Received: 26 August 2019 | Revised: 1 July 2020 | Accepted: 2 July 2020 DOI: 10.1002/ab.21916 RESEARCH ARTICLE Twin study of laboratory‐induced aggression Bojana M. Dinić1 | Snežana Smederevac1 | Selka Sadiković1 | Milan Oljača1 | Nataša Vučinić2 | Mechthild Prinz3 | Zoran Budimlija4 1 Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia 2 Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Medicine, University of Novi Sad, Novi Sad, Serbia 3 Department of Science, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York City, New York 4 Department of Neurology, NYU School of Medicine, New York City, New York Abstract The aim of this study was to explore genetic and environmental contributions to laboratory‐induced aggressive behavior. On a sample of 478 adult twins (316 monozygotic), the Competitive Reaction Time Task was used for aggression induction. The results showed that the initial, basic level of aggression could be explained by both shared (45%) and nonshared environmental factors (55%), while only nonshared environmental factors (100%) had a significant influence on changes in aggression as provocation increased. Genetic factors had no influence on laboratory‐ Correspondence Bojana M. Dinić, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Novi Sad, Dr Zorana Đinđića 2, 21000 Novi Sad, Serbia. Email: bojana.dinic@ff.uns.ac.rs Funding information Ministry of Education, Science and Technological Development of the Republic of Serbia, Grant/Award Number: ON179006 induced aggression. The results highlight the importance of environmental factors in shaping situation‐specific aggressive responses to provocation. KEYWORDS Competitive Reaction Time Task, laboratory aggression, twin study 1 | INTRODUCTION each other in reaction time and received “punishments” that represented situational provocation. Thus, aggression is induced by si- Aggression is a complex phenomenon that could refer to different tuational provocation, which increases during the procedure. The types of behavior, with many variations across forms and functions (e.g., procedure allows for the measurement of different aspects of ag- Bushman & Bartholow, 2010). While behavioral manifestations of ag- gression, such as the initial, basic level (intercept) of aggression and gression may take different forms, such as physical or verbal, the changes (slope) of aggression due to increasing provocation. Conse- functions of aggression indicate the motivation for aggressive behavior, quently, the behavior induced in the CRTT should correspond to re- which allows for the distinction between proactive and reactive active aggression. Previous results have shown that the CRTT is a valid aggressive patterns. Reactive aggression is a response to a real or procedure for the assessment of aggressive behavior (e.g., Chester & perceived provocation or threat, aimed to harm another person, while Lasko, 2018). Moreover, despite various measures of aggression in proactive aggression is instrumental, aimed to achieve other goals, such research using the CRTT and different CRTT analytic approaches, as money, social status, or justice (e.g., Bushman & Bartholow, 2010). reasonably similar empirical aggressive profiles have been consistently Although aggression can be seen as a stable individual difference confirmed (Hyatt, Chester, Zeichner, & Miller, 2019). with a high level of cross‐situational consistency (Tremblay & An increasing number of studies have attempted to explain the Belchevski, 2004), research has also shown that aggressive behavior is causes of aggression by examining its biological roots or genetic basis highly situation‐specific (Campbell, Bibel, & Muncer, 1985). (e.g., Veroude et al., 2016). Twin studies have offered an insight into Several paradigms have been developed to examine laboratory‐ heritability of the examined characteristics, comparing the covaria- induced, situation‐specific aggression (e.g., McCarthy & Elson, 2018). tion between monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins (see Plomin, In this study, we used the Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRTT). Chipuer, & Neiderhiser, 1994). MZ twins share 100% of their genetic The CRTT is an umbrella term for many procedures based on Taylor's material, while the percentage of genetic material shared by DZ (1967) methodology (see Warburton & Bushman, 2019). In this pro- twins amounts to 50%. In a twin design, two aspects of environ- cedure, participants were led to believe that they competed against mental influences could be distinguished—shared and nonshared Aggressive Behavior. 2020;1–9. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/ab © 2020 Wiley Periodicals LLC | 1 2 | DINIĆ ET AL. environmental influences. Both MZ and DZ twins share 100% of their aggression (e.g., verbal and indirect assault, verbal and indirect hos- shared environmental factors. Shared environmental factors refer to tility) can mostly be explained by nonshared environmental influ- experiences that are the same for all family members and make ences (e.g., Coccaro, Bergeman, Kavoussi, & Seroczynski, 1997; Dinić, members of the same family who live together similar to each other. Nikolašević, Oljača, & Bugarski Ignjatović, 2018; Seroczynski, Those factors refer not only to family‐related characteristics such as Bergeman, & Coccaro, 1999; Sluyter et al., 2000; Vernon, McCarthy, the socioeconomic status, common social rituals, or coping strategies, Johnson, Jang, & Harris, 1999). Nevertheless, some research has but also to characteristics outside the family, such as neighborhood shown higher nonshared environmental contribution (62%) on the characteristics (e.g., housing density), school‐related characteristics physical aggression factor (including indicators of fighting and phy- (e.g., academic track), or any other feature of the environment to sical assault), compared to genetic contribution (38%), while also which family members are jointly exposed. indicating a different pattern of heritability for physical aggression By contrast, all variance within twin pairs that is not explained and the general aggression factor (which includes indicators of by genetic and shared environmental factors is attributed both to temper tantrums, verbal, and indirect aggression, see Yeh, Coccaro, & nonshared environmental factors and measurement error (Plomin, Jacobson, 2010). Nonshared environmental effects have also been DeFries, Craig, & McGuffin, 2003). Nonshared environment refers higher in reactive than in proactive aggression (e.g., Baker, Raine, Liu, to individual experiences and perceptions of events that contribute & Jacobson, 2008). However, the stability of reactive aggression can to differences between family members who live together. Just like be attributed to genetic and nonshared environmental factors, while shared, nonshared environmental factors encompass experiences the stability of proactive aggression can primarily be attributed to either within or outside the family. Thus, twins may be treated the genetic factors (Tuvblad, Raine, Zheng, & Baker, 2009). In other differently by their parents or they could belong to different peer words, although previous findings indicate a strong genetic influence groups, which, in turn, may be related to differences in twins' be- on aggressive behavior, the environment has a crucial role in learning havior. It is important to note that if twins in a pair perceived the and the manifestation of aggression. same events differently and were affected by these events in dif- Twin studies of adult aggression have usually been designed as ferent ways, the influence would then be attributed to nonshared self‐assessment of relatively stable behaviors and tendencies. environmental effects. For example, the family's socioeconomic However, the pattern of genetic and environmental influences on a status could be perceived as average from the standpoint of one specific aggressive reaction, induced in a particular situation, may twin in the pair, but below‐average from the standpoint of the be different. To the best of our knowledge, there has been only one second twin in the pair. In sum, shared environmental factors are all twin study to date in which aggression was experimentally induced factors that make twins in a pair more alike, whereas nonshared and measured (Achterberg, Duijvenvoorde, Meulen, Bakermans‐ environmental factors are all influences that make twins different Kranenburg, & Crone, 2018). In this study on 7‐ to 9‐year‐old twins, from each other. results showed that nonshared environmental factors accounted Meta‐analyses of twin studies have shown that heritability of for the largest part of the variance in aggression after negative aggression among children and adolescents up to 18 years of age is social feedback (74–90%), while genetic and shared environmental 65% (Burt, 2009) or around 50% when considering a wider age factors were quite low (up to 20%, see Achterberg et al., 2018). range (Miles & Carey, 1997). In the twin studies review by Other studies including laboratory‐induced behaviors (e.g., risk Veroude et al. (2016), it was shown that heritability of aggression taking, cooperative behavior) have shown that nonshared en- ranged from 39 to 60% for children between ages 2 and 6 and from vironmental factors constitute a much more important source of 46 to 60% for children between ages 6 and 14, while in adults, it phenotypic variation than genetic factors, while shared environ- was difficult to compare the results, but overall heritability was mental factors have nonsignificant or very low contributions (e.g., slightly lower and around 30%. Possible reasons for the wide Cesarini, Dawes, Johannesson, Lichtenstein, & Wallace, 2009; variation in heritability among studies are the use of different Cesarini et al., 2008). measures of aggression, different research designs (e.g., parent/ The objective of this study was to determine genetic and en- teacher report, observation, and self‐report), and different sample vironmental effects on situationally provoked aggressive responses. characteristics and size. However, all previous results point to the Aggression was induced by employing the CRTT procedure. Previous fact that the environment is important for shaping aggressive studies have shown robust effects of genetic and environmental behavior. While a nonshared environment has had an impact on factors on aggression (e.g., Tuvbald & Baker, 2011). However, the aggression in every study, the contribution of the shared en- fact that they were mainly based on self‐assessment questionnaires vironment has been less common (see Tuvbald & Baker, 2011), could contribute to the neglect of the role of situational character- detected mostly on samples of children and adolescents (e.g., istics. In line with the results from previous twin studies (e.g., Tuvbald Veroude et al., 2016). & Baker, 2011) and of laboratory‐induced behavior (e.g., Achterberg Similar results have been obtained in twin studies of specific et al., 2018), the assumption was that genetic and environmental forms of aggression conducted on adult samples. For example, phy- influences would have significant contributions in shaping a specific sical aggression and direct assault can be explained by both genetic aggressive response in a provocative situation. Since this is the first and nonshared environmental influences, while other forms of study to combine twin and experimental design in examining adult DINIĆ | ET AL. aggressive behavior, predictions based on the specifics of the used 3 2.3 | Competitive Reaction Time Task procedure were necessary. Namely, in the CRTT procedure, opponents have usually been Aggression was induced through the CRTT in which twins were led to unfamiliar. In this study, twins competed against each other. It is believe they were competing with each other in the reaction time in possible that the interpersonal dynamic between twins could con- visual perception tasks in separate rooms. Before each trial in the tribute to their expectations based on previous shared experiences. CRTT, each twin had an opportunity to set the “punishment” for his/ Previous studies have shown greater closeness, familiarity (Segal, her twin pair. The punishment comprised of the settled intensity (on a Hershberger, & Arad, 2003), and cooperation between MZ twins scale from 0 = no punishment, 1 = 60 db to 10 = 105 db, as in Bertsch, compared to DZ twins (Segal & Hershberger, 1999), supporting as- Böhnke, Kruk, Richter, & Naumann, 2011) and duration of an aversive sociations between genetic relatedness and prosocial behavior. To noise (on a scale from 1 = 0.5 s to 10 = 5 s). After each twin settled the test whether CRTT aggression has different patterns in MZ and DZ punishment, they started competing in the reaction time task. The trial twins compared to participants who compete with an unknown op- consisted of a square that could appear in one of the five positions on ponent, we included a control group of nontwins from the study by the screen (left, right, up, down, and center) with a different inter- Dinić and Smederevac (2019) where the same procedure was used. stimulus interval for each trial. The task of each twin was to press the bar as quickly as possible when the square appeared on the screen. The following two outcomes were possible for each twin: win (faster 2 | METHOD reaction time than the other twin) and lose (slower reaction time than the other twin). Then, the slower twin would receive the punishment 2.1 | Participants and data collection determined by the faster, winning twin. Before the experiment, each twin heard the minimum and the maximum punishment noises to gain The Serbian Twin Registry includes data on 1,654 participants. The insight into the range of punishments. Thus, twins did not receive procedure for collecting data from twins is described elsewhere information about the exact punishment setting of their opponent, but (Smederevac et al., 2019). Participants in this study were 316 MZ and only the punishment itself. There were four blocks in the procedure 162 DZ twins raised together (478 in total; see Table 1). Data col- (each contained 10 trials), with the first block designed as practice. In lection was mostly done at the faculty premises in the period from this block, twins only administered the punishment and did not receive 2011 to 2018. Participation was voluntary and each participant it if they lost. In blocks 2 through 4, twins received predetermined signed an informed consent form before the research session. The punishments, which increased during the procedure: in the second study was approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee. The ap- block, Mintensity = 70 db (60–75) and Mduration = 0.75 s (0.5–1); in the proval certificate can be found at the following link: http:// third block, Mintensity = 85 db (80–90) and Mduration = 2 s (1.5–3); and in psihologija.ff.uns.ac.rs/etika/?odobreno=20111020000004_e1b8. the fourth block, Mintensity = 100 db (95–105) and Mduration = 4.2 s To determine a possible difference in the aggressive response (3.5–5), based on the study by Bertsch et al. (2011). In each block, between twins and respondents competing with unknown same‐sex twins randomly won in 50% of the trials. The most common measures opponent, the sample from the study by Dinić and Smederevac of aggression—intensity and duration of determined punishments— (2019) was used as a control group. In this study, the same CRTT were used. Thus, it was possible to test a biometric latent growth procedure was used. This sample included 60 university students model (LGM) to explore genetic and environmental factors on both the (71.7% females), the majority of whom were in the second year of basic level of aggression (intercept) and changes, that is, the increase studies (about 19–20 years old). or decrease of aggression during the trials as the provocation increased (slope). After the CRTT, participants were debriefed to check whether they believed the experimental manipulation and to explain 2.2 | Zygosity the purpose of this procedure. One twin did not believe the cover story. Thus, the data for this twin pair were excluded from the sample Zygosity was determined by a DNA analysis of the buccal swab. DNA description and analysis. In the case of the control group, out of the was extracted using standard DNA extraction techniques (QIAgen®, total sample used in Dinić and Smederevac (2019), three participants Hilden, Germany) and zygosity was determined using the Global Filer did not believe the experimental manipulation and their data were kit (Applied Biosystems; Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA). excluded from the sample description and analysis. T A B L E 1 Twin sample characteristics Sex Age Twins Total Male Female Different sex Age range M SD Monozygotic 316 76 (24.1%) 240 (75.9%) – 16–58 24.89 7.64 Dizygotic 162 62 (38.3%) 100 (61.7%) 76 (46.9%) 16–48 22.89 5.92 Abbreviations: M, mean; SD, standard deviation. 4 | 2.4 | Data analysis DINIĆ ET AL. covariate, but results were the same, thus we kept the analysis without the age effect. Additionally, differences between same‐sex First, descriptive statistics and univariate cross‐twin correlations (both and opposite‐sex pairs were tested compared the following groups: Pearson's and intraclass coefficients of correlation [ICC] with corre- (a) MZ twins, (b) DZ same‐sex, (c) DZ opposite‐sex, and (d) control sponding 95% confidence interval [CI]) were calculated for measures group with the same‐sex unknown opponent. For posthoc compar- of aggression. The ICCs were calculated using a random‐effects one‐ isons, the Bonferroni test was used. Effect size was calculated as ηp2, way analysis of variance (ICC 1.1; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979), by the for- which should be up to 0.06 for small, 0.07–0.14 for medium, and mula ICC = (MSbetween − MSwithin)/(MSbetween + MSwithin), in which the >0.14 for large effects (Cohen, 1988). expected values of the within‐pair mean‐square (MSwithin) and the Fourth, the LGM (McArdle & Hamagami, 2003) was applied to between‐pair mean‐square (MSbetween) in MZ were compared to those estimate genetic and environmental influences of the initial, basic in DZ twin pairs. A higher and positive ICC value indicates higher level of aggression (intercept) and the rate of aggression change concordance of variables in the twin pair. For multiple Pearson's during the four blocks of the CRTT procedure (slope). An LGM is correlation coefficients, Bonferroni correction of p‐value was used. presented in Figure 1. The top part of the model represents the Second, the effect of manipulation in the CRTT procedure was standard biometric model. The total variation of the phenotype could tested by the combined repeated measures general linear model be explained by additive genetic variance (A), shared environmental (GLM). Since previous studies have shown sex (e.g., Archer, 2004) variance (C), and nonshared environmental variance (E). A reflects and age effects (e.g., Piquero, Carriaga, Diamond, Kazemian, & the component of the total genetic variance that can be explained by Farrington, 2012) on aggression, these effects were tested to de- genes within genotypes, C defines common environmental effects for termine which characteristics should be controlled, that is, partialized both twins, whereas E reflects individually specific factors of en- out from aggression for the biometric models. Thus, sex was entered vironmental and individual experiences for each individual and as a between‐subject factor, age as a covariate, and the block of the measurement error. Four models were tested—full ACE and reduced CRTT procedure as a within‐subject factor or repeated measure, AE, CE, and E models. An important specificity of the biometric model while aggression in the CRTT procedure was a dependent variable. is fixing the values of certain parameters. Parameter A was fixed at Third, since twins competed with each other, to explore the 1.00 for MZ, since they shared 100% of the genes, while this para- possible differences in aggression between twins and participants meter was fixed at 0.50 for DZ, since they shared about 50% of their who compete with unknown opponents, the GLM was also applied to genes on average. Parameter C was fixed at 1.00 in both MZ and DZ, group membership (MZ twins, DZ twins, control group—competition due to the assumption that twins shared 100% of the shared en- with an unknown opponent) and sex as between‐subject factors, the vironmental variance. Path coefficients for the intercept were fixed block of the CRTT as a within‐subject factor, and aggression in the at 1 because the intercept is constant across time and it is assumed CRTT as a dependent variable. Preliminary, age was entered as a to be the baseline level of certain trait/state, independent of changes F I G U R E 1 A biometric latent growth model. Note: Ac = common additive genetic factor, Acs = unique additive genetic factor, Ec = common nonshared environmental factor, Ecs = unique nonshared environmental factor, Cc = common shared environmental variance, Ccs = unique shared environmental variance, λ1, λ2 = freely estimated path coefficients, e1–8 = errors DINIĆ | ET AL. 5 due to experimental procedure or time (McArdle, 2006). The slope 0.87, rblock2 = 0.91, rblock3 = 0.91, and rblock4 = 0.91 for dizygotic twin path coefficient was fixed at 0 for the first block (as the first block is pairs), composite scores (average scores) combining intensity and basically the baseline level of the trait or the phenomenon of inter- duration were used in further analysis, as in previous studies (e.g., est) and was fixed at 1 for the last block, while for the second and the Dinić & Smederevac, 2019). Descriptives for the average aggression third blocks it was freely estimated. This allowed for the monitoring scores across blocks for MZ and DZ twins are presented in Table 2. of the shape of the change through the blocks of the CRTT. Model fit was evaluated by using several fit indices: the quotient χ2/df, which should be lower than 2, the comparative fit index, the Tucker–Lewis index, which should be higher than 0.90, and the root mean square 3.2 | Manipulation check, sex, and age effects on laboratory‐induced aggression error of approximation, which should be lower than 0.08. Comparisons between nonnested models were tested via the change in the The results of the combined repeated measures GLM showed that Akaike information criterion (AIC), with lower values indicating the block of the CRTT procedure had a significant effect on aggres- better fit (e.g., Kline, 2010). Statistical analyses were performed in sion (F[3,465] = 7.25, p < .001; ηp2 = 0.045), as well as the interaction Amos 23 by using the maximum likelihood estimator and in SPSS 24. between block and sex (F[3,465] = 2.68, p = .046; ηp2 = 0.017), while The data that support the findings on this study are openly available the interaction between block and age was not significant in Open Science Framework (OSF) at https://osf.io/j4vqx/. (F[3,465] = 1–71, p = .164; ηp2 = 0.011). In the case of main effect of block, posthoc Bonferroni tests showed that there were significant differences in aggression between the fourth block and the rest of 3 | RESULTS the blocks (all p < .001), as well as between the third block and the rest of the blocks (all p < .001), while the only nonsignificant differ- 3.1 | Descriptive statistics and univariate cross‐ twin correlations ence was between the first and the second block (p = .35; see Table SA). Thus, there was increment of aggresssion from the second to the fourth block of the CRTT procedure (see Table 2 and Figure 2). Univariate cross‐twin correlations in aggression measures between Regarding interaction effect between the block and sex, posthoc DZ twins were almost equal or slightly higher compared to MZ twins Bonferroni tests showed that although males had higher aggression, (Table 2). Because correlations among punishment intensity and the increment of aggression among females was higher from the duration for the same blocks were very high (rblock1 = 0.86, rblock2 = second through the fourth block (see Table SB and Figure SA). Thus, 0.84, rblock3 = 0.86, and rblock4 = 0.91 for monozygotic and rblock1 = the effect of sex was partialized in biometric LGM. T A B L E 2 Descriptive statistics and univariate cross‐twin correlations for laboratory‐induced aggression Monozygotic twins Dizygotic twins Control group Aggression Block M SD r ICC (95% CI) M SD r ICC (95% CI) M SD Punishment intensity 1 4.22 2.93 .41 0.41 (0.27–0.53) 4.61 2.55 .37 0.37 (0.16–0.54) 3.77 2.42 2 4.10 2.94 .39 0.38 (0.24–0.51) 4.80 2.91 .37 0.37 (0.16–0.54) 3.56 2.55 3 4.32 2.95 .37 0.36 (0.21–0.49) 5.19 3.03 .51 0.51 (0.33–0.65) 3.98 2.83 4 4.66 3.08 .40 0.38 (0.24–0.51) 5.46 3.00 .46 0.46 (0.27–0.62) 4.51 3.07 1 3.76 2.77 .33 0.30 (0.16–0.44) 4.15 2.54 .36 0.35 (0.14–0.53) 3.43 2.04 2 3.60 2.62 .35 0.28 (0.14–0.42) 4.28 2.81 .41 0.38 (0.18–0.55) 3.29 2.24 3 4.02 2.79 .41 0.36 (0.22–0.49) 4.71 2.86 .47 0.44 (0.25–0.60) 3.78 2.61 4 4.54 2.98 .41 0.38 (0.24–0.51) 5.21 2.85 .49 0.48 (0.29–63) 4.53 2.88 1 4.13 2.65 .41 0.40 (0.26–0.52) 4.51 2.57 .41 0.42 (0.22–0.58) 3.60 2.13 2 4.08 2.72 .40 0.37 (0.23–0.50) 4.72 2.87 .44 0.44 (0.25–0.60) 3.43 2.30 3 4.43 2.82 .43 0.40 (0.26–0.52) 5.14 3.00 .53 0.52 (0.35–0.67) 3.88 2.63 4 4.85 2.94 .44 0.42 (0.28–0.54) 5.47 3.00 .50 0.50 (0.32–0.65) 4.53 2.92 Punishment duration Average scores for intensity and duration of punishments Note: Control group – participants who competed with an unknown same‐sex person. Descriptives for the control group were calculated from the data used in Dinić and Smederevac (2019). All Pearson's correlations were statistically significant at p < .001 after applying the Bonferroni correction for p‐value (for 24 Pearson's correlation coefficients, with default critical p‐value set on .05, the corrected critical value was p = .00208). Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; ICC, intraclass coefficient of correlation; M, mean; SD, standard deviation. 6 | DINIĆ ET AL. indicated that females from DZ opposite‐sex twin pairs showed somewhat higher aggression scores compared to females from the control group (p = .065; see Figure SB), while the same was not observed in males.1 Thus, it seems that the differences between DZ twins and the control group are due to the differences in aggression between females from opposite‐sex DZ twin pairs and females from the control group. 3.4 | Biometric latent growth model of laboratory‐induced aggression Although all proposed biometric models showed good model fit, according to the AIC, the most optimal was the CE model (Table 3). In F I G U R E 2 Laboratory‐induced aggression across blocks of the CRTT procedure among monozygotic twins, dizygotic twins, and control group (bars represent 95% CI of the standard errors of mean) this model, nonshared environment effects were stronger for the intercept (baseline) level (β = −0.74, p < .001; E = 0.55) than the effects of shared environment (β = 0.67, p < .001; C = 0.45). The slope variance, or the variance of change, was fully explained by the nonshared environment effects (β = 1.00, p = .051; E = 1.00). 3.3 | Comparison between twins and nontwins in laboratory‐induced aggression 4 | D IS C U S S I O N First, the three groups of participants (MZ twins, DZ twins, and the control group) were compared in aggression, including sex as the The objective of this twin study was to determine genetic and en- second between‐subject factor. The results of the combined repeated vironmental effects on laboratory‐induced aggression. First, a ma- measures GLM showed that group membership had a marginally nipulation check of the used CRTT procedure for aggression significant small effect on aggression (F[2,532] = 2.88, p = .057; ηp2 = induction showed that increasing provocation results in a stronger 0.011), while none of the interactions including group membership aggressive response, which is in line with previous findings (e.g., were significant (all p > .05; see Table SC). Bonferroni posthoc tests Bettencourt & Miller, 1996; Chester & Lasko, 2018; Weidler showed that a significant difference was obtained only between DZ et al., 2019). Such “tit for tat” strategy evolves as a common reaction twins and the control group (mean difference = 1.08; p = .021), with within the CRTT paradigm (e.g., Lindsay & Anderson, 2000) and DZ twins obtaining higher scores on aggression (see Figure 2). There corresponds to reactive aggression, shaped by specific situational were no significant differences in aggression between MZ and DZ triggers or provocation. Although the CRTT paradigm could en- twins (p = .326), nor between MZ twins and controls (p = .219). compass several levels of provocation, such as no‐provocation, con- To further test this effect, the distinction between same‐sex and stant low‐provocation, or increasing provocation, which corresponds opposite‐sex DZ twins was also included in the second GLM analysis. to the high‐provocation level (see Dinić & Smederevac, 2019), only Thus, differences between four groups were tested. Again, sex was the high‐provocation level was applied in this study. This fact is im- entered as a between‐subject factor. The results did not show a portant since previous results have only shown an increase in ag- significant effect of group membership (F[3,530] = 2.32, p = .074; gression in high‐provocation conditions (e.g., Dinić & Smederevac, ηp2 = 0.013) nor the rest of the interactions with group membership 2019), implying that such an aggressive response is influenced by the (all p > .05; see Table SC). strength of the provocation, not its repetition. Furthermore, con- However, since we were interested in differences between DZ sistent with the results of previous studies, sex had a significant twins and the control group, we tested the differences between three effect on laboratory‐induced aggression, since men provided longer groups—same‐sex DZ twins, opposite‐sex DZ twins, and the control and more intense punishments (e.g. Zeichner, Parrott, & Frey, 2003). group. Sex was entered as a between‐subject factor, as in previous Thus, to control the effect of sex, sex was partialized out from the analyses. The results showed a significant small effect of group aggression scores in the main biometric analysis. membership (F[2,216] = 3.56, p = .030; ηp2 = 0.032), while the rest of The most important result was that laboratory‐induced aggres- the interactions with group memberships were not significant (all sion was best explained by both shared and nonshared environ- p > .05, see Table SC). Posthoc Bonferroni tests showed that there mental factors. This result is in line with a previous longitudinal twin were significant differences only between opposite‐sex DZ twins and study that showed that environmental factors are important for the controls (p = .012), with opposite‐sex DZ twins showing higher aggression scores (see Table SD). Additional posthoc Bonferroni tests when only opposite‐sex DZ twins and controls were included 1 In an additional analysis, a differentiation between female and male MZ pairs was also included, but there were no significant differences in group membership. DINIĆ | ET AL. T A B L E 3 Fit indices for the tested biometric latent growth models and for the intercept and slope of laboratory‐induced aggression Model χ ACE 939.40 2 df χ /df CFI TLI RMSEA AIC 68 13.82 0.65 0.71 0.23 979.40 2 7 opposite‐sex DZ twin pairs obtaining higher scores. Since there are no differences between females from same‐sex DZ and opposite‐sex DZ twin pairs, this result could indicate a specific interpersonal dynamic between opposite‐sex DZ twins. A previous study on school‐ aged children showed that opposite‐sex DZ pairs had a direct influ- AE 158.83 70 2.27 0.97 0.97 0.07 194.83 CE 153.81 70 2.20 0.97 0.97 0.07 189.81 et al., 2020). In this study, a higher level of aggression in the male E 206.31 72 2.87 0.95 0.96 0.09 238.31 twin resulted in less aggression in the female co‐twin, but females Note: All χ2 tests were significant at p < .001. Abbreviations: AIC, Akaike information criterion; CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; TLI, Tucker–Lewis index. ence on each other that differed from same‐sex pairs (Luningham with higher aggression tended to interact with their male co‐twin by promoting higher aggression, although heritability of aggression was consistent for males and females. In another study likewise conducted on school‐aged children, it was shown that females from opposite‐sex DZ twin pairs reached the male level in hyperactivity‐ stability of reactive aggression, in contrast to proactive aggression impulsivity and aggression and they were more aggressive compared (Tuvblad et al., 2009). Although the higher contribution of environ- to singletons (Pulkkinen, Vaalamo, Hietala, Kaprio, & Rose, 2003). mental factors was expected, the lack of genetic influence was not in Furthermore, they showed higher leadership compared to other line with predictions. A possible explanation is that the CRTT pro- twins and singletons. cedure induces aggression in a highly contextual situation, which Thus, we could conclude that the higher aggression in females leads to significant contributions of environmental influences only. In from opposite‐sex DZ twin pairs was rather the result of a specific other words, it is possible that specific reactions to provocation are social learning context and experiences from the interaction with generally determined by environmental factors and that the triggers male co‐twins, which provided opportunities for shaping, imitating, for the appearance of these reactions are in the environment. A and practicing various behavioral patterns related to both typical previous study exploring laboratory‐induced aggression in children female and typical male behaviors. This result also indicates the need showed that nonshared environmental factors had a high contribu- for future exploration of the impact of relationship quality and dy- tion (74–90%), with a small proportion of genetic (10–20%) and namic between the twins on aggressive behavior. shared environmental factors (0–8%; see Achterberg et al., 2018). There are several limitations to this study that prevent a reliable Moreover, the results showed that shared and nonshared en- generalization of the results. First, females seemed to be over- vironmental factors almost equally contributed to the explanation of represented in the study sample. Since aggression is a phenomenon the initial level of aggression, but the change of aggression under that contributes to significant sex differences, it is possible that the provocation depended solely on nonshared environmental factors. It results were influenced by sex‐specific biases, despite controlling for is possible that the initial level of aggression is more influenced by the effect of sex. Second, it remains unclear whether the results were the learned patterns developed in a shared, family environment, influenced by specific expectations of the situation due to the ac- while a strong provocation causes reactions determined by concrete quaintance between participants, sibling closeness, or twin‐specific circumstances and idiosyncratic experience. interaction. In future studies of laboratory‐induced aggression, it The specificity of the research design of this study suggests the would be particularly important to vary the degree of acquaintance need for alternative explanations of the results. It is possible that the between opponents to determine the possible impact of prior ex- interpersonal dynamic between the twins and the previous experi- perience on the characteristics of an aggressive response. Third, re- ence of possible competition and mutual comparison contributed to sults in this study are inconsistent with previous studies that have their expectations and reactions in this specific situation. Namely, in shown a significant proportion of genetic factors to specific behavior twin studies of laboratory‐induced cooperative or risk‐taking beha- responses (Achterberg et al., 2018; Cesarini et al., 2008), leaving the vior, nonshared environmental factors have been the most important possibility that it could be due to the used CRTT procedure in which source of phenotypic variation (Cesarini et al., 2008, 2009), implying twins competed with each other or due to the small sample size. the possibility that the specific interpersonal dynamics between ac- Thus, future research would need to explore aggression in a CRTT quaintances can significantly define the nature of reactions. procedure in which twins would compete against unknown persons. The results showed that MZ and DZ twins did not differ in ag- Finally, while the sample size could be considered as appropriate for gression, despite the hypothesis about the connection between ge- the experimental design, it could be considered as moderate for netic relatedness and prosocial behavior (Segal & Hershberger, multivariate biometric models (e.g., Verhulst, 2017). A larger sample 1999). However, there was a significant difference between DZ twins could certainly provide the basis for further exploration of the po- and members of the control group who competed with unknown tential moderation effect of sex, some important environmental same‐sex opponents, with DZ obtaining higher aggression scores. variables, or stable individual differences. Additional analyses showed that those differences probably stem Despite these limitations, the results highlight the importance of from the differences between females from opposite‐sex DZ twin both shared and nonshared environmental factors in the explanation pairs and females from the control group, with females from of aggressive responses in provocative situations. In other words, 8 | DINIĆ while there is compelling evidence to suggest the genetic basis of aggressive behavior (Burt, 2009; Veroude et al., 2016), it is possible that there are highly provocative situations that elicit aggressive responses fully determined by contextual factors. A C K N O W L E D GM E N T S The authors thank Prof. Rainer Reiman for suggestions regarding statistical analysis, Bojan Branovački for maintenance of the data matrix, and volunteers in the Centre for Behavioral Genetics for help in collecting the data. This study was supported by the Ministry of Education, Science and Technological Development of the Republic of Serbia (Grant ON179006). CO NFLICT OF I NTERE STS The authors declare that there are no conflict of interests. D A T A A V A I L A B I LI TY S TA T E ME N T The data that support the findings on this study are openly available in the OSF at https://osf.io/j4vqx/. OR CID Bojana M. Dinić http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5492-2188 REFERENC ES Achterberg, M., Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K. V., Meulen, M. V. D., Bakermans‐ Kranenburg, M. J., & Crone, E. A. (2018). Heritability of aggression following social evaluation in middle childhood: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 39(7), 2828–2841. https://doi.org/10.1002/ hbm.24043 Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in aggresion in real‐world settings: A meta‐analytic review. Review of General Psychology, 8(4), 291–322. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291 Baker, L. A., Raine, A., Liu, J., & Jacobson, K. C. (2008). Differential genetic and environmental influences on reactive and proactive aggression in children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1265–1278. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9249-1 Bertsch, K., Böhnke, R., Kruk, M. R., Richter, S., & Naumann, E. (2011). Exogenous cortisol facilitates responses to social threat under high provocation. Hormones and Behavior, 59, 428–434. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.yhbeh.2010.12.010 Bettencourt, B. A., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: A meta‐analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 422–447. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.422 Burt, S. A. (2009). Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta‐analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(2), 163–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2008.12.004 Bushman, B. J., & Bartholow, B. D. (2010). Aggression. In R. F. Baumeister & E. J. Finkel (Eds.), Advanced social psychology: The state of the science (pp. 303–340). New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press. Campbell, A., Bibel, D., & Muncer, S. (1985). Predicting our own aggression: Person, subculture or situation? British Journal of Social Psychology, 24(3), 169–180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309. 1985.tb00678.x Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Fowler, J. H., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Wallace, B. (2008). Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(10), 3721–3726. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0710069105 Cesarini, D., Dawes, C. T., Johannesson, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Wallace, B. (2009). Experimental game theory and behavior genetics. Annals of the ET AL. New York Academy of Sciences, 1167(1), 66–75. https://doi.org/10. 1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04505.x Chester, D. S., & Lasko, E. N. (2018). Validating a standardized approach to the Taylor Aggression Paradigm. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(5), 620–631. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550618775408 Coccaro, E. F., Bergeman, C. S., Kavoussi, R. J., & Seroczynski, A. D. (1997). Heritability of aggression and irritability: A twin study of the Buss‐ Durkee aggression scales in adult male subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 41(3), 273–284. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00257-0 Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Dinić, M. B., Nikolašević, Ž., Oljača, M., & Bugarski Ignjatović, V. (2018). Twin study of aggressiveness and impulsiveness relationship. Primenjena psihologija, 11(4), 451–470. https://doi.org/10.19090/pp. 2018.4.451-470 Dinić, M. B., & Smederevac, S. (2019). Effects of HEXACO traits and experimental provocation on aggression. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 227(3), 225–229. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000381 Hyatt, C. S., Chester, D. S., Zeichner, A., & Miller, J. D. (2019). Analytic flexibility in laboratory aggression paradigms: Relations with personality traits vary (slightly) by operationalization of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 45(4), 377–388. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab. 21830 Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Lindsay, J. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2000). From antecedent conditions to violent actions: A general affective aggression model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(5), 533–547. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 0146167200267002 Luningham, J. M., Hendriks, A., Kraphol, E., Plomin, R., Kaprio, J., Bartels, M., … Lubke, G. H. (2020). Harmonizing behavioral outcomes across studies, raters, and countries: Application to the genetic analysis of aggression in the ACTION Consortium. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13188 McArdle, J. J. (2006). Latent curve analyses of longitudinal twin data using a mixed‐effects biometric approach. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(3), 343–359. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.9.3.343 McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (2013). Structural equation models for evaluating dynamic concepts within longitudinal twin analyses. Behavior Genetics, 33, 137–159. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:10225539018 McCarthy, R. J., & Elson, M. (2018). A conceptual review of lab‐based aggression paradigms. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 4. https://doi.org/10. 1525/collabra.104 Miles, D. R., & Carey, G. (1997). Genetic and environmental architecture on human aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 207–217. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.1.207 Piquero, A. R., Carriaga, M. L., Diamond, B., Kazemian, L., & Farrington, D. P. (2012). Stability in aggression revisited. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17(4), 365–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2012.04.001 Plomin, R., Chipuer, H. M., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (1994). Behavioral genetic evidence for the importance of nonshared environment. In E. M. Hetherington, D. Reiss & R. Plomin (Eds.), Separate social worlds of siblings: The impact of nonshared environment on development (pp. 1–31). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Plomin, R. E., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. E. (2003). Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10480-000 Pulkkinen, L., Vaalamo, I., Hietala, R., Kaprio, J., & Rose, R. J. (2003). Peer reports of adaptive behavior in twins and singletons: Is twinship a risk or an advantage? Twin Research, 6(2), 106–108. https://doi.org/10. 1375/136905203321536236 Segal, N. L., & Hershberger, S. L. (1999). Cooperation and competition between twins: Findings from a Prisoner's dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(1), 29–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S10905138(98)00039-7 DINIĆ | ET AL. Segal, N. L., Hershberger, S. L., & Arad, S. (2003). Meeting one's twin: Perceived social closeness and familiarity. Evolutionary Psychology, 1, 70–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490300100105 Seroczynski, A. D., Bergeman, C. S., & Coccaro, E. F. (1999). Etiology of the impulsivity/aggression relationship: Genes or environment? Psychiatry Research, 86(1), 41–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00013-X Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86(2), 420–428. Sluyter, F., Keijser, J. N., Boomsma, D. I., van Doornen, L. J. P., van den Oord, E. J. C. G., & Snieder, H. (2000). Genetics of testosterone and the aggression‐hostility‐anger (AHA) syndrome: A study of middle‐ aged male twins. Twin Research, 3(4), 266–276. https://doi.org/10. 1375/twin.3.4.266 Smederevac, S., Mitrović, D., Sadiković, S., Milovanović, I., Branovački, B., Dinić, B. M., … Milutinović, A. (2019). Serbian Twin Registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2019.114 Taylor, S. (1967). Aggressive behavior and physiological arousal as a function of provocation and the tendency to inhibit aggression. Journal of Personality, 35, 297–310. Tremblay, P. F., & Belchevski, M. (2004). Did the instigator intend to provoke? A key moderator in the relation between trait aggression and aggressive behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 30(5), 409–424. https:// doi.org/10.1002/ab.20027 Tuvbald, C., & Baker, L. A. (2011). Human aggression across the lifespan: Genetic propensities and environmental moderators. Advances in Genetics, 75, 171–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-380858-5. 00007-1 Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Zheng, M., & Baker, L. A. (2009). Genetic and environmental stability differs in reactive and proactive aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 35(6), 437–452. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20319 Verhulst, B. (2017). A power calculator for the classical twin design. Behavior Genetics, 47(2), 255–261. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519016-9828-9 Vernon, P. A., McCarthy, J. M., Johnson, A. M., Jang, K. L., & Harris, J. A. (1999). Individual differences in multiple dimensions of aggression: A 9 univariate and multivariate genetic analysis. Twin Research, 2(1), 16–21. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905299320566068 Veroude, K., Zhang‐James, Y., Fernàndez‐Castillo, N., Bakker, M. J., Cormand, B., & Faraone, S. V. (2016). Genetics of aggressive behavior: An overview. American Journal of Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 171(1), 3–43. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ajmg.b.32364 Warburton, W. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2019). The competitive reaction time task: The development and scientific utility of a flexible laboratory aggression paradigm. Aggressive Behavior, 45, 389–396. https://doi. org/10.1002/ab.21829 Weidler, C., Habel, U., Hüpen, P., Akkoc, D., Schneider, F., Blendy, J. A., & Wagels, L. (2019). On the complexity of aggressive behavior: Contextual and individual factors in the Taylor Aggression Paradigm. Frontiers Psychiatry, 10, 521. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fpsyt.2019.00521 Yeh, M. T., Coccaro, E. F., & Jacobson, K. C. (2010). Multivariate behavior genetic analyses of aggressive behavior subtypes. Behavior Genetics, 40(5), 603–617. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-010-9363-z Zeichner, A., Parrott, D., & Frey, F. C. (2003). Gender differences in laboratory aggression under response choice conditions. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.10030 SUPPO RTING IN F ORMATION Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section. How to cite this article: Dinić BM, Smederevac S, Sadiković S, et al. Twin study of laboratory‐induced aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 2020;1–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21916