Our memory works by encoding information into something the brain can understand, where it is stored and can later be retrieved. Sensory memory quickly holds onto sensory information. But if we pay a bit more attention, that information can reach our short-term memory. Short-term memory is limited in both how much information it can hold and for how long. That's why you can forget the name of someone you just met. Chunking is a way to remember more information by grouping it together. For example, 0705609510 is harder to remember by just memorizing each digit, but easier like this: 070 560 95 10. Long-term memory is where all our most robust memories stay. As far as we know, the space is infinite and can be held throughout our lives. The serial position effect is the effect where we have better ability to remember information based on where it is located in a series. This effect has two components: the primacy and recency effect. This means that we remember more of the first and last of a series of information, like a shopping list forgotten at home, for example. The better we encode information into long-term memory, the better the possibility of retrieving it. Effortful and automatic processing: When studying for a test, for example, we use effortful processing as we repeat the information and focus to remember it. Automatic processing is encoding that occurs without meaning and requires minimal attention. When studying, we remember more if we focus on the meaning of information. This is called elaborative rehearsal. It requires deeper processing to focus and reflect on the information, making it easier to remember than just reading something and hoping it sticks. When we only read something without focusing and reflecting, it only stays in short-term memory, which is called maintenance rehearsal. Schema helps us organize our world. For example, in FL, I have built a schema for each project. Just by seeing how it is organized, I can remember which song it is. STORAGE The associative network is explained as a massive network of associated ideas and concepts. For example, when we think of a car, we can more easily think of related concepts like wheels, color, glass, steering wheel, etc. This helps us understand how some people can remember pi by associating each digit with something. Declarative memory involves factual knowledge and includes two subcategories. Episodic memory is our knowledge of our personal experiences. Like my favorite movie or what I had for lunch. Semantic memory represents general factual knowledge about the world, language, words, and concepts. Like I know that Mount Everest is the world's tallest mountain. These are called declarative because if we are to demonstrate our knowledge, we must be able to explain it. Tell others what we know. Procedural memory is reflected in competence and action. Here, our knowledge is demonstrated by doing something like exercising at the gym or driving a car. Explicit memory involves conscious memory retrieval, while implicit memory affects our behavior without consciousness. RETRIEVAL Retrieval cues are like breadcrumbs that can activate information in long-term memory. If I tell my dad a match, but he says he doesn't remember it, I can mention some retrieval cues that he probably associated with the match so he remembers it. Like Rödin scored two goals or Bertilsson got a match penalty. The more cues we have, the more we remember. The encoding specificity principle is a theory that suggests that we better remember something if we are in the same environment or context as when the memory was encoded. For example, when I take a test, it's better if I use headphones because I studied with them. That's why my retrieval is better. State-dependent memory is the same, but this time it's our inner state instead of the outer. If I study while cycling at the gym, my ability to retrieve the memory is better if I experience the same breath/internal state. FORGETTING/DISTORTION We can forget things if we didn't initially put enough energy into remembering them. So it stays only in short-term memory until it disappears. Sometimes our old memories get in the way of our new ones. Like if you move, the address for your old home may be what you think of when you think of your new address. This is called proactive interference. On the other hand, there is retroactive interference, which means that our new memories get in the way of our old ones. Childhood amnesia: Our memory usually doesn't contain what happened before we were 3-4 years old. This may have to do with the brain not being fully developed to encode long-term memory. Then we don't have the same understanding at that age, so it's harder to create memories. Misinformation effect means the distortion of memories of misleading information after an event. If I show a video of two cars colliding and then ask, "What speed do you think the cars were going when they made contact?" The answer may be less than if I ask, "What speed do you think the cars were going when they crashed into each other?" Because "crash" is a more brutal word than "contact," it can distort our memory of just that change after.