Psychology Content Category 6A: Sensing the environment This content category has 5 major topics: Sensory Processing, Vision, Hearing, Other Senses, and Perception. Sensory Processing All of our knowledge of the outside world comes to us through one of several sensory systems devoted to detecting a specific type of environmental stimulus and transmitting that detection to the brain. This initial topic first covers concepts, methods and mathematical principles that govern the study of sensation along with a general overview of sensory receptors. • Sensation - The topic of sensation deals with how stimulation from the environment (in terms of light, air vibrations, pressure, odors, etc.) is detected, transduced (or converted from stimulation to neural impulses), and transmitted to the brain. This first section covers topics from psychophysics, which attempts to quantify our psychological experience of physical stimuli. o Thresholds A threshold is the amount of stimulus that needs to be detected before you notice the stimulus. Absolute threshold • As seen in figure 1, an absolute threshold is the stimulus intensity above which you will detect a stimulus at a better than chance (50%) level. Difference threshold • A difference threshold is also known as the just noticeable difference. This is how much a stimulus needs to increase or decrease in intensity so that you will notice a change in the stimulus’ intensity. • Difference thresholds change with Figure 1: Absolute Threshold stimulus intensity; as the stimulus gets more intense, the difference threshold increases) o Example: If you go camping in a quiet alpine forest, you notice (and may get freaked out by) lots of very quiet sounds. But if you’re sitting in the crowd at Mile High stadium during a Broncos game, you probably won’t notice if one additional person starts cheering with the crowd. o Weber’s Law This law attempts to quantify the just noticeable difference. The law is the form of an equation: ΔI/I=k, where ΔI represents the difference threshold, I represents the initial stimulus intensity and k is a constant (known as the Weber fraction). o In English, this means that the just noticeable difference for a stimulus is proportional to the magnitude of the stimulus and is a constant ratio across magnitudes. It turns out that k (the ratio) is logarithmic for human vision and hearing (although Weber’s law isn’t accurate for extremely high and low intensity sounds). Signal detection theory This theory was an advance on psychophysics research that relied only on thresholds. Signal detection theory incorporates both thresholds and individual differences in human judgment. • Different people have different standards for judgment; some are very careful, some are very concerned about never making a mistake, and some are very concerned about never missing an opportunity. • In a stimulus detection task (like being asked to indicate if they heard any sound in a particular time interval), loss averse or careful people are going to be more biased to respond “no” while those that don’t want miss something may be more biased to respond “yes”. • Signal detection theory allows psychologists to quantify how good people are at detecting stimuli and quantify and response biases they have. • Signal detection theory states that detecting a stimulus requires making a judgment about whether it is there or not, and this judgment is based on subjective interpretations of ambiguous stimuli. • Signal detection theory methodology o In signal detection studies, participants are asked to indicate whether or not a stimulus (sound, object, or something else) was present o As can be seen in Figure 2, there are four possible outcomes for any trial. Correct answers are hits (saying “yes” when the stimulus is present) and correct rejections (saying “no” when the stimulus is absent). Incorrect answers can be separated into False Alarms (saying “yes” when the stimulus is absent) and Misses (saying “no” when the stimulus is present). o Response bias is the extent to which an individual is likely to answer “yes.” Figure 2: Possible Trial Outcomes in Signal Detection Theory o Sensory adaptation Our sensory receptors and brain circuits have evolved to detect changes in our environment. This means that when a stimulus’ level is not changing, then our sensory processes become less sensitive to that stimulus Psychologically, this is experienced as “getting used” to a particular stimulus or ceasing to pay attention to that stimulus. • Sensory receptors o Other sections in this category will go into specific detail for the receptors for the major sensory systems. As a general overview, however, receptors are cells that are specialized to detect a type of physical, chemical, or wave stimulus; translate that stimulus to electrical impulses; and transmit those impulses to nerves. o Sensory pathways Other sections will go into more detail into the specific sensory pathways, but in general sensory information reaches the brain by first being detected by a receptor cell. The receptor cell then excites nerve cells which then go directly into the specific brain area that begins to interpret the stimulus or connect or (if the receptor is relatively far away from the brain) connects to other nerve cells which then connect to the brain. o Types of sensory receptors Figure 3 shows the various types of receptors specialized for detecting most of the various forms of sensation that humans process. In addition to the receptors listed, we also have receptors (in the skin) for pain and temperature. Figure 3: Types of Sensory Receptors Vision Figure 4: Structure of the Eye • Structure and function of the eye (figure 4) • Cornea – the cornea is the transparent outer covering over the lens. It serves to focus incoming light. • Pupil – the pupil is the dark circle at the center of the eye and is actually an empty hole in the iris above the lens. Light goes through the pupil to the lens. • Iris – the iris is the colored part of the eye. It serves to regulate the amount of light that goes through the pupil to the lens. By dilating and contracting, it can allow more or less light through allow us some adjustability of our visual acuity in different amounts of light. The iris also is sensitive to emotional content – it dilates when we see something we like. • Lens – The lens serves to focus light and transmit it to the light sensitive cells on the retina. • Sclera – The sclera is the white part of the eye. It serves as a protective outer covering. Figure 5: Detailed Structure of the Retina • Retina (Figure 5 & Figure 6) o The retina is the part of the eye that transduces photons into nerve impulses. It is located at the back of the eye. Light focused from the lens falls onto the retina. o There are two types of photosensitive cells: rods and cones. These are located at the very back of the retina (figure 6), which means that photons must pass by the optic nerve and other retinal cells before being sensed by the rods and cones. o Rods- Rods are sensitive to very low levels of illumination, and are therefore responsible for night vision. They also do not do a very good job of discriminating fine details or color. They are concentrated at the edges of the retina. The eye has approximately 120 million rods. o Cones are most densely concentrated at the fovea. Unlike rods, they do a good job discriminating color and fine details. There are three types of cones, each most sensitive to a different wavelength of light (figure 7); combinations of activation of these cones help produce our experience of color. There are approximately 6 million cones in the eye. o Bipolar cells (along with amacrine and horizontal cells, which are not shown in figure 5), take the input from rods and cones and perform some initial computations. This is then passed along to the ganglion cells. o Ganglion cells take input and, once a threshold is reached, produce action potentials that cause stimulation along their axons. These axons are bundled together and exit the eye through the back of the retina. The bundle of axons is the optic nerve. Figure 6: Comparison of Rods and Cones Blind Spot – there are no rods or cones where the optic nerve exits the eye. For this reason you have a blind spot in each of your visual fields. You do not experience a sensation of blindness, however, because the brain guesses what is most likely to be in the part of your visual field and fills in your visual experience accordingly. • The fovea is the center of the retina. It is the area where cones are the densest and is therefore the area where your visual acuity is greatest. Color vision is determined by the wavelengths of the light that is detected by the cones. o Wavelengths (figure 7) - There are three types of cones, each maximally sensitive to a different wavelength of light. Color vision results from the combination of activity of the different cones (this is known as trichromatic theory). o “S” cones are most sensitive to short wavelengths of lights (figure 7). This wavelength of light is experienced as blue to violet. o “M” cones are most sensitive to medium wavelengths (which are experienced as yellow to green) o “L” cones are most sensitive to longer wavelengths of light (which are experienced as red to orange).o o Visible light is only a small part of the entire electromagnetic spectrum. While gamma rays can have wavelengths as short as 10-5 nm, and radio waves have wavelengths as long as 1013 nm, visible light in only in the range of 400 nm to 700 nm. Figure 7: Trichromatic Vision • Opponent-process is another theory that describes the way that color is perceived. Some colors appear to be opposites. For example it is very difficult to imagine a “reddish green, while it is comparatively easy to imagine a “yellowish green.” Staring too long at a red object also leaves a green afterimage in your vision. o Colors that can’t mix are considered to be opponents. o This has nothing to do with the cones; instead, it involves how input from the cones is processed by the ganglion cells. o One type of ganglion is excited by S cones and inhibited by L cones and M cones. This creates the perception that blue and yellow (which is a combination of red and green) are opponents.” o Another type of ganglion is excited by L cones and inhibited by M cones, which creates the perception that red and green are opponents. Figure 8: How visual information gets from the eyes to the brain Figure 9: Dorsal and Ventral processing streams • • We describe colors using the variables of hue, saturation, and brightness. o Hue is the position of the color in the color spectrum, and is determined by the wavelength of the light that is reflected by an object. Shorter wavelengths look violet, purple, and blue while longer wavelengths look yellow, orange, or red. o Saturation is the vividness or purity of the hue. It is determined by the mixture of wavelengths (the fewer the number of wavelengths reflected by an object, the greater the saturation). o Brightness is the intensity or luminance of a color. The more light that is reflected by an object, the brighter the object’s color will appear. Visual processing The eyes are at the front (anterior) of the brain while the primary visual cortex is at the back (posterior) of the brain. This means that visual information must travel the length of the brain before it undergoes significant processing. Each retina has a separate optic nerve. These two nerves meet close to the midpoint of the brain at the optic chiasm (figure 8). At this point, most (but not all) of the fibers in each optic nerve cross over to the other hemisphere. • This means that most of the input from the right eye and the right visual field is processed in the left hemisphere (and vice versa). • After the chiasm, the visual input travels to the thalamus and then to the right and left visual cortex in the occipital lobes of each hemisphere. Parallel processing means that several different types of qualities are processed simultaneously for each object. These qualities include color, motion, shape, and depth. • The two best studies forms of parallel processing involve shape and motion, which provide information about what an object is and where it is (and where it is going). These are each processed by a separate pathway in the brain (figure 9) • The dorsal “where stream” projects from the primary visual cortex into the parietal lobe. • The ventral “what stream” projects from the primary visual cortex into the temporal lobe. Feature detection refers to the phenomenon that certain groups of neurons in the primary visual cortex seem to preferentially respond to the presence of certain features. • For example, some neurons respond strongly to the presence of horizontal lines, while others respond to lines that are at 45 degree angles. Others respond to vertical lines and still further neurons respond to every other angle in between. • Therefore, different neurons show a selective response to certain features of the visual input. • Other types of feature detectors include edge detectors and length detectors. • This is important because it suggests that vision is a constructive process. o Entire scenes are not transmitted as a whole. o On the contrary, the visual system somewhat seems to break up visual input into its constituent parts and then reconstruct it using feature detectors and parallel processing. Hearing Our sense of hearing is used to detect sound waves in the air and translate the information about the waves’ frequencies and amplitudes into information about pitch, volume, and duration. • Auditory processing (figures 10 and 11) Outer Ear • The auditory canal is shaped to channel sound waves to the eardrum • The eardrum vibrates when struck by sound waves, and these vibrations cause the ossicles to move. Figure 10: The outer, middle, and inner ear Middle Ear • The ossicles are three bones named for the tools they somewhat resemble (the hammer, anvil, & stirrup). The ossicles are connected at one end to the eardrum and at the other end to the inner ear (the cochlea) at the oval window. The ossicles transmit vibrations from the eardrum to the cochlea Inner Ear • The cochlea is a snail-shaped, fluid-filled structure. Vibrations from the ossicles cause pressure changes in the cochlea which causes the cochlear fluid to move. • The basilar membrane is a thin membrane running through the center of the cochlea. When the cochlear fluid moves, this causes the basilar membrane to move. On the basilar membrane are hair cells which are moved when the basilar membrane moves. This movement stimulates the hair cells, which in turn stimulate the auditory nerve. The hair cells are the main receptor cells of the • auditory Figure 11: Signal Transduction in the inner ear. system. The auditory nerve is connected to the hair cells and connects the auditory system to the brain. • Auditory pathways in the brain • Nerve impulses from the auditory nerve make their way to the thalamus, a structure in the midbrain located just above the brainstem. The thalamus is involved in consciousness, sleep, and the relaying of sensory and motor signals throughout the cortex. The thalamus has strong connections to the primary auditory cortex. • Located in the temporal lobe in both hemispheres of the brain, the primary auditory cortex. Neurons in different parts of the auditory cortex respond selectively to different frequencies (and therefore lead to the conscious experience of different pitches). Further processing allows humans to group, recognize, and interpret the sounds into something meaningful. • Damage to the primary auditory cortex results in the loss of conscious awareness of sound, although individuals with such damage can react reflexively and unconsciously to sound due to the processing in the thalamus. Sensory reception by hair cells As discussed above, the hair cells are located inside the cochlea on the basilar membrane. The hair cells can move, and when the ossicles cause vibrations in the cochlea (transmitted via the oval window), the fluid inside the cochlea moves, which causes the hair cells to move, which releases neurotransmitters that cause the auditory nerve to fire. • Hair cells are grouped in bundles, and are connected to each at their tips by proteins called tip links. When the hair cells are moved in the inner ears, the tip links are stretched, which causes the tip link to mechanically lift open a potassium channel in the hair cell membrane (figure 12). This depolarizes the hair cell, which releases neurotransmitters across the synapse to the auditory nerve. Figure 12: Hair cell structure and function Other Senses Sight and hearing are the best known and best studied senses, but humans have several other systems to acquire sensory information from the environment, including somatosensation, taste, smell, the kinesthetic sense, and the vestibular sense. • Somatosensation o Pain perception Pain serves as a warning system that tells you to stop or change what you are doing. Pain receptors can be found in many places in the body including skin, membranes around muscles and joints, muscles, and organs. There are two main types of pain receptors (as can be seen in figure 13) • Fast fibers are myelinated and communicate sharp, Figure 13: Pain Receptors immediate pain. They are activated by strong physical pressure and extreme temperature changes. • Slow fibers are unmyelinated and communicate chronic, dull pain. They are activated by the chemical changes that occur in skin when it is damaged. Figure 14: Taste from tongue to brain • Taste is a chemical sense. Your taste bud receptors (inside your papillae; figure 14) bind to different molecules that then are translated into different tastes. o Taste buds/chemoreceptors that detect specific chemicals : There are five basic types of taste buds devoted to detecting different combinations of basic qualities. All of your taste experiences start with different amounts of activation of these receptors. • Sweet, salty, sour, bitter, and umami • Umami is Japanese for “savory” and results from the detection of glutamate. o • The different taste bud receptors (contrary to popular belief) are not concentrated in particular regions of the tongue, but instead are spread relatively uniformly across the tongue. Supertasters are individuals that have a particularly high number of taste buds (often 6 times that of non-supertasters). They are more likely to be highly aware of flavors and textures, experience pain when eating spicy foods, and report disliking bitter foods and drinks (like coffee or alcohol). Smell o Smell (like taste) is a chemical sense. Olfactory receptors bind to molecules and send the signal to the olfactory bulb and then on to the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. The olfactory epithelium (figure 15) is a lyaer of tissue deep in the nasal cavity which contains the olfactory receptors. When olfactory receptors detect an odor molecule they send their input to the olfactory bulb, the area for odor processi ng in the brain. Figure 15: Olfactory epithelium and olfactory bulb Because the olfactory bulb is so close to the olfactory receptors, smells have the most direct route to the brain. The prefrontal cortex areas help process the pleasantness/unpleasantness of the odors, while the amygdala is involved in processing the emotional and memory content of the odors (and explains why smells can evoke strong feelings and memories). o Olfactory cells/chemoreceptors that detect specific chemicals Each olfactory receptor is sensitive to only one type of molecule. However, unlike taste (where there are only five types of receptors), there are thousands of different types of receptors for odor molecules. Although we have so many receptors for sensing olfactory information, humans are surprisingly bad at perceiving and identifying specific smells. • Humans can quickly and easily identify whether a smell is pleasant or unpleasant, but can have an accuracy rate (for identifying even common food items) of less than 50%. Humans (like other animals) also have olfactory receptors to detect pheromones (chemicals that animals release to trigger behavioral and physiological changes in other animals), but it is unclear to what extent (if any) that pheromones affect human behavior. • Kinesthetic sense o Kinesthetic sense is your perception of your body in space. o This sense is crucial for coordination and voluntary movement. o If you keep your eyes closed and are able to touch your finger to your nose without missing, you have successfully used your kinesthetic sense. o Information for the kinesthetic sense comes from receptors in your joints, muscles, and tendons. • Vestibular sense o Your vestibular sense is your sense of balance. o It uses receptors in the semicircular canals of the ear (figure 16) that can detect the level of liquid in the canals which the brain can use to interpret the head’s rotation. o This explains why ear infections can affect the sense of balance. o Seasickness/motion sickness involves the vestibular sense, but not exclusively. It arises when the visual system and vestibular system provide conflicting and irreconcilable signals. Figure 16: The semicircular canals in the inner ear are the site of receptors that provide you information about your balance. Perception Our understanding of the outside world is not a direct record of the input from our sensory processing organs. Instead, our perceptions are a selective record of the environment around us, influenced by our goals, motivations, attention, and the basic processes of the brain. • • Bottom-up/Top-down processing • Both of these types of processing affect our pattern recognition, or our ability to recognize, contextualize, and understand our perceptual input. • Bottom-up processing refers to pattern recognition that is driven by features of the stimulus as well as how the brain processes those features. Information is then transmitted up to higher levels of mental processing. This is sometimes called data-driven processing. Examples of bottom-up processing include the perceptual organization principles discussed below. • Top-down processing refers to pattern recognition that is driven by higher-level processes such as attention, context, or expectations. These higher-level processes then transmit information down to lower levels of mental processing to guide perception. This is sometimes called schema-driven processing. Example: In the adjacent figure, the same shape is interpreted as both an “h” and an “a.” The surrounding context of the other letters helps guide the perception of the ambiguous letters. Figure 17: Example of topPerceptual organization down processing effects. • There are a number of cues both in the body and environment that the brain uses to judge depth/distance, motion, and our perception of the unity and constancy of environmental objects. • Depth/Distance Depth perception cues can be either monocular cues (which are available to either eye alone) or binocular cues (which depend on comparing input from both eyes). Binocular Cues • Binocular disparity o Human’s eyes are set apart, and therefore have a slightly different view of the world. By comparing how differentially far apart two objects appear to each eye, the brain can calculate the distance to each object and their relative depth. • Convergence o When we look at objects that are close, our eye muscles turn inward. The brain can use the flex in each eye muscle to compute how close an object is. Monocular Cues • Occlusion – in general, objects that are closer block and occlude objects that are further away. • Relative Size – If you know that two objects are relatively the same size, then the object that projects a smaller image on the retina is usually farther off. • Familiar Size – We can use the known size of familiar objects to judge how far away they are. • Linear perspective is the phenomenon where parallel lines seem to distantly converge. • Texture gradient – If an object has a uniform texture or pattern, the closer parts of that pattern will appear to be larger than the farther parts of that pattern. • • • Size Cues for size are intimately related to cues for distance. Most of these cues rely on interpreting the meaning of how large an object appears on the retina. By knowing an object’s true size, you can interpret its distance, and vice versa. When these cues are ambiguous or hard to interpret, visual illusions (such as the Ponzo Illusion, figure 18) can result. Motion Motion aftereffects • Also known as the waterfall effect, these aftereffects occur when you look at a moving object for a long time and then look at a still object which then appears to be moving in the opposite direction. o This is excellent evidence of motion-sensitive neurons in the brain. By staring at the moving object, the neurons that Figure 18: The Ponzo illusion are sensitive to motion in that direction are adapting to the motion (getting fatigued and used to it). When you look away from the motion, the neurons that respond to motion in the other direction are less fatigued, and therefore more able to fire, which convinces your brain that the still object is actually moving in the other direction. Stroboscopic motion • This explains how movies (on film) appear to move despite being composed of individual still frames. When two or more images are presented in rapid succession (about 60 milliseconds apart), they appear to show continuous motion. • Constancy Usually, changing an object’s angle, distance, or illumination does not cause us to change how we perceive the object’s size, shape, color, or lightness. This is because the brain computes size, shape, color, and lightness in a relative fashion (for example, lightness is computed by comparing how much light is reflected from an object with how much light is reflected by the background). If this was computed in an absolute fashion, we would think that (for example) an object’s actual color and lightness had changed if it was moved from the sun to the shade. Gestalt principles • Gestalt psychologists were an early group of researchers who emphasized that in perception, the “whole is more than the sum of its parts.” They proposed that the brain uses several principles to organize sensory information. • Good continuation The mind tends to think that shapes of similar size, color, and orientation are part of the same object when they are obscured behind another object. Therefore, in figure 19, we see a continuous long green rectangle rather than two shorter green rectangles. Figure 19: Gestalt principle of good continuation • • • • • Figure/Ground The mind easily distinguishes between things that are in the foreground and things that are in the background as well as between the focal and important parts of a scene versus the unimportant parts. When this is ambiguous, you get illusions like the reversible figure illusion (figure 20) that looks like both two faces looking at each other (the purple areas) and a candlestick (the white area). Proximity We tend to see things that are close to each other as being part of the same entity. Therefore, in part (b) in figure 22, we see four lines of four green dots, rather than one group of sixteen dots or sixteen individual dots. Similarity We tend to group things that are similar to each other together in the same group. Therefore, in part (c) in figure 22, we are more likely to interpret the figure on the left as vertical columns of alternating red circles and squares and the figure on the right ad horizontal rows of alternating red circles and Figure 21: The Gestalt principle of figure ground illustrated by the squares. Figure 20: The Gestalt reversible figure illusion principle of illusory contours. Closure We tend to “fill in the blanks” and complete figures that have gaps, as you can see in part (a) in figure 22, where we easily see a beige triangle, purple square, and blue horse despite the many gaps in the figures. Illusory contours The mind tends to interpret the existence of the parts of a figure that are not actually there but only implied (to the point that as you see in figure 21, unless you closely attend to the white space, you may “fill in” the sides of the blue square in the white spaces even though they aren’t actually drawn). Figure 22: Examples of the Gestalt principles of (a) Closure (b) Proximity and (c) Similarity Unless noted on the image itself, all images copyright and courtesy of McGraw-Hill Education. All images are used solely for non-profit educational purposes.