 54, z = 36, p < .001, unc.). Thus, there was a main effect of genotype on lSupFrontal coactivity. Contrast estimates at [14, 54, 36] ε4- Genotype ε4+ Figure 6. Parameter estimates for the main effect of genotype on lSupFrontal (seed) coactivation with the right superior frontal gyrus. The red bars represent 90% confidence intervals. The above contrast (Figure 6) indicates increased coactivation in the right superior frontal region with the lSupFrontal region (the seed region) for ε4+s compared to ε4-s. 18 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Figure 7. lSupFrontal (seed) main effect of genotype (ε4+s vs. ε4-s) indicated by a significant difference in coactivation with areas 3a and 4p as a function of genotype. There was a significant increase in coactivation in areas 3a and 4p with the lSupFrontal region (the seed region) for ε4+s compared to ε4-s (see Figure 7) (cluster k = 57, Peak MNI = x= 43, y= -9, z= 34, p < .001, unc.). Contrast estimates at [43, -9, 34]] ε4- ε4+ Genotype Figure 8. Parameter estimates for the main effect of genotype on lSupFrontal (seed) coactivation with areas 3a and 4p in ε4+s compared to ε4-s. The red bars represent 90% confidence intervals. The above contrast (Figure 8) indicates increased coactivation in area 3a and 4p with the lSupFrontal region (the seed region) for ε4+s compared to ε4-s . 19 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 3.3. Seed Region: Right Inferior Parietal Cortex (rInfParietal) 3.3.1. Average Effect of Condition and Main Effect of Genotype in the Middleage Cohort. No main effect of condition (PM vs. ongoing) or genotype was observed; thus, there was no significant increase or decrease in rInfParietal coactivation during the PM trials compared to the ongoing trials. Also, there was no significant increased or decreased rInfParietal coactivation in ε4+s compared to ε4-s. 3.3.2. Genotype-specific Age Difference. In addition to no main effect of genotype, there were no genotype-specific age differences. Thus, there was no significant increase or decrease in rInfParietal coactivation as a function age (middle-age vs. younger adults) for a specific genotype (ε4-s vs. ε4+s). 3.4. Seed Region: Left Inferior Frontal Cortex (lInfFrontal) 3.4.1. Age x Genotype Interaction (including the younger and middle-age cohort) Figure 9. lInfFrontal (seed) genotype-specific coactivity difference as a function of age with the right and left superior medial gyrus. Figure 9 indicates that there was a significant genotype-specific age difference in lInfFrontal (seed) coactivation with the right and left superior medial gyrus. (cluster k = 95, Peak MNI = x= 8, y= 25, z= 46, p < .001). 20 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Contrast estimates at [8, 25, 46] Young ε4- Young ε4+ Mid ε4- Mid ε4+ Age and Genotype Figure 10. Parameter estimates for the genotype-specific effect of age on lInfFrontal (seed) coactivity with the right and left superior medial gyrus. Red bars indicate 90% confidence intervals. The above contrast (Figure 10) indicates increased coactivation of the right and left superior medial gyrus with the lInfFrontal (seed) region (the seed region) for middle-aged ε4+s (“Mid ε4+”) compared to younger ε4+s (“Young ε4+”), when masked by the contrast, middle-aged ε4-s (“Mid ε4-”) > younger ε4-s (“Young ε4-”). Figure 11. lInfFrontal (seed) genotype-specific coactivity difference as a function of age with the right superior orbital gyrus. In addition, there was a significant genotype-specific age difference in lInfFrontal 21 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 (seed) coactivation with the right superior orbital gyrus (see Figure 11) (Cluster k = 56, Peak MNI = x= 25, y= 41, z= -9, p < .001, unc.). Contrast estimates at [25,41,-9] Young ε4- Young ε4+ Mid ε4- Mid ε4+ Age and Genotype Figure 12. . Parameter estimates for the genotype-specific effect of age on lInfFrontal (seed) coactivity with the right superior orbital gyrus. Red bars indicate 90% confidence intervals. The above contrast (Figure 12) indicates increased coactivation in the right superior orbital gyrus with the lInfFrontal region (the seed region) for middle-aged ε4+s (“Mid ε4+”) compared to younger ε4+s (“Young ε4+”), when masked by the contrast, middle-aged ε4-s (“Mid ε4-”) > younger ε4-s (“Young ε4-”). 4. Discussion 4.1. Summary of Findings The majority of the results supported the researcher’s predictions. More specifically, the BA 10 seed region coactivated significantly more with several frontal regions (right and left caudate nucleus, right cuneus and left anterior cingulate gyrus) for PM vs. ongoing trials. In addition, the lSupFrontal seed region coactivated significantly more with the left post central gyrus and right superior frontal gyrus during PM trials compared to ongoing trials. In 22 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 addition, there was decreased lateralisation in frontal region coactivation for ε4+s compared to ε4-s within the middle-aged cohort (i.e. coactivity with the right frontal and parietal gyri). More specifically, the lSupFrontal seed region coactivated significantly more with the rSupFrontal gyrus and areas 3a and 4p. Finally, there was a significant increase in frontal coactivity (when lInfFrontal was the seed region) with the right and left superior medial gyrus and the right superior orbital gyrus as a function of genotype-specific age differences in the middle-age ε4+ cohort compared to the younger ε4+s. The one finding that deviated from the current study’s predictions was the rInfParietal lobe as the seed region: the rInfParietal lobe did not significantly coactivate with any other brain areas as a function of trial-type, genotype or a genotype-specific effect of age. Potential explanations for this absence of brain coactivity will be discussed. 4.2 Analysis of Findings: in light of PM Theories 4.2.1. Seed Region: BA 10. Although there was no main effect of genotype when BA 10 was the seed region, there was a prominent main effect of trial-type. More specifically, there was increased BA 10 coactivity during PM trials (compared to ongoing trials) in the right and left caudate nucleus, the right cuneus and the left anterior cingulate. These results support Burgess’s Gateway Hypothesis, which suggests that BA 10 plays an executive role during PM trials. It is important to note that BA 10’s increased coactivity with the anterior cingulate, specifically, is widely supported by past PM imaging research (Burgess et al., 2011; Hashimoto, Umeda & Kojima, 2011). Hashimoto et al. (2011) found that increased anterior cingulate activation (or coactivation) during PM tasks was associated with increased attentiveness. With regards to Rusted et al.’s (2009) card-sorting task, increased vigilance is reflected in the main effect found for condition. 23 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 4.2.2. Seed Regions: lSupFrontal and rInfParietal. The increased brain coactivity as a function of trial-type in the left post central gyrus (i.e., a notable parietal region which makes up the primary somatosensory cortex) when lSupFrontal was the seed region suggests that other frontal regions, in addition to BA 10, play a central role during PM trials. In addition, the increased brain coactivity between frontal and parietal regions supports the frontal-parietal hypothesis, which contends that during a PM task, parietal regions work with frontal regions in order to carry out the more executive tasks associated with PM. The lack of increased brain coactivity as a function of trial-type when rInfParietal was the seed region does not discredit the fronto-parietal hypothesis; rather, the results suggest that frontalparietal coactivity during PM may only occur between specific frontal and parietal regions. 4.3. Analysis of Findings: in light of Cognitive aging Theories and AD 4.3.1. Seed Region: lSupFrontal. The left superior frontal cortex (lSupFrontal) was the only seed region with increased brain coactivity as a function of genotype. More specifically, there was increased coactivity with the rSupFrontal gyrus and areas 3a and 4p in ε4+s compared to ε4-s in the middle-aged cohort. This result supports Greenwood et al.’s (2000) finding that the effect of cue validity was greatest for middle-age ε4+s (compared to ε4-s)—a function of weaker executive control in middle-age ε4+s. Even though middle-age ε4+s and ε4-s performed similarly on the card-sorting task —a task that required executive control to switch attention from ongoing to PM trials— this study supports Greenwood et al.’s (2000) findings because the increased bilateral brain coactivity in middle-age ε4+s suggests that they exerted more effort to perform as well as the ε4-s. 4.3.3. Seed Region: lInfFrontal. In order to determine the genotype-specific effect of age on coactivity in the brain, a final analysis was completed that included younger adults. 24 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 The analysis concluded that brain coactivity increased as a function of age only for ε4+s. More specifically, there was increased lInfFrontal coactivation in the right and left superior medial gyrus and the right superior orbital gyrus in middle-age ε4+s vs. younger ε4+s whilst no difference between age groups was found for ε4-s. . In the context of Filippini’s (2011) study, which found that cognitive aging was associated with decreased brain activity in older ε4+s and increased activity in ε4-s, this study found a more complex effect. The contrasting findings can be attributed to the fact that Filippini (2011) included an older cohort that was comprised of middle-age and older adults whilst the current study only focused on middle-age ε4+s and ε4-s. More specifically, our findings suggest that after ε4+s period of enhanced cognition during younger adulthood and before ε4+s significant decrease in cerebral activity during older adulthood, there is a short period of increased coactivation during middle-age, which mimics the compensation and dedifferentiation processes of older non-carriers (Parker & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Han et al., 2009). In the context of the PASA model of cognitive aging, the increased coactivation in lInfFrontal—a form of compensation in response to neural structure and function—suggests earlier cognitive aging (Parker & Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). In light of the HAROLD model of cognitive aging, the decrease in lateralisation and the increase in activation clusters suggest earlier cognitive aging in ε4+s (Cabeza, 2001). In terms of AD, the decrease in lateralisation may also be a form of compensation for preclinical declines in PM (Bondi et al., 2006; Han et al., 2009). 4.3.2. Seed Region: rInfParietal. The lack of increased brain coactivity in middle-age ε4+s compared to ε4-s when rInfParietal was the seed region does not discredit the theory that the ε4+ allele causes accelerated cognitive aging. Instead, the finding can attributed to 25 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 past research that has found that atrophy in the inferior parietal lobe occurred alongside cognitive decline rather than before it (Jacobs et al., 2011). Thus, increased recruitment is more necessary in frontal regions as a form of cognitive age-related compensation before the onset of cognitive-decline. 4.4. Limitations It is important to note that our data was not corrected for family-wise error (FWE) because doing so would have eliminated all significant results. Although conservative parameters were applied (p < .001 and a 50-voxel minimum), Bennett, Wolford & Miller (2009) have contended that the legitimacy of uncorrected data (i.e., the true likelihood of false positives) cannot be determined until the results are replicated. In addition, Bennett al. (2009) noted that within the current model of publication (where for the most part, only significant results are published) false positives are not easily correctible (i.e., if a group of researchers fail to reproduce the results of a published study, the null findings would be difficult to share). 4.5. Future Research Based on past research, it is possible that the significant increase in frontal recruitment in middle-age ε4+s (compared to young ε4+s and mid ε4-s) may have been a function of a reduction in cortical choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) activity in the frontal cortex. More specifically, the possession of at least one APOE e4 allele has been linked to a reduction in ChAT activity in the hippocampus (Poirier et al., 1995). In terms of the frontal cortex and AD specifically, the loss of ChAT was recently found in the superior frontal cortex of patients with AD (Ikonomovic et al., 2007). Thus, perhaps in future research, the 26 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 relationship between a reduction in ChAT activity in frontal regions and increased brain coactivity as a function of genotype and age should be looked at. In regards to the current study’s focus on the relationship between brain coactivity, age and genotype, it would be interesting to see whether ε4+s with the greatest coactivation patterns in middle-age are more likely to experience significant clinical decline (e.g., to MCI and/or AD) when followed longitudinally. 4.6. Conclusions In terms of AD research, the most significant finding from this study was that bilateral coactivity in frontal regions increased in middle-age ε4+s compared to young ε4+s, when masked by ε4-s mids > ε4-s youngs, which suggests that the antagonistic pleiotropic ε4 allele accelerates cognitive aging. Whilst this result verifies previous studies that have found similar results, how this finding contributes to the AD literature remains unclear (Marchant et al., 2010). In order to determine how this finding would correspond to the likelihood of developing AD, researchers would need to conduct a longitudinal study that looked at coactivation patterns as a function of age, genotype and AD conversion. 27 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 5. References Ashburner, J., Barnes, G., Chen, C., Daunizeau, J., Flandin, G., Friston, K., & Phillips, C. (2012). SPM8 Manual: The FIL methods group (and honorary members). Functional imaging laboratory. Retrieved November 5, 2012, from http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/. Bennett, C.M., Wolford, G.L., & Miller, M.B. (2009). The principled control of false positive in neuroimaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 417-422. Bertram, L., McQueen, M.B., Mullin K., Blacker D., & Tanzi, R.E. (2007). Systematic metaanalyses of Alzheimer disease genetic association studies: The AlzGene database. Nature Genetics, 39(1), 17-23. Bloss, C. S., Delis, D. C., Salmon, D. P. & Bondi, M. W. (2010). APOE genotype is associated with left-handedness and visuospatial skills in children. Neurobiology of Aging, 31(5), 787-795. Bondi, M.W., Jak, A.J., Delano-Wood, L., Jacobson, M.W., Delis, D.C., & Salmon, D.P. (2008). Neuropsychological contributions to the early identification of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychological Review, 18, 73-90. Bondi, M.W., Wierenga, C.E., Stricker, J.L., Houston, W.S., Eyler, L.T., & Brown, G.G. (2006). Functional connectivity comparisons of learning by APOE genotype among nondemented older adults. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 2(3), S308-S309. Burgess, P.W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: What have we learnt so far. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2246-2257. Burgess, P.W., Scott, S.K., & Frith, C.D. (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: A lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 41, 906-918. Buttini, M., Orth, M., Bellosta, S., Askeefe, H., Pitas, R.E., Wyss-Coray, T., Mucke, L., & 28 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Mahley, R.W. (1999). Expression of human apolioprotein E3 or E4 in the brains of Apoe-/- mice: Isoform-specific effects on neurodegeneration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19(12), 4867-4880. Cabeza, R. (2001). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Contributions of functional neuroimaging. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 277-286. Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. The Psychology of Aging, 17, 85–100. Corder, E.H., Saunders, A.M., Strittmatter, W.J., Schmechel, D.E., Gaskell, P.C., Small, G.W., Roses, A.D., Haines, J.L., & Pericak-Vance, M.A. (1993). Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s Disease in late onset families. Science, 261, 921-923. Davis, S.W., Dennis, N.A., Daselaar, S.M., Fleck, M.S, & Cabeza, R. (2008). Qué PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1201-1209. Eickhoff, S., Stephan, K.E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G.R., Amunts, K., & Zilles, K. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage 25(4), 1325-1335. Filippini, N. (2011). Differential effects of the APOE genotype on brain function across the lifespan. NeuroImage, 54, 602-610. Friston, K., Phillips, J., Chawla, D., & Buchel, C. (1999). Revealing interactions among brain systems with nonlinear PCA. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 92-97. Grady C.L., Maisog J.M., Horwitz B., Ungerleider L.G., Mentis M.J., & Salerno J.A. (1994). Age related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. The Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 1450–1462. Greenwood, P.M., Sunderland, T., Friz, J.L., & Prasuraman, R.. (2000). Genetics and visual 29 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 attention: Selective deficits in healthy adult carriers of the ε4 allele of the apolipoprotein E gene. PNAS, 97(21), 11661-11666. Han, S.D., Bangen, K., J., & Bondi, M.W. (2009). Funcitonal magnetic resonance imaging of compensatory neural recruitment in aging and risk for Alzheimer’s disease: Review and recommendations. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 27, 1-10. Hashimoto, T., Umeda, S., & Kojima, S. (2011). Neural substrates of implicit cueing effect on prospective memory. NeuroImage, 54, 645-652. Henry, J.D., MacLeod, M.S., Phillips, L.H., & Crawford, J.R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. The Psychology of Aging, 19(1), 27-39. Hubacek J.A., Pitha J., Skodova Z., Adamkova V., Lanska V., & Poledne R. (2001). A possible role of apolipoprotein E polymorphism in predisposition to higher education. Neuropsychobiology,43, 200- 203. Ikonomavic, M.D., Abrahamson, E.E., Isankt, B.A., Wuu, J., Mufson, E.J., DeKovsky, S.T. (2007). Superior frontal cortex cholinergic axon density in mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 67(9), 1312-1317. Jacobs, H.I.L., Van Boxtel, M.P.J., Uylings, H.B.M., Gronenschild, E.H.B.M., Verhey, F.R., & Jolles, J. (2011). Atrophy of the parietal lobe in preclinical dementia. Brain and Cognition, 75(2), 154-163. Luo, L., & Craik, F. (2008). Aging and memory: A cognitive approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(6), 346-353. Mann, D.M.A. (1991). The topographic distribution of brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathologica, 83(1), 81-86. Marchant, N.L., King, S.L., Tabet, N., & Rusted, J.M. (2010). Positive effects of cholinergic stimulation favour young APOE ε4 carriers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 10901096. 30 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Mondadori, C.R.A., Buchmann, A., Mustovic, H., Schmidt, C.F., Boesiger, P., Nitsch, R.M., Hock, C., Streffer, J. & Henke, K. (2006). Enhanced brain activity may precede the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease by 30 years. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 129(11), 2908-2922. O’Reilly, J.X., Woolrich, M.W., Behrens, T.E.J., Smitch, S.M., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). Tools of the trade: Psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(5), 604-609. Park, D.C, & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding, The Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173-196. Poirier, J., Delisle, M, Quiron, R., Aubert, I., Farlow, M., Lahiri, D., Hui, S., Bertrand, P., Nalbantoglu, J., Gilfix, B.M., & Gauthier, S. (1995). Apolipoprotein E4 allele as a predictor of cholinergic deficits and treatment outcome in Alzheimer disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 92(26), 12260-12264. Rocchi, A., Pellegrini, S., Sciilliano, G., & Murri, L. (2003). Causative and susceptibility genes for Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Brain Research Bulletin, 61(1), 1-24. Rusted, J., Ruest, T. & Gray, M. A. (2011). Acute effects of nicotine administration during prospective memory, an event related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 49, 2362-2368. Rusted, J.M., Sawyer, R., Jones, C., Trawley, S.L., & Marchant, N.L. (2009). Positive effects of nicotine on cognition: the deployment of attention for prospective memory. Psychopharmacology, 202, 93-102. Selkoe, D.J. (2011). Resolving controversies on the path to Alzheimer’s therapeutics. Nature Medicine, 17, 1060-1065. Simons, J. S., Scholvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Differential components of prospective memory? Evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 44(8), 1388–1397. 31 BRAIN COACTIVITY: PROSPECTIVE MEMORY, APOE AND AGE Candidate no. 87533 Ward, J. (2010). The student’s guide to cognitive neuroscience. Hove: Psychology Press. Wei, W. (2010). Amyloid beta from axons and dendrites reduces local spine number and plasticity. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 190-196. Yu Y.W., Lin C.H., Chen S.P., Hong C.J., & Tsai S.J. (2000). Intelligence and event-related potentials for young female human volunteer apolipoprotein E ε4 and non-ε4 carriers. Neuroscience Letters, 294, 179–81. 6. Appendix I Ethical approval screen-shot: 32