Running Head: DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN Disability Across the Lifespan Jaime San Miguel Florida International University 1 DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 2 The human lifespan encompasses a constant state of developmental milestones that most humans will encounter at certain ages throughout their lives. Developmental milestones such as learning how to walk, a child’s first words, and becoming potty trained are some prominent examples of early human development that help identify whether a child is meeting developmental milestones appropriate for their age. Although the actual age at which children reach these developmental milestones can vary slightly, it is important that they be reached within certain age parameters. Children typically learn to walk between 9-17 months of age and can walk on their tip toes between 16-30 months of age. Learning how to walk after the age of 16 months has become an established marker of atypical development (Onis, 2006b) and has been found to support early language development (West et al. 2017; Walle and Campos 2014). These studies show that children born with autism spectrum disorder exhibit delayed achievement of certain developmental milestones which are used as an indicator of developmental delays. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) refers to a group of developmental disorders with an onset beginning in the early preschool years. The disorder is characterized by difficulties in social interaction and communication along with restricted and repetitive patterns in behaviors, interests and activities (American Psychiatric Association 2013). Autism is becoming prevalent among American children, as one in 44 children at the age of 8 were reported to have autism as of 2018 and is four times more common in boys than in girls (Maenner et al., 2021). John doe is a 64 year white Hispanic male born and raised in south Florida. John was diagnosed with severe autism spectrum disorder as a teenager and lived with his parents until their death, after which he was transferred to live in a group home due to his inability to care for DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 3 himself independently due to factors attributed to his condition. John is nonverbal due to his severe autism and has limited communication skills, mainly communicating with group home staff with the use of a picture exchange communication system (PECS). John was born to parents of Colombian descent and was raised with both parents as well as two older brothers in the same household. As a child John Does brothers report him learning to walk at around 19 months of age, well ahead of the expected 9-17 month window babies are expected to walk. Jean Piaget’s Sensorimotor stage spans the first two years of a child’s life and describes various methods babies use to understand their world. The sensorimotor stage is divided into 6 substages detailing adaptive behaviors babies obtain at certain ages throughout the sensorimotor stage. Behaviors such as reaching for and manipulating objects are encountered during substage four, between 8 and 12 months of age. Neurotypical infants at this age can identify objects hidden behind another object and move the object to gain access to the desired object. According to Adrien et al (1995) Autistic infants have a comparably tougher time in resolving object permanence tasks and tend to hesitate, looked under the wrong areas for desired objects, and stopped searching altogether, and engaged in other activities unrelated to finding the desired objects (Adrien et al., 1995). Despite the difficulties exhibited by the infants in the study, they were able to successfully locate the desired object, albeit at a lower rate compared to neurotypical peers (Adrien et al., 1995). John Does siblings reported that as an infant, he had difficulty making and maintaining eye contact with them and his parent as well as did not face them when they called his name. John would substitute language with physical prompts to communicate his needs, such as giving his parents a closed bottle for the parents to open. John was also nonverbal as a child and was only able to learn simple sentences as he progressed in age. In relation to Piaget's Sensorimotor stages, this anecdotal evidence shows a lack of development during the preoperational stage of development. DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 4 Piaget’s preoperational stage states that children typically engage in make-believe play which helps to strengthen their newly acquired representational schemes of their surroundings. Make-believe play at this age is characterized by less self-centered play, where children become detached from the toys they play with and centers around objects engaging in activities while the children act as a sort of audience to the occurring events. Neurotypical children in the preoperational stage engage in play that includes more complex combination of schemes that allow children to engage in sociodramatic play where they can act out social situations such as drinking water or eating models of food in a pretend manner. Autistic children such as John differ in that they are more to likely engage in parallel play, a play style common in autistic children in which they play alongside their peers in a solitary fashion, preferring not to engage in group play with other children. Research has shown that children with autism have significant delays in the development of symbolic play (Jarrold et al. 1993; Baron-Cohen 1987). Autistic children in the preoperational stage frequently engage in functional play, playing with objects in line with their intended function, and are more likely to engage in repetitive actions while playing, such as repeatedly stacking cups and tossing them to the floor or throwing dirt or throwing loose objects into the air repeatedly. John Doe’s siblings recall him becoming fixated with objects that were hyper-stimulating to one of the five senses, such as toys that created strong vibrations or had multi-colored, rapidly spinning lights which John would stare at for extended periods of time. These toys could have functioned as stimulatory objects that provided a more intense sensory stimulation that may have reinforced John's preference to play with those certain toys. Autistic children are also known for engaging in restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB), such as hand flapping, repetitive use of speech or objects, and intense interest in a given DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 5 subject or object, for greater frequencies and durations than neurotypical children (Jacques et al. 2018). The third stage of Piaget's stages of cognitive development is the concrete operational stage and includes children between the ages of 7 and 11. In this stage, children's reasoning becomes logical, flexible, and organized. Children at this age begin to exhibit proficiency in observation tasks, the ability to understand how items retain their original form in some way even after being altered visually. An example would be knowing that a certain amount of water retains its mass even if it's moved into differently shaped receptacles. Reversibility is also seen in children at this stage, where an individual can think through steps and backtrack if needed to the first step in the process. Seriation is the ability to order items according to quantitative dimensions, such as weight or length, a skill that is acquired by children at this operational stage. Research by Yirmiya and Shulman (1996) finds that autistic children performed worse in solving seriation tasks compared to neurotypical counterparts (Yirmiya & Shulman, 1996). This difficulty in arranging items according to their physical characteristics could have affected John as a child while performing daily tasks in school and at home such as stacking measuring cups inside one another or being able to stand in line at school by height order. Transitive inference is the ability to seriate mentally, an ability also found in children in the concrete operational stage which allows children to infer certain characteristics of objects given in their relation to other objects. A child at this stage can deduce that if object A is longer than object B, and object B is longer than object C, than object A is also longer than object C. A study conducted by Solomon et al (2015), in which autistic and neurotypical children were placed in a magnetic resonance imager (MRI) machine, demonstrated that neurotypical children recruited more of the prefrontal cortex, parietal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex compared to their autistic peers (Solomon et DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 6 al, 2015). The study reinforces the concept that individuals with autism process information regarding their physical and social environment differently than neurotypical children. Given that John's autism is severe enough to prevent him from becoming a functional, independent adult we can assume that John’s ability to employ transitive inference is even more restricted compared to individuals whose autism is less severe. Spatial reasoning in this stage has improved and children are able to construct cognitive maps of their surroundings more accurately than children in the preoperational stage. Cognitive maps include mental representations of areas familiar to children such as their homes, schools or neighborhoods. It is unclear how John’s autism may have affected his ability to create cognitive maps as a child but as an adult he is reported to successfully navigate his group home and can walk to common areas such as the kitchen and bathroom without assistance. Piaget's final stage, the formal operational stage begins at age 11 and is where children develop the capacity for abstract, scientific thinking. At this stage, the children have entered adolescence and become capable of hypothetico-deductive reasoning. Individuals use abstract thought to brainstorm possible solutions to situations and rationalize them in real-world environments. Propositional thought evaluates the logic of propositions without referring to reallife circumstances. These characteristics require a thorough understanding of language and other symbolic forms of communication, such as mathematical expressions, for this stage to be considered fully mastered. Given that John’s severe autism diagnosis prevented him from successfully mastering the concrete operational stage of development, we can infer that he has also failed to master the formal operational stage and will likely never be able to due to the severity of his developmental delay. DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 7 In order to give a detailed discussion of John Doe’s transition into adulthood, Erik Erikson’s psychosocial theory will be discussed to provide insight on how individuals with severe autism navigate their disability across the remainder of their lives. The psychological conflict present in Erikson's theory in the stage of early adulthood is that of intimacy vs isolation, where individuals strive to build long-term, meaningful relationships with friends and romantic partners. Adults at this stage must preserve a strong sense of own identity which will then foster the attainment of intimacy sought after during this stage. Adults who fail to do so risk becoming isolated from their peer group. Generativity also becomes a focus in this stage, as adults begin to concern themselves with child rearing and contributing to society via their profession or community. In relation to John and autistic individuals in general, this population has a more difficult time establishing friendships, engaged in more unacceptable behavior when pursuing romantic interests, and persisted in pursuing relationships when non mutual interests were apparent (Stokes et al, 2007). This results in many individuals with autism feeling unfulfilled in their pursuits of meaningful social and romantic connections. John’s siblings reported few significant relationships, mainly the siblings and his parents until their passing. These details, unfortunately, reveal that John was isolated from others throughout this stage of development. Erickson's psychological conflict during midlife is known as the generativity versus stagnation period. It is noted for individuals reaching out and contributing to the welfare of the next generation by way of child-rearing, mentoring for young adults and modeling what an individual who has successfully navigated this stage should be. Stagnation refers to adults who have become self-absorbed and self-indulgent despite having attained success in marriage, child rearing and achieving career goals. Erikson describes these individuals as selfish who only look DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 8 out for themselves and prefer to meet their own needs and lack interest in further bettering themselves or society. In this context, John has little hope to achieve any sort of generativity given his severe diagnosis and his extreme lack of communication skills. The final conflict in Erikson’s theory is integrity versus despair. This conflict involves individuals coming to terms with what they achieved or failed to achieve in their life. Individuals who have successfully navigated Erikson’s prior stages and achieved fulfillment in their profession, marriage and community will achieve integrity. Despair is the feeling of having committed many errors in one's life. Individuals might struggle to accept that their lives have been riddled with failure and attempt to find other ways to achieve integrity. These individuals become bitter and tend to express the contempt they have for themselves towards others in the form of anger. As John continues to advance in age he is unlikely to exhibit either of these traits as his limited communication skills will likely prevent him from conveying any sort of meaningful conversation. John’s limited capacity for abstract thought may also deny him the opportunity to reflect on his life and make any sort of impactful observations of his past. Throughout my research, while completing this assignment, I was reminded of the many similarities found in some of the children I have worked with throughout my time as a Registered Behavior Technician. Many of the anecdotes provided by John Does family members have been actual experiences I have faced while working with children with disabilities. This paper has helped me reflect on the many difficulties individuals with disabilities face as they age. Most people have limited experience seeing let alone interacting with people with disabilities and mostly do so when these individuals are school age children between the ages of 5-18. As this population ages, many are either confined to living at home with their parents or sent to live in an adult living facility due to their caregiver's inability or unwillingness to continue to care for DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN the individual. An unfortunate reality is that many individuals with autism will inevitably be impacted throughout their lives based on the severity of their condition. A family friend of mine who was diagnosed with what used to be labeled “Asperger syndrome” lives a relatively normal life, complete with a career, romantic partner, and a social circle of friends who treat him the way he and all other individuals with autism deserve to be treated, as equals. Unfortunately, as the severity of an individual’s autism increases, their capacity to live what most people would call an “ordinary” life dwindles. We as a society should continue to strive for inclusion that accepts and celebrates our differences and foster a loving environment for everyone, regardless of any developmental disability. 9 DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 10 References American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) (5th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Adrien, Jean-Louis & Tanguay, P & Barthélémy, C & Martineau, Joëlle & Perrot, A & Hameury, L & Sauvage, D. (1993). Autistic children and the object permanence task. Acta paedopsychiatrica. 56. 25-9. Baron-Cohen S. Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1987;5:129–148. Jacques, C., Courchesne, V., Meilleur, A. A. S., Mineau, S., Ferguson, S., Cousineau, D., Labbe, A., Dawson, M., & Mottron, L. (2018). What interests young autistic children? An exploratory study of object exploration and repetitive behavior. PloS one, 13(12), e0209251. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209251 Jarrold C, Boucher J, Smith P. Symbolic play in autism: A review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1993;23:281–307. Maenner MJ, Shaw KA, Bakian AV, et al. Prevalence and Characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2018. MMWR Surveill Summ 2021;70(No. SS-11):1–16. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss7011a1 Onis, M. (2006b). WHO motor development study: Windows of achievement for six gross motor development milestones. Acta Paediatrica, 450, 86–95. Solomon, Ragland, J. D., Niendam, T. A., Lesh, T. A., Beck, J. S., Matter, J. C., Frank, M. J., & Carter, C. S. (2015). Atypical Learning in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Transitive Inference. Journal of the American DISABILITY ACROSS THE LIFESPAN 11 Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(11), 947–955. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.08.010 Stokes M, Newton N, Kaur A. Stalking, and social and romantic functioning among adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007 Nov;37(10):196986. PubMed PMID: 17273936. eng. Walle, E. A., & Campos, J. J. (2014). Infant language development is related to the acquisition of walking. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 336–348. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033238. West, K. L., Leezenbaum, N. B., Northrup, J. B., & Iverson, J. M. (2017). The relation between walking and language in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12980. Yirmiya, & Shulman, C. (1996). Seriation, Conservation, and Theory of Mind Abilities in Individuals with Autism, Individuals with Mental Retardation, and Normally Developing Children. Child Development, 67(5), 2045–2059. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14678624.1996.tb01842.x